An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Revised edition
An Introduction
to the Grammar of English
Revised edition
Elly van Gelderen
Arizona State University
John Benjamins Publishing Company
Amsterdam / Philadelphia
Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data
Gelderen, Elly van.
An introduction to the grammar of English / Elly van Gelderen. -- Rev. ed.
p. cm.
Rev. ed: .
Includes bibliographical references and index.
. English language--Grammar. . English language--Grammar, Historical. . English
language--Social aspects. . English language--Syntax. I. Title.
PE.G 
.--dc 
      (; alk. paper) /      (; alk. paper)
      ()
©  – John Benjamins B.V.
No part of this book may be reproduced in any form, by print, photoprint, microfilm, or any
other means, without written permission from the publisher.
John Benjamins Publishing Co. · P.O. Box  ·   Amsterdam · e Netherlands
John Benjamins North America · P.O. Box  · Philadelphia  - · 
e paper used in this publication meets the minimum requirements of
American National Standard for Information Sciences – Permanence of
Paper for Printed Library Materials,  z39.48-1984.
8
TM
Table of contents
Foreword xi
Preface to the second edition xv
Abbreviations xvii
List of gures xix
List of tables xxi
chapter 1
Introduction 1
1.
Examples of linguistic knowledge 1
1.1
Sounds and words 1
1.2
Syntactic structure 2
2.
How do we know so much? 5
3.
Examples of social or non-linguistic knowledge 6
4.
Conclusion 8
Exercises 9
Class discussion 9
Keys to the exercises 10
Special topic: Split innitive 10
chapter 2
Categories 12
1.
Lexical categories 12
1.1
Nouns (N) and Verbs (V) 13
1.2
Adjectives (Adj) and Adverbs (Adv) 15
1.3
Prepositions (P) 18
2.
Grammatical categories 19
2.1
Determiner (D) 19
2.2
Auxiliary (Aux) 21
2.3
Coordinator (C) and Complementizer (C) 21
3.
Pronouns 23
4.
What new words and loanwords tell us! 24
5.
Conclusion 25
Exercises 27
Class discussion 29
Keys to the exercises 30
Special topic: Adverb and Adjective 32
vi An Introduction to the Grammar of English
chapter 3
Phrases 35
1.
e noun phrase (NP) 36
2.
e adjective phrase, adverb phrase, verb phrase
and prepositional phrase 39
2.1
e adjective phrase (AdjP) and adverb phrase (AdvP) 39
2.2
e verb phrase (VP) 40
2.3
e prepositional phrase (PP) 41
3.
Phrases in the sentence 42
4.
Coordination of phrases and apposition 43
5.
Finding phrases and building trees 45
5.1
Finding the phrase 45
5.2
Building trees 46
6.
Conclusion 49
Exercises 50
Class discussion 51
Keys to the exercises 52
Special topic: Negative concord 56
Review of Chapters 1–3 59
Exercises relevant to these Chapters: 60
Class discussion 60
Keys to the exercises 61
Example of an exam/quiz covering Chapters 1 to 3 63
Keys to the exam/quiz 63
chapter 4
Functions in the sentence 65
1.
Subject and predicate 65
2.
Complements 68
2.1
Direct and indirect object 68
2.2
Subject and object predicate 70
3.
Verbs and functions 72
4.
Trees for all verb types 74
5.
Light verbs (optional) 76
6.
Conclusion 77
Exercises 78
Class discussion 80
Keys to the exercises 80
Special topic: Case and agreement 83
Table of contents vii
chapter 5
More functions, of prepositions and particles 86
1.
Adverbials 86
2.
Prepositional verbs 90
3.
Phrasal verbs 90
4.
Phrasal prepositional verbs (optional) 93
5.
Objects and adverbials 93
6.
Conclusion 96
Exercises 97
Class discussion 99
Keys to the exercises 100
Special topic: e passive and ‘dummies 102
chapter 6
e structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 105
1.
Auxiliary verbs 105
2.
e ve types of auxiliaries in English 107
2.1
Modals 107
2.2 Perfect have
(pf) 109
2.3 Progressive be
(progr) 110
2.4 Passive be
(pass) 111
2.5 e ‘dummy’ do
112
3.
Auxiliaries,‘ax hop, and the verbgroup (VGP) 113
4.
Finiteness 114
5.
Relating the terms for verbs (optional) 116
6.
Conclusion 118
Exercises 120
Class discussion 121
Keys to the exercises 122
Special topic: Reduction of have
and the shape of participles 122
Review of chapters 4–6 124
Examples of midterm exams covering Chapters 4 to 6 127
Example 1 127
Example 2 127
Example 3 128
Key to example 1 129
Key to example 2 130
Key to example 3 131
viii An Introduction to the Grammar of English
chapter 7
Finite clauses: Embedded and coordinated 132
1.
Sentences and clauses 133
2.
e functions of clauses 134
3.
e structure of the embedded clause: e Complementizer Phrase (CP) 135
4.
Coordinate sentences: e Coordinator Phrase (CP)? 138
5.
Terminological labyrinth and conclusion 139
Exercises 141
Class discussion 142
Keys to the exercises 143
Special topic: Preposition or complementizer: e ‘prepositionlike
146
chapter 8
Non-finite clauses 149
1.
Non-nite clauses 149
2.
e functions of non-nites 151
3.
e structure: CP 152
4.
Coordinating non-nites 154
5.
Conclusion 155
Exercises 156
Class discussion 157
Keys to the exercises 159
Special topic: Dangling participles and gerunds 161
Review of Chapters 7 and 8 164
Exercises 165
Keys to the exercises 165
Sample quiz/exam, covering Chapters 7 and 8 166
Keys to the quiz/exam 167
chapter 9
e structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 169
1.
e structure of the PP, AdjP, and AdvP and the functions inside 170
2.
e structure of the NP and functions inside 172
3.
Arguments for distinguishing complements from modiers (optional) 176
3.1
Complement and modier follow the head N 176
3.2
Complement and modier precede the head N 177
4.
Conclusion 179
Exercises 181
Table of contents ix
Class discussion 182
Keys to the exercises 183
Special topic: Pronoun resolution 188
chapter 10
Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 189
1.
Relative clauses (RC) 189
2.
Inside the NP: Relative and complement clauses 190
2.1
Relatives 190
2.2
Complement clauses 191
2.3
Reduced relative clauses 192
3.
NPs as compared to AdjPs, AdvPs, and PPs 193
4.
More on RCs 194
5.
e structure of modiers and complements (optional) 195
6.
Conclusion 198
Exercises 199
Class discussion 200
Keys to the exercises 200
Special topic: Relative choice and preposition stranding 203
chapter 11
Special sentences 205
1.
Questions/Interrogatives: e CP 205
2.
Exclamations 207
3.
Topicalization, passive, cle, and pseudo-cle 208
4.
Conclusion 209
Exercises 210
Keys to the exercises 210
Special topic: Comma punctuation 211
Review of Chapters 9–11 214
Home work 1, on Chapter 1 and Special topics 215
Home work 2, covering Chapters 2 –11 215
Home work 3, or take-home exam, covering Chapters 7–11 216
Examples of Final Exams 217
Example 1 217
Example 2 219
Example 3 220
Glossary 222
References 229
Index 230
x An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Foreword
To the student:
You don’t have to read long books or novels in this course – no Das Kapital, Phenom-
enology of Spirit, Middlemarch, or War and Peace. ere isn’t too much memorization
either. It should be enough if you become familiar with the keywords at the end of
each chapter. Use the glossary, if it is helpful, but dont overemphasize the importance
of terminology.
e focus is on arguments, exercises, and tree drawing. You need to practice from
the rst week on, however, and you may also have to read a chapter more than once.
Pay attention to the tables and gures; they oen summarize parts of the text. e
course is not particularly dicult but, once you get lost, go for help!
e book is divided in four parts (Chapters 1 to 3, Chapters 4 to 6, Chapters 7 and 8,
and Chapters 9 to 11), with review sections aer each. Chapter 1 is the introduction;
skip the ‘about the original edition’ and ‘preface to the second edition, if you want.
About the original edition
e philosophy behind the book hasn’t changed in the second edition so I have
adapted the preface to the rst edition here and have then added things special to the
second edition.
is grammar is in the tradition of the Quirk family of grammars, such as the
work of Huddleston, Burton-Roberts, Aarts & Wekker, Quirk, Greenbaum, Leech,
and Svartvik whose work in turn is based on a long tradition of grammarians such
as Jespersen, Kruisinga, Poutsma, and Zandvoort.
1
However, it also uses the insights
from generative grammar.
While following the traditional distinction between function (subject, object, etc.)
and realization (NP, VP, etc), the book focuses on structure and makes the function
derivative, as in more generative work. e books focus on structure can be seen in the
treatment of the VP as consisting of the verb and its complements. Abstract discussions,
such as what a constituent is, are largely avoided (in fact, the term constituent is since it
1. ese are all well-known references, so I have refrained from listing them in the references.
xii An Introduction to the Grammar of English
is a stumbling block in my experience), and the structure of the NP and AP is brought
in line with that of the VP: NPs and APs have complements as well as modiers.
A clear distinction is made between lexical and functional (here called grammati-
cal) categories. Lexical categories project to phrases and these phrases have functions
at sentence level (subject, predicate, and object). In this book, the functional categories
determiner, auxiliary, and (phrasal) coordinator do not project to phrases and have
no function at sentence level. ey function exclusively inside a phrase or connect
phrases. Hence, determiner, auxiliary, and coordinator express realization as well as
function. Complementizers and those coordinators that introduce clauses do head the
CP in this second edition. e reason that I have changed the S’ from the rst edition
into a CP is two-fold. (a) e S’ is confusing since it is not an intermediate projection
and (b) the CP is more in line with current syntactical frameworks. e CP can func-
tion as subject, object, and adverbial. In a generative syntax book, I would of course
have all functional categories project to phrases such as DP, QP, and TP, but for an
introductory grammar book, I think having the lexical categories (and the C) project
is a better choice. e distinction between lexical and functional category is of course
not always clearcut, e.g. adverbs, pronouns, and some prepositions are in between. I
do bring this up.
On occasion, I do not give a denitive solution to a problem because there isnt
one. is lack of explanation can be caused either because an analysis remains contro-
versial, as in the case of ditransitive verbs and coordinates, or because of the continual
changes taking place in English (or any other language for that matter). Instead of giv-
ing one solution, I discuss some options. I have found that students become frustrated
if, for instance, they can reasonably argue that a verb is prepositional in contexts where
the book saysit is an intransitive verb. e emphasis in this book is on the argumenta-
tion, and not on presenting ‘the’ solution. e chapter where I have been quite conser-
vative in my analysis is Chapter 6. e reason is that to provide the argumentation for
a non-at structure involves theta-theory, quantier-oat, and the introduction of the
TP and other functional categories. is leads too far.
e book starts with a chapter on intuitive linguistic knowledge and provides
an explanation for it based on Universal Grammar. At the end of each chapter, there
is a discussion of prescriptive rules. In my experience, students want to know what
the prescriptive rule is. Strangely enough, they dont want the instructor to tell them
that, linguistically speaking, there is nothing wrong with splitting an innitive or
using like as a complementizer. ey want to (and should) know the rule. I have not
integrated the topics in the chapters since I want to keep descriptive and prescriptive
rules separate although that is sometimes hard. e topics are added to give a avor
for the kinds of prescriptive rules around and, obviously, cannot cover all traditional
usage questions.
e chapters in this book cover standardmaterial: categories, phrases, functions,
and embedded sentences. ere are a few sections that I have labeled optional, since,
depending on the course, they may be too much or too complex. e last chapter
could either be skipped or expanded upon. It should be possible to cover all chapters
in one semester. e students I have in mind (because of my own experience) are
English, Humanities, Philosophy, and Education majors as well as others taking an
upper level grammar course in an English department at a university. I am assuming
students using this book know basic ‘grammar, for instance, the past tense of go, and
the comparative of good. Students who do not have that knowledge should be encour-
aged to consult a work such as O’Dwyer (2000).
Even though I know there is a danger in giving one answer where more than one
is sometimes possible, I have provided answers to the exercises. It is done to avoid
having to go over all exercises in class. I hope this makes it possible to concentrate on
those exercises that are interesting or challenging.
I would like to thank my students in earlier grammar courses whose frustration
with some of the inconsistencies in other books has inspired the current work. I am
sure this is not the rst work so begun. Many thanks also to Johanna Wood for much
helpful discussion that made me rethink fundamental questions and for suggesting
the special topics, to Harry Bracken for great comments and encouragement, to
Viktorija Todorovska for major editorial comments to the rst edition, to Tom Stroik
for supportive suggestions, to Barbara Fennell for detailed comments and insightful
clarications, and to Anke de Looper of John Benjamins for her insights on the rst
edition. For help and suggestions with the (originally planned) e-text as well as the
paper version, I am very grateful to Lut Hussein, Je Parker, Laura Parsons, and to
Susan Miller.
Foreword xiii
Preface to the second edition
It was time for an updated version of A Grammar of English. Some of the example
sentences read as if they were 10 years old and they are. us, Bill Clinton hasnt been
the US president for a long time and Benazir Bhutto and Yasser Arafat are no longer
alive. It is also so much more accepted to use corpus sentences, and these examples
may speak more to the users. To keep the text clean of references, I give very basic
references, e.g. CBS 60 Minutes, and not always the exact date. It is now so easy to
nd those references that I think they arent needed. Many contemporary example
sentences come from Mark DaviesCorpus of Contemporary American English and the
British National Corpus; the older ones from the Oxford English Dictionary or from
well-known plays.
I have updated the cartoons, added texts to be analyzed, rearranged and added to
the Special Topics, and provided more gures and tables. ere is also a website that
lists relevant links, repeats practice texts from this book for analysis, and contains
some resources: http://www.public.asu.edu/~gelderen/grammar.htm. I have deleted
the ‘Further Reading’ section since it was useless: too much detail on the one hand
and then very general references to introductory textbooks on the other hand. I think
the students who would use this section are smart enough to gure out other refer-
ences for themselves.
Due to a computer error that changed N and V etc into N and V (aer the second
page proofs had been corrected), the rst edition of this book had to be physically
destroyed and what ended up the rst edition in 2002 was actually a reprint. ere
were a few typos that survived this process. I hope that these are corrected and that
not too many new ones have been created.
I am very happy that the rst edition has been useful in a number of dier-
ent settings and places, e.g. in Puerto Rico, Norway, Turkey, Spain, Macedonia, e
Netherlands, the US, and Canada. I have used it myself with a lot of satisfaction, and
would like to thank many of my students in ENG 314 at Arizona State University. e
areas that I personally did not like in the rst edition are the at auxiliary verb struc-
tures in Chapter 6 and the S (and S) in Chapter 7. As mentioned, I have only changed
the S to CP, but haven’t introduced a DP, TP, or an expanded TP because this isnt
appropriate for the audience. I have eliminated traces and use what looks like a copy’
or sometimes the strike-through font. In Chapter 6, I have also introduced timelines
for tense and aspect since students oen ask about the names of tenses.
I would like to thank some of the same people as I did for the rst edition, in
particular Johanna Wood, Harry Bracken, and Laura Parsons. For comments in book
xvi An Introduction to the Grammar of English
reviews and beyond, I would like to thank Anja Wanner, Carsten Breul, Christoph
Schubert, and Nina Rojina. I am especially grateful to Mariana Bachtchevanova,
Eleni Buzarovska, Lynn Sims, James Berry, Amy Shinabarger, James Dennis, Wim
van der Wur, and Richard Young for detailed comments aer teaching with the
book, and also to Terje Lohndal. anks to Alyssa Bachman for providing a student
perspective and helping me add to sections that were less clear. Continued thanks to
Kees Vaes and Martine van Marsbergen.
Elly van Gelderen
Apache Junction, Arizona
November 2009
Abbreviations
Adj Adjective N N-bar, intermediate category
AdjP Adjective Phrase neg negative
Adv Adverb NP Noun Phrase
Adv-ial Adverbial ObjPr Object Predicate
AdvP Adverb Phrase OED Oxford English Dictionary
AUX Auxiliary P Preposition
BNC British National Corpus pass passive auxiliary
BrE British English pf perfect auxiliary
C Complementizer or PO Prepositional Object
Coordinator PP Prepositional Phrase
CP Complementizer Phrase Pre-D Pre-determiner
(or Coordinator Phrase) Pred Predicate
COCA Corpus of Contemporary prog progressive auxiliary
American Pron pronoun
D Determiner RC Relative Clause
(D)Adv Degree Adverb S Sentence (or Speech on time line)
DO Direct Object SC Small Clause
E Event time SU Subject
e.g. for example SuPr Subject Predicate
i.e. ‘namely V Verb
inf innitive marker to V V-bar, intermediate category
IO Indirect Object VGP Verb group
N Noun VP Verb Phrase
? Questionable sentence.
* Ungrammatical sentence.
^ May occur more than one.
List of gures
Figure 1.1 Structural Ambiguity 3
Figure 1.2 How to use ‘dude’! 7
Figure 2.1 Connecting sentences 22
Figure 2.2 Gently into that 28
Figure 3.1 From inside or into? 52
Figure 3.2 Multiple Negation 57
Figure 4.1 A schema of the functions of NPs, VPs, and AdjPs 77
Figure 4.2 Lie ahead 79
Figure 4.3 Who or whom? 84
Figure 5.1 Adverbials 89
Figure 5.2 More Phrasal verbs 92
Figure 5.3 e functions of PPs and AdvPs 96
Figure 5.4 Glasses 98
Figure 5.5 Put o until aer 99
Figure 5.6 Back up over 100
Figure 6.1 Timelines for tense and aspect 110
Figure 6.2 ree progressives 111
Figure 6.3 I think not 113
Figure 6.4 Drawed and drew 117
Figure 6.5 Timelines for tense and aspect (nal version) 121
Figure 7.1 A pony 133
Figure 7.2 Quotative ‘like 148
Figure 8.1 Embedded sentences 157
List of tables
Table 1.1 Alices Ambiguities 3
Table 2.1 Some dierences between N(oun) and V(erb) 14
Table 2.2 Dierences between adjectives and adverbs 18
Table 2.3 Some prepositions in English 19
Table 2.4 Determiners 21
Table 2.5 A few complementizers 22
Table 2.6 e categories in English 26
Table 3.1 Finding a phrase 45
Table 4.1 Subject tests 66
Table 4.2 Verbs with direct and indirect objects 70
Table 4.3 Examples of verbs with subject predicates 71
Table 4.4 Verbs with direct objects and object predicates 71
Table 4.5 Examples of the verb classes so far with their complements 74
Table 5.1 Examples of phrasal verbs 93
Table 5.2 Dierences among objects, su/obj predicates,
and adverbials 93
Table 5.3 Verb types and their complements 96
Table 6.1 Characteristics of auxiliary verbs 106
Table 6.2 Auxiliaries and their axes 114
Table 6.3 Some nite, lexical, and auxiliary verbs 119
Table 7.1 Terms for clauses 140
Table 8.1 Embedded clause 152
Table 8.2 e non-nite CP 154
Table 9.1 Components of the PP, AdjP, and AdvP 172
Table 9.2 Examples of nouns with modiers and with complements 174
Table 9.3 Functions inside the NP 175
Table 9.4 Modiers and complements to N: a summary 179
Table 10.1 Restrictive and Non-Restrictive RC 191
Table 10.2 Relative Clauses and Complement Clauses 192
Table 10.3 Examples of Reduced RC 193
Table 10.4 e sisters of CP 198
Chapter 1
Introduction
1. Examples of linguistic knowledge
2. How do we know so much?
3. Examples of social or non-linguistic knowledge
4. Conclusion
All of us know a lot about language. Most of the time, however, we are not conscious
of this knowledge. When we actually study language, we attempt to nd out what
we know and how we acquire this linguistic knowledge. In this chapter, a number of
instances will be given of what speakers of English intuitively or subconsciously know
about the grammar of English, both about its sounds and its structure. e remainder
of the book focuses on syntax, i.e. the categories, phrases, and the functions of phrases to
account for our intuitive knowledge. e chapter also discusses social, i.e. non-linguistic,
rules. ese are oen called prescriptive rules and some of these prescriptive rules are
dealt with as ‘special topics’ at the end of each chapter.
1.
Examples of linguistic knowledge
Speakers of a language know a lot about their languages. For instance, we know about
the sounds (phonology), the structure of words (morphology), and the structure of
sentences (syntax).
1.1
Sounds and words
If you are a native speaker of English, you know when to use the article a and when
to use an. All of us know how to do this correctly though we might not be able to
formulate the rule, which says that the article a occurs before a word that starts with a
consonant, as in (1), and an occurs before a word that starts with a vowel, as in (2):
(1) a nice person, a treasure
(2) an object, an artist
2 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
If a child is given a nonsense word, such as those in (3), the child knows what form of
the article to use:
(3) ovrite, cham
e rule for a(n) does not need to be taught explicitly in schools. It is only mentioned in
connection with words that start with h or u. Teachers need to explain that what looks
like a vowel in writing in (4) is not a vowel in speech and that the a/an rule is based on
spoken English. So, the form we choose depends on how the word is pronounced. In
(4) and (5), the u and h are not pronounced as vowels and hence the article a is used.
In (6) and (7), the initial u and h are pronounced as vowels and therefore an is used:
(4) a union, a university
(5) a house, a hospital
(6) an uncle
(7) an hour
e same rule predicts the pronunciation of the in (8). Pronounce the words in (8) and
see if you can state the rule for the use of the:
(8) e man, the table, the object, the hospital...
Examples (1) to (8) show the workings of a phonological (or sound) rule. e assumption
is that we possess knowledge of consonants and vowels without having been taught
the distinction. In fact, knowledge such as this enables us to learn the sound system
of the language.
Apart from the structure of the sound system, i.e. the phonology, a grammar will have
to say something about the structure of words, i.e. the morphology. Speakers are quite cre-
ative building words such as kleptocracy, cyberspace, antidisestablishmentarianisms, and
even if you have never seen them before, knowing English means that you will know
what these words mean based on their parts. Words such as occinaucinihilipilication,
meaning the categorizing of something as worthless or trivial, may be a little more dif-
cult. is book will not be concerned with sounds or with the structure of words; it
addresses how sentences are structured, usually called syntax. In the next subsection,
some examples are given of the syntactic knowledge native speakers possess.
1.2
Syntactic structure
Each speaker of English has knowledge about the structure of a sentence. is is obvi-
ous from cases of ambiguity where sentences have more than one meaning. is oen
makes them funny. For instance, the headline in (9) is ambiguous in that cello case
can mean either a court case related to a cello or someone called Celloor a case to
protect a cello’:
Chapter 1. Introduction 3
(9) Drunk Gets Ten Months In Cello Case.
In (9), the word caseis ambiguous. We call this lexical ambiguity since the ambiguity
depends on one words multiple meanings. e headlines in (10) to (12) are funny
exactly because drops, le, waes, strikes and idle are ambiguous:
(10) Eye drops o shelf.
(11) British le waes on Falkland Islands.
(12) Teacher strikes idle kids.
Word ambiguities such as (10) to (12) are oen produced on purpose for a certain
eect, and are also called ‘puns. Some well-known instances from Lewis Carroll appear
in Table 1.1.
Table 1.1. Alices Ambiguities
“Mine is a long and sad tale!” said the Mouse, turning to Alice and sighing. “It is a long tail,
certainly,” said Alice, looking with wonder at the Mouses tail, “but why do you call it sad?”
“How is bread made?” “I know that!” Alice cried eagerly. “You take some our -” “Where do you
pick the ower?” the White Queen asked. “In a garden, or in the hedges?” “Well, it isn’t picked at
all,” Alice explained; “its ground-” “How many acres of ground?” said the White Queen.
ere are also sentences where the structure is ambiguous, e.g. (13) and (14). In
(13), the monkey and elephant can both be carried in or just the monkey is. In (14),
planes can be the object of ying or the subject of the sentence:
(13) Speaker A: I just saw someone carrying a monkey and an elephant go into the circus.
Speaker B: Wow, that someone must be pretty strong.
(14) Flying planes can be dangerous.
Cartoons thrive on ambiguity and the combination of the unambiguous visual
representation with the ambiguous verbal one often provides the comic quality, as
in Figure 1.1.
Figure 1.1. Structural Ambiguity (Hi & Lois king features syndicate)
4 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
e aim of this book is to understand the structure of English sentences; ambiguity
helps understand that structure, and well come back to it in Chapter 3.
Knowing about the structure of a sentence, i.e. what parts go with other parts, is
relevant in many cases. In a yes/no question, the verb (in bold) is moved to the front of
the sentence, as from (15) to (16):
(15) e man is tall.
(16) Is the man tall?
is rule is quite complex, however. Starting from (17), we cant simply front any verb,
as (18) and (19) show. In (18), the rst verb of the sentence is fronted and this results
in an ungrammatical sentence (indicated by the *); in (19), the second verb is fronted
and this is grammatical:
(17) e man who is in the garden is tall.
(18) *Is the man who in the garden is tall?
(19) Is the man who is in the garden tall?
ese sentences show that speakers take the structure of a sentence into account when
formulating questions (see also Chapter 3). We intuitively know that the man who is in
the garden is a single unit and that the second verb is the one we need to move in order
to make the question. is is not all, however. We also need to know that not all verbs
move to form questions, as (20) shows:
(20) *Arrived the bus on time?
Only certain verbs, namely auxiliaries (see Chapter 6) and the verb to be, as in (16) and
(19), are fronted.
Apart from yes/no questions, where the expected answer is yes or no, there are
wh
questions, where more information is expected for an answer. In these sentences, the
wh-word is fronted as well as the auxiliary did. In (21), who is the object (see Chapter 4)
of the verb meet and we can check that by putting the object ‘back, as in (22), which is
possible only with special intonation:
(21) Who did Jane meet?
(22) Jane met WHO?
is rule too is complex. Why would (23a) be grammatical but (23b) ungrammatical?
(23) a. Who did you believe that Jane met?
b. *Who did you believe the story that Jane met?
Without ever having been taught this, native and most non-native speakers know
that about the dierence between (23a) and (23b). With some trouble, we can gure
out what (23b) means. ere is a story that Jane met someone and you believe
this story. e speaker in (23b) is asking who that someone is. Sentence (23b) is
Chapter 1. Introduction 5
ungrammatical because who moves ‘too far. It is possible, but not necessary here, to
make precise what ‘too far’ means. e examples merely serve to show that speakers
are aware of structure without explicit instruction and that who moves to the initial
position.
us, speakers of English know that (a) sentences are ambiguous, e.g. (13)
and (14), (b) sentences have a structure, e.g. (17), (c) movement occurs in ques-
tions, e.g. in (16) and (21), and (d) verbs are divided into (at least) two kinds: verbs
that move in questions, as in (19), and verbs that dont move, as in (20). Chapter 3
will give more information on the rst two points, Chapter 11 on the third point,
and Chapter 6 on the dierence between auxiliaries and main verbs. e other
chapters deal with additional kinds of grammatical knowledge. Chapter 2 is about
what we know regarding categories; Chapter 4 is about functions such as subject
and object; Chapter 5 about adverbials and objects; Chapter 9 about the struc-
ture of a phrase; and Chapters 7, 8, and 10 about the structure of more complex
sentences.
2.
How do we know so much?
In Section 1, we discussed examples of what we know about language without being
explicitly taught. How do we come by this knowledge? One theory that accounts for
this is suggested by Noam Chomsky. He argues that we are all born with a language
faculty that when stimulated by appropriate and continuing experience, creates
a grammar that creates sentences with formal and semantic properties” (1975: 36).
us, our innate language faculty (or Universal Grammar) enables us to create a set of
rules, or grammar, by being exposed to (rather chaotic) language around us. e set
of rules that we acquire enables us to produce sentences we have never heard before.
ese sentences can also be innitely long (if we have the time and energy). Language
acquisition, in this framework, is not imitation but an interaction between Universal
Grammar and exposure to a parti cular language.
is need for exposure to a particular language explains why, even though we
all start out with the same Language Faculty or Universal Grammar, we acquire
slightly dierent grammars. For instance, if you are exposed to a certain variety of
Missouri or Canadian English, you might use (24); if exposed to a particular variety
of British English, you might use (25); or, if exposed to a kind of American English,
(26) and (27):
(24) I want for to go.
(25) You know as she le. (meaning ‘You know that she le’)
(26) She don’t learn you nothing.
(27) Was you ever bit by a bee?
6 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
us, “[l]earning is primarily a matter of lling in detail within a structure that is
innate (Chomsky 1975: 39). A physical organ, say the heart, may vary from one
person to the next in size or strength, but its basic structure and its function within
human physiology are common to the species. Analogously, two individuals in the
same speech community may acquire grammars that dier somewhat in scale and
subtlety. . . . ese variations in structure are limited . . .” (p. 38).
Hence, even though Universal Grammar provides us with categories such as
nouns and verbs that enable us to build our own grammars, the language we hear
around us will determine the particular grammar we build up. A person growing
up in the 14th century heard multiple negation, as in (28), and would have had
a grammar that allowed multiple negation. e same holds for a person from the
15th century who has heard (29). e Modern English equivalents, given in the
single quotation marks, show that many varieties of English now use ‘any’ instead of
another negative:
(28) Men neded not in no cuntre A fairer body for to seke.
‘People did not need to seek a fairer person in any country.
(Chaucer, e Romaunt of the Rose, 560–1)
(29) for if he had he ne nedid not to haue sent no spyes.
‘because if he had, he would not have needed to send any spies.
(e Paston Letters, letter 45 from 1452)
Linguists typically say that one variety of a language is just as ‘good’ as any other. Peo-
ple may judge one variety as ‘bad’ and another as ‘good’, but for most people studying
language, (24) through (27) are just interesting, not ‘incorrect. is holds for language
change as well: the change from (28) and (29) to Modern English is not seen as either
progressor decay’, but as a fact to be explained. Languages are always changing and
the fascinating part is to see the regularities in the changes.
Society has rules about language, which I call social or ‘non-linguistic, and which
we need to take into account to be able to function. ese are occasionally at odds with
the (non-prescriptive) grammars speakers have in their heads. is is addressed in the
next section.
3.
Examples of social or non-linguistic knowledge
We know when not to make jokes, for instance, when lling out tax forms or speaking
with airport security people. We also know not to use words and expressions such
as all you guys, awesome, and I didn’t get help from nobody in formal situations such as
applying for a job or in a formal presentation. Using dude
in the situation of Figure 1.2
may not be smart either. We learn when and how to be polite and impolite; formal
Chapter 1. Introduction 7
and informal. e rules for this dier from culture to culture and, when we learn
a new language, we also need to learn the politeness rules and rules for greetings,
requests, etc.
Figure 1.2. How to use ‘dude’! (Used with the permission of Mike Twohy and the Cartoonist
Group. All rights reserved)
When you are in informal situations (e.g. watching TV with a friend), everyone
expects ‘prescriptively proscribed’ expressions, such as (30). In formal situations (testify-
ing in court), you might use (31) instead:
(30) I didn’t mean nothin’ by it.
(31) I didn’t intend to imply anything with that remark.
e dierences between (30) and (31) involve many levels: (a) vocabulary choice,
e.g. mean rather than intend, (b) phonology, e.g. nothin
for nothing, (c) syntax, namely
the two negatives in (30) that make one negative, and (d) style, e.g. (30) is much less
explicit. People use the distinction between formal and informal for ‘eect’ as well, as
in (32):
(32) You should be better prepared the next time you come to class. Ain’t no way Im
gonna take this.
8 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Style and grammar are oen equated but they are not the same. Passive constructions,
for instance, occur in all languages, and are certainly grammatical. ey are oen advised
against for reasons of style because the author may be seen as avoiding taking responsibility
for his or her views. In many kinds of writing, e.g. scientic, passives are very frequent.
is book is not about the ght between descriptivism (‘what people really say’)
and prescriptivism (‘what some people think other people ought to say’). As with all
writing or speech, this book makes a number of choices, e.g. use of contractions,
1
use
of I’ and ‘we’ as well as a frequent use of passives, and avoidance of very long sentences.
is, however, is irrelevant to the main point which is to provide the vocabulary and
analytical skills to examine descriptive as well as prescriptive rules. e eld that exam-
ines the status of prescriptive rules, regional forms as in (24) to (27), and formal and
informal language, as in (30) to (32), is called sociolinguistics.
Some prescriptive rules are analysed in the special topics sessions at the end of
every chapter. e topics covered are split innitives (
to boldly go where . . .), adverbs
and adjectives, multiple negation, as in (30), case marking and subject–verb agree-
ment, the use of passives, the use of of rather than have (I should of done that), the
preposition like used as a complementizer (like I said ...), dangling modiers, singular
and plural pronouns, relative pronouns, and the correct’ use of commas. ere are
many more such rules.
4.
Conclusion
is rst chapter has given instances of rules we know without having been taught
these rules explicitly. It also oers an explanation about why we know this much:
Universal Grammar ‘helpsus. Other chapters in the book provide the categories and
structures that we must be using to account for this intuitive knowledge. e chapter
also provides instances of social or non-linguistic rules. ese are oen called pre-
scriptive rules and some of these are dealt with as ‘special topicsat the end of each of
the chapters. is chapters special topic discusses one of the more infamous prescrip-
tive rules, namely the split innitive.
e key terms in this chapter are syntax; lexical and structural ambiguity; puns;
linguistic as opposed to social or non-linguistic knowledge; descriptive as opposed
to prescriptive rules; formal as opposed to informal language; innate faculty; and
Universal Grammar.
1. A copy-editor for the first edition changed the contracted forms to full ones, however, and I
havent put the contractions back where they had been changed.
Chapter 1. Introduction 9
Exercises
A. Using the words lexical and structural ambiguity, explain the ambiguity in (33) to (37):
(33) light house keeper
(34) old dogs and cats
(35) She gave her dog biscuits.
(36) Speaker A: Is your fridge running?
Speaker B: Yes.
Speaker A: Better go catch it!
(37) Fish are smart. They always swim in schools.
B. Do you think the following sentences are prescriptively correct or not. Why/why not?
(38) It looks good.
(39) Me and my friend went out.
(40) Hopefully, hunger will be eliminated.
(41) There’s cookies for everyone.
Class discussion
C. Can you think of something you would say in an informal situation but not in a formal one?
Suggestion: If you have access to the internet, check the British National Corpus (BNC at
http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/) or the Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA
at http://www.americancorpus.org/) to see if this use is found. If you wonder what a
corpus is, it is a carefully selected set of texts that represents the language of a particular
time or variety (British in the case of the BNC and American in the case of the COCA).
D.
Has your English ever been corrected? Can you remember when?
E. List some stylistic rules. In the text, I mentioned the avoidance of the passive. You might
check http://www.libraryspot.com/grammarstyle.htm with links to a collection of
grammar and style books.
F.
Discuss why prescriptively correct constructions are often used in formal situations.
G. You may have heard of best-selling ‘language mavens’ such as William Sare or Edwin
Newman. Sare was a political commentator who also wrote a weekly column in the
Sunday New York Times. Titles of his books include Good Advice, I Stand Corrected: More
on Language, and Language Maven Strikes Again. Newman, a former NBC correspondent,
writes books entitled A Civil Tongue and Strictly Speaking. These lead reviewers to say
“Read Newman! Save English before it is fatally slain. (backcover)
Why are there language authorities?
Why do people listen to them?
H. Have you seen titles such as An History of the English Language’? Is this correct according
to our rule in Section 1.1? Google it and see if a history or ‘an history is more frequent.
10 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Keys to the exercises
A. (33) shows structural ambiguity: [[light house] keeper] or [light][house keeper].
(34) shows structural ambiguity: [old dogs] and [cats] or [old [dogs and cats]]
(35) again shows structural ambiguity: She gave [her] [dog biscuits] or She gave [her
dog] [biscuits].
(36) shows lexical ambiguity: running can be physical running or running as an
engine does.
(37) shows lexical ambiguity: schools has two meanings.
B. (38) is ok since good is an adjective giving more information about the pronoun it
(see Chapter 2 and special topic).
(39) is not prescriptively correct since the subject should get nominative case (see Chapter 4
and special topic) and because many people are taught not to start with themselves rst.
(40) is not since hopefully is not supposed to be used as a sentence adverb, i.e. an adverb
that says something about the attitude of the speaker (see Chapter 5 and special topic of
Chapter 2).
(41) is not since the verb is singular (is) and the subject is plural (cookies). This violates
subject-verb agreement (see Chapters 3 and 4).
Special topic: Split innitive
In a later chapter, we will discuss innitives in great detail. For now, I just want to discuss the
prescriptive rule against splitting innitives that almost everyone knows and show that split
innitives have occurred in English at least for 700 years.
Innitives are verbs preceded by a to that is not a preposition but an innitive marker.
Some examples are given in (42) to (44), where the innitive and its marker are in bold:
(42) To err is human.
(43) It is nice to wander aimlessly.
(44) To be or not to be is to be decided.
The prescriptive rule is not to split this innitive from its marker, as stated in (45):
(45) Do not separate an innitival verb from its accompanying to, as in Star Trek’s ‘to
boldly go where no man has gone before’.
2
2. This is the version from the early episodes of Star Trek which was much criticized for the split
innitive. Later episodes changed no man to no one and that’s how the 2009 lm version has it.
Chapter 1. Introduction 11
Swan writes that “[s]plit innitive structures are quite common in English, especially in an
informal style. A lot of people consider them ‘bad style’, and avoid them if possible, placing the
adverb before the to, or in end-position in the sentence (1980: 327). Fowler writes as follows:
The English-speaking world may be divided into (1) those who neither know nor care what a
split innitive is; (2) those who do not know but care very much; (3) those who know & condemn;
(4) those who know & approve; & (5) those who know & distinguish. (1926 [1950]: 558)
Fowler himself disapproves of the use of the split innitive. Quirk & Greenbaum are less critical.
The inseparability of to from the innitive is . . . asserted in the widely held opinion that it is bad
style to split the innitive. Thus rather than:
?He was wrong to suddenly leave the country
many people (especially in BrE) prefer:
He was wrong to leave the country suddenly
It must be acknowledged, however, that in some cases the split innitive is the only tolerable
ordering, since avoiding the ‘split innitive results in clumsiness or ambiguity. (1973: 312)
Split innitives have occurred from the Middle English period, i.e. from 1200, on, as (46) to (52) show.
(46) I want somebody who will be on there not to legislate from the bench but to
faithfully interpret the constitution. (George Bush, quoted in The Economist,
6 July 1991)
(47) Remember to always footnote the source.
(48) [This] will make it possible for everyone to gently push up the fees. (New York
Times, 21 July 1991)
(49) . . .to get the Iraqis to peacefully surrender... (New York Times, 7 July 1991)
(50) fo[r] to londes seche
‘To see countries. (Layamon Brut Otho 6915, early 13th century)
(51) Y say to 3ou, to nat swere on al manere
‘I say to you to not curse in all ways. (Wyclif, Matthew 5, 34, late 14th century)
(52) Poul seiþ, þu þat prechist to not steyl, stelist,
‘Paul says that you who preaches to not steal steals. (Apology for the Lollards 57,
late 14th century)
Would you change these? If so, how? In this book, I have not avoided them on purpose and
know of at least one instance where I have split an innitive.
Chapter 2
Categories
1. Lexical categories
2. Grammatical categories
3. Pronouns
4. What new words and loanwords tell us!
5. Conclusion
In this chapter, I provide descriptions of the categories or parts of speech. Categories
can be divided into two main classes: lexical and functional. e lexical categories
include Noun, Verb, Adjective, Adverb, and Preposition and are called lexical because
they carry lexical meaning. ey are also called content words since they have syn-
onyms and antonyms. As we’ll see in the next chapter, syntactically, lexical categories
are the heads of phrases.
ere are also functional or grammatical categories: Determiner, Auxiliary, Coor-
dinator, and Complementizer. ese categories are called grammatical or functional
categories since they do not contribute much to the meaning of a sentence but deter-
mine the syntax of it. ey do not function as heads of phrases but merely as parts or
as connectors. I’ll refer to them as grammatical categories. Prepositions and adverbs
are a little of both as will be explained in Sections 1.2 and 1.3 respectively, as are pro-
nouns, e.g. it, she, and there, to be discussed in Section 3.
When languages borrow new words, these will mainly be nouns, verbs, and adjec-
tives, i.e. lexical categories. erefore, the dierence between lexical and grammatical
is oen put in terms of open as opposed to closed categories, the lexical categories
being open (new words can be added) and the grammatical ones being closed (new
words are not easily added). Section 4 will examine this in a limited way.
.
Lexical categories
e ve lexical categories are Noun, Verb, Adjective, Adverb, and Preposition. ey
carry meaning, and often words with a similar (synonym) or opposite meaning
Chapter 2. Categories 
(antonym) can be found. Frequently, the noun is said to be a person, place, or thing
and the verb is said to be an event or act. ese are semantic denitions. In this chapter,
it is shown that semantic denitions are not completely adequate and that we need to
dene categories syntactically (according to what they combine with) and morpholog-
ically (according to how the words are formed). For example, syntactically speaking,
chair is a noun because it combines with the article (or determiner) the; morphologi-
cally speaking, chair is a noun because it takes a plural ending as in chairs.
.
Nouns (N) and Verbs (V)
A noun generally indicates a person, place or thing (i.e. this is its meaning). For
instance, chair, table, and book are nouns since they refer to things. However, if the
distinction between a noun as person, place, or thing and a verb as an event or action
were the only distinction, certain nouns such as action and destruction would be verbs,
since they imply action. ese elements are nevertheless nouns.
In (1) and (2), actions and destruction are preceded by the article the, actions can be
made singular by taking the plural -s o, and destruction can be pluralized with an -s.
is makes them nouns:
(1) e actions by the government came too late.
(2) e hurricane caused the destruction of the villages.
As will be shown in Chapter 4, their functions in the sentence are also typical for
nouns rather than verbs: in (1), actions is part of the subject, and in (2), destruction
is part of the object.
Apart from plural -s, other morphological characteristics of nouns are shown
in (3) and (4). Possessive ’s (or genitive case) appears only on nouns or noun phrases,
e.g. the noun Jenny in (3), and axes such as -er and -ism, e.g. writer and postmodern-
ism in (4), are also typical for nouns:
(3) Jenny ’s neighbor always knows the answer.
(4) at writer has modernized postmodernism.
Syntactic reasons for calling nouns nouns are that nouns are oen preceded by the,
as actions and government are in (1), as destruction and villages are in (2), and as
answer is in (3). Nouns can also be preceded by that, as in (4), and, if they are fol-
lowed by another noun, there has to be a preposition, such as by in (1) and of in (2),
connecting them.
e nouns action and destruction have verbal counterparts, namely act and destroy,
and (1) and (2) can be paraphrased as (5) and (6) respectively:
(5) e government acted too late.
(6) e hurricane destroyed the villages.
Just as nouns cannot always be dened as people or things, verbs are not always acts,
even though acted and destroyed are. e verb be in (7), represented by the third
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
person present form is, does not express an action. Hence, we need to add state to the
semantic denition of verb, as well as emotion to account for sentences such as (8):
(7) e book is red and blue.
(8) e book seemed nice (to me).
Some of the morphological characteristics of verbs are that they can express tense,
e.g. past tense ending -ed in (5), (6), and (8); that the verb ends in -s when it has a
third person singular subject (see Chapter 4) and is present tense; and that it may have
an ax typical for verbs, namely -ize, e.g. in modernized in (4) (note that it is -ise in
British English). Syntactically, they can be followed by a noun, as in (6), as well as by
a preposition and they can be preceded by an auxiliary, as in (4). Some of the major
dierences between nouns and verbs are summarized in Table 2.1 below.
Table 2.1. Some dierences between N(oun) and V(erb)
Noun (N) Verb (V)
Morphology a. plural -s with a few exceptions,
e.g. children, deer, mice
h. past tense -ed with a few
exceptions, e.g. went, le
b. possessive ’s i. third person singular
agreement -s
c. some end in -ity, -ness -ation, -er,
-ion, -ment
j. some end in -ize,-ate
Syntax d. may follow the/a and this/that/
these/those
k. may follow an auxiliary
e.g. have and will
e. may be modied by adjective l. may be modied by adverb
f. may be followed by preposition
and noun
m. may be followed by noun or
preposition and noun
Semantics g. person, place, thing n. act, event, state, emotion
In English, nouns can easily be used as verbs and verbs as nouns. erefore, it is
necessary to look at the context in which a word occurs, as in (9), for example, where
Shakespeare uses vnckle, i.e. ‘uncle, as a verb as well as a noun:
(9) York: Grace me no Grace, nor Vnckle me,
I am no Traytors Vnckle; and that word Grace
In an vngracious mouth, is but prophane.
(Shakespeare, Richard II, II, 3, 96, as in the First Folio edition)
us, using the criteria discussed above, the rst instance of unclewould be a verb
since the noun following it does not need to be connected to the verb by means of a
preposition, and the second uncleis a noun since traitor’ has the possessive ’s. Note
that I have le Shakespeares spelling, punctuation, and grammar as they appear in the
First Folio Edition.
Chapter 2. Categories 
Other examples where a word can be both a noun and a verb are table, to table;
chair, to chair; oor, to oor; book, to book; fax, to fax; telephone, to telephone; and walk,
to walk. Some of these started out as nouns and some as verbs. For instance, fax is the
shortened form of the noun facsimile which became used as a verb as well. Currently,
when people say fax, they oen mean pdf (portable document format), another noun
that is now used as a verb. A sentence where police is used as noun, verb, and adjective
respectively is (10a); (10b) is nicely alliterating where pickle is used as a verb, adjective,
and noun; and (10c) has fast as adjective, adverb, and noun:
(10) a. Police police police outings regularly in the meadows of Malacandra.
b. Did Peter Piper pickle pickled pickles?
(Alyssa Bachmans example)
c. e fast girl recovered fast aer her fast.
(Amy Shinabargers example)
As we’ll see, other words can be ambiguous in this way.
As a summary to Section 1.1, use Table 2.1. Not all of these properties are always
present of course. Morphological dierences involve the shape of an element while
syntactic ones involve how the element ts in a sentence. e semantic dierences
involve meaning, but remember to be careful here since nouns, for instance, can have
plural -s in (1) and (2) above.
Dierences (e) and (l) will be explained in the next section. ey are evident in
(11), which shows the adjective expensive that modies (i.e. says something about) the
noun book and the adverb quickly that modies the verb sold out:
(11) at expensive book sold out quickly.
. Adjectives (Adj) and Adverbs (Adv)
Adverbs and Adjectives are semantically very similar in that both modify another ele-
ment, i.e. they describe a quality of another word: quick/ly, nice/ly, etc. As just men-
tioned, the main syntactic distinction is as expressed in (12):
(12) e Adjective-Adverb Rule
An adjective modies a noun;
an adverb modies a verb and (a degree adverb) modies an adjective or adverb.
Since an adjective modies a noun, the quality it describes will be one appropri-
ate to a noun, e.g. nationality/ethnicity (American, Navajo, Dutch, Iranian), size (big,
large, thin), age (young, old), color (red, yellow, blue), material/personal description
(wooden, human), or character trait (happy, fortunate, lovely, pleasant, obnoxious).
Adverbs oen modify actions and will then provide information typical of those,
e.g. manner (wisely, fast, quickly, slowly), or duration (frequently, oen), or speaker
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
attitude (fortunately, actually), or place (there, abroad), or time (then, now, yesterday).
As well and also, and negatives such as not and never, are also adverbs in that they
usually modify the verb.
When adverbs modify adjectives or other adverbs, they are called degree adverbs
(very, so, too). ese degree adverbs have very little meaning (except some that can
add avor to the degree, such as exceedingly and amazingly) and it is hard to nd
synonyms or antonyms. It therefore makes more sense to consider this subgroup of
adverbs grammatical categories. ey also do not head a phrase of their own, and
when it looks as if they do, there really is another adjective or adverb le out. e very
in (13) modies important, which is le out:
(13) How important is your job to you? Ver y.
(from CBS 60 Minutes 1995).
Some instances of the use of the adjective nice are given in (14) and (15). Traditionally,
the use in (14) is called predicative and that in (15) attributive:
(14) e book is nice.
(15) A nice book is on the table.
e adverbs very and quickly appear in (16) and (17):
(16) is Hopi bowl is very precious.
(17) He drove very quickly.
In (14) and (15), nice modies the noun book. In (16), the degree adverb very modi-
es the adjective precious; and in (17), it modies the adverb quickly, which in its turn
modies the verb drove. (We will come back to some of the issues related to the precise
nature of the modication in Chapters 3, 4, and 9). In the special topic’ section at the
end of this chapter, it will be shown that speakers oen violate rule (12), but that these
so-called violations are rule-governed as well.
Sentence (16) shows something else, namely that the noun Hopi can also be used
to modify another noun. When words are put together like this, they are called com-
pound words. Other examples are given in (18) and (19):
(18) So the principal says to the [chemistry teacher], “You’ll have to teach physics this
year.” (from Science Activities 1990)
(19) Relaxing in the living room of his unpretentious red [stone house], …
(from Forbes 1990)
Some of these compounds may end up being seen or written as one word; others are
two words e.g. girlfriend, bookmark, mail-carrier, re engine, dog food, and stone age.
When we see a noun modifying another noun, as in (18) and (19), we will discuss if
they are compounds or not. e space and hyphen between the two words indicate
degrees of closeness.
Chapter 2. Categories 
Oen, an adverb is formed from an adjective by adding -ly, as in (17). However,
be careful with this morphological distinction: not all adverbs end in -ly, e.g. fast,
and hard can be adjectives as well as adverbs and some adjectives end in -ly,
e.g. friendly, lovely, lively, and wobbly. If you are uncertain as to whether a word is an
adjective or an adverb, either look in a dictionary to see what it says, or use it in a
sentence to see what it modies. For instance, in (20), fast is an adjective because it
modies the noun car, but in (21), it is an adverb since it modies the verb drove:
(20) at fast car must be a police car.
(21) at car drove fast until it saw the photo radar.
In a number of cases, words such as hard and fast can be adjectives or adverbs, depend-
ing on the interpretation. In (22), hard can either modify the noun person, i.e. the
person looks tough or nasty, in which case it is an adjective, or it can modify look
(meaning that the person was looking all over the place for something, i.e. the eort
was great) in which case hard is an adverb:
(22) at person looked hard.
As a reader of this sentence, what is your preference? Checking a contemporary
American corpus, i.e. a set of representative texts, I found that most speakers use hard
as an adverb aer the verb look. Do you agree?
Some of the discrepanciesbetween form and function are caused by language change.
For instance, the degree adverb very started out its life being borrowed as an adjective from
the French verrai (in the 13th century) with the meaning ‘true, as in (23):
(23) Under the colour of a veray peax, whiche is neuertheles but a cloked and furred peax.
Under the color of a true peace, which is nevertheless nothing but a cloaked and
furred peace.’ (Cromwell’s 16th century Letters)
Here, what looks like a -y ending is a rendering of the Old French verrai. Whats worse for
confusing Modern English speakers is that, in Old English, adverbs did not need to end in
-lich or -ly. ats why ‘old’ adverbs sometimes keep that shape, e.g. rst in (24) is a correct
adverb, but second is not. e reason that secondly is prescribed rather than second is that
it was borrowed late from French, at a time when English adverbs typically received -ly
endings.
(24) first I had to watch the accounts and secondly I’m looking at all this stu for
when I start my business. (from a conversation in the BNC Corpus)
A last point to make about adjectives and adverbs is that most (if they are gradable)
can be used to compare or contrast two or more things. We call such forms the com-
parative (e.g. better than) or superlative (e.g. the best). One way to make these forms is
to add -er/-est, as in nicer/nicest. Not all adjectives/adverbs allow this ending, however;
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
some need to be preceded by more/most, as in more intelligent, most intelligent. Some-
times, people are creative with comparatives and superlatives, especially in advertising,
as in (25) and (26), or in earlier forms as in (27):
(25) mechanic: “the expensivest oil is …
(26) advertisement: “the bestest best ever phone.
(27) To take the basest and most poorest shape … (Shakespeare, King Lear II, 3, 7)
ere are also irregular comparative and superlative forms, such as good, better, best;
bad, worse, worst. ese have to be learned as exceptions to the rules, and can be played
with, as in the pun ‘When I am bad, I am better.
To summarize this section, I’ll provide a table listing dierences between adjectives
and adverbs. Not all of these dierences have been discussed yet, e.g. the endings -ous, -ary,
-al, and -ic are typical for adjectives and -wise, and -ways for adverbs, but they speak for
themselves.
Table 2.2. Dierences between adjectives and adverb
Adjectives (Adj) Adverbs (Adv)
Morphology a. end in -ous, -ary, -al, -ic;
mostly have no -ly;
and can be participles
d. end in -ly in many cases, -wise,
-ways, etc. or have no ending
(fast, now)
Syntax b. modify N e. modify V, Adj, or Adv
Semantics c. describe qualities typical of nouns,
e.g: nationality, color, size
f. describe qualities of verbs,
e.g: place, manner, time,
duration, etc. and of adjectives/
adverbs: degree
. Prepositions (P)
Prepositions typically express place or time (at, in, on, before), direction (to, from, into,
down), causation (for), or relation (of, about, with, like, as, near). ey are invariable
in form and have to occur before a noun, as (28) shows, where the prepositions are in
bold and the nouns they go with are underlined:
(28) With their books about linguistics, they went to school.
On occasion, what look like prepositions are used on their own, as in (29):
(29) He went in; they ran out; and he jumped down.
In such cases, these words are considered adverbs, not prepositions. e dierence
between prepositions and adverbs is that prepositions come before the nouns they
relate to and that adverbs are on their own.
Some other examples of one word prepositions are during, around, aer, against,
despite, except, without, towards, until, till, and inside. Sequences such as instead of,
Chapter 2. Categories 
outside of, away from, due to, and as for are also considered to be prepositions, even
though they consist of more than one word. Infrequently, prepositions are transformed
into verbs, as in (30):
(30) ey upped the price.
Some prepositions have very little lexical meaning and are mainly used for grammati-
cal purposes. For instance, of in (31) expresses a relationship between two nouns rather
than a locational or directional meaning:
(31) e door of that car.
Prepositions are therefore a category with lexical and grammatical characteristics.
Here, however, I will treat them as lexical, for the sake of simplicity. A partial list is
given in Table 2.3.
Table 2.3. Some prepositions in English
about, above, across, aer, against, along, amidst, among, around, at, before, behind, below,
beneath, beside(s), between, beyond, by, concerning, despite, down, during, except, for, from, in,
into, inside, like, near, of, o, on, onto, opposite, outside, over, past, since, through, to, toward(s),
under, underneath, until, up, upon, with, within, without
. Grammatical categories
e main grammatical categories are Determiner, Auxiliary, Coordinator, and Com-
plementizer. As also mentioned above, it is hard to dene grammatical categories in
terms of meaning because they have very little. eir function is to make the lexical
categories t together.
.
Determiner (D)
e determiner category includes the articles a(n) and the, as well as demonstratives,
possessive pronouns, possessive nouns, some quantiers, some interrogatives, and
some numerals. So, determiner (or D) is an umbrella term for all of these. Determin-
ers occur with a noun to specify which noun is meant or whose it is. If you are a native
speaker, you know how to use the indenite article a and the denite article the. For
non-native speakers, guring out their use is very dicult.
e indenite article is oen used when the noun that follows it is new in the text/
conversation, such as the rst mention of Florida manatee in (32) is. e second and
third mentions of it are preceded by the denite article the:
(32) e fate of a Florida manatee that has wandered into northern New Jersey waters
remained unclear Saturday night. e wayward male – known as Ilya – has been
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
stuck near a Linden oil renery, and ocials say plunging temperatures and a lack of
food were endangering its life. And while the gentle sea cow appears to be in good
health, it had been huddling near an outfall pipe at an oil renery – the only place it
could nd warm water. (from Hungton Post)
ere are four demonstratives in English: this, that, these, and those, with the rst two
for singular nouns and the last two for plural ones. See (33a). Possessive pronouns
include my, your, his, her, its, our, and their, as in (33b). Nouns can be possessives as
well, but in that case they have an -’s (or ) ending, as in (33c):
(33) a. at javelina loved these trails.
b. eir kangaroo ate my food.
c. Guccis food was eaten by Coco.
3
In (33b), their and my specify whose kangaroo and whose food it was, and the posses-
sive noun Guccis in (33c) species whose food was eaten.
Determiners, as in (32) and (33), precede nouns just like adjectives, but whereas
a determiner points out which entity is meant (it species), an adjective describes the
quality (it modies). When both a determiner and an adjective precede a noun, the
determiner always precedes the adjective, as in (34a), and not the other way round, as
in (34b) (indicated by the asterisk):
(34) a. eir irritating dog ate my delicious food.
b. *Irritating their dog ate delicious my food.
Interrogatives such as whose in whose books, what in what problems, and which in
which computer are determiners. Quantiers such as any, many, much, and all are usu-
ally considered determiners, e.g. in much work, many people, and all research. Some are
used before other determiners, namely, all, both, and half, as in (35). ese quantiers
are called pre-determiners, and abbreviated Pre-D. Finally, quantiers may be adjectival,
as in the many problems and in (36):
(35) All the books; half that mans money; both those problems.
(36) e challenges are many/few.
Numerals are sometimes determiners, as in two books, and sometimes more like
adjecti ves, as in my two books. Table 2.4 shows the determiners in the order in which
they may appear. I have added the category adjective to the table since some of the
words that are clear determiners can also be adjectives. e categories are not always a
100% clear-cut, and (37) tries to shed some light on the dierence.
3.  Believe it or not, Gucci and Coco are names of real dogs!
Chapter 2. Categories 
Table 2.4. Determiners
Pre-D D Adj
quantier all, both
half
some, many, all, few(er)
any, much, no, every, less, etc.
many, few
article the, a
demonstrative that, this, those, these
possessive my, etc., NP’s
interrogative whose, what, which, etc.
numeral one, two, etc. one, two, etc.
(37) e Determiner-Adjective Rule
A Determiner points to the noun it goes with and who it belongs to;
An Adjective gives background information about the noun.
. Auxiliary (AUX)
is category will be dealt with in detail in Chapter 6. For now, it suces to say that, as
its name implies, the auxiliary verb functions to help another verb, but does not itself
contribute greatly to the meaning of the sentence.
Verbs such as have, be, and do can be lexical verbs or auxiliaries. In (38a), have
is a lexical verb because it has a meaning ‘to possessand occurs without any other
lexical verb. In (38b), on the other hand, have does not mean possessor ‘hold’, but
contributes to the grammatical meaning of the sentence, namely past tense with pres-
ent relevance. It therefore is an auxiliary to the lexical verb worked. e same is true
for be in (39). In (39a), it is the only verb and therefore lexical; in (39b), it contributes
to the grammatical meaning emphasizing the continuous nature of the event. Lexical
and auxiliary uses of do are given in (40a) and (40b) respectively:
(38) a. I have a book in my hand.
b. I have worked here for 15 years.
(39) a. at man is a hard worker.
b. at reindeer may be working too hard.
(40) a. She did her homework.
b. She didnt sleep at all.
Because auxiliaries help other verbs (except when they are main verbs as in (38)),
they cannot occur on their own. us, (41) is ungrammatical:
(41) *I must a book.
. Coordinator (C) and Complementizer (C)
In this section, we discuss two categories that join other words or phrases. Coordinators
are relatively simple and join similar categories or phrases. Complementizers introduce
subordinate clauses and look remarkably similar to prepositions and adverbs. We abbre-
viate both as C.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Coordinators such as and and or join two elements of the same kind, e.g. the
nouns in (42):
(42) Rigobertha and Pablo went to Madrid or Barcelona.
ey are also sometimes called coordinating conjunctions, as in Figure 2.1, but in this
book, well use coordinator. ere are also two-part coordinators such as both and,
either or, and neither nor.
Figure 2.1. Connecting sentences (Reprinted with the permission of Universal Press Syndicate. All
rights reserved)
Complementizers such as that, because, whether, if, and since join two clauses
where one clause is subordinate to the other (see Chapter 7 for more), as in (43). e
subordinate clause is indicated by means of brackets:
(43) Rigobertha and Pablo le [because Isabella was about to arrive].
ey are also called subordinating conjunctions or subordinators. We will use com-
plementizer. Like prepositions, coordinators and complementizers are invariable
in English (i.e. never have an ending), but complementizers introduce a new clause
whereas prepositions are connected to a noun. Some examples of complementizers
and some of their other functions (if they have them) are provided in Table 2.5.
Table 2.5. A few complementizers
C Example of C use Other use Example of other use
aer Aer she le, it rained. preposition aer him
as Fair as the moon is, it… degree adverb as nice
because (43)
before Before it snowed, it rained. preposition before me
for I expect for you to do that. preposition for Santa
if If she wins, that will be great.
so He was tired, so he went to sleep. adverb so tired
that I know that the earth is round. D that book
when I wonder when it will happen. adverb He le when?
while She played soccer, while he slept. noun A short while
Chapter 2. Categories 
ere is a group of words, namely yet, however, nevertheless, therefore, and so, as in
(44), that connects one sentence to another:
(44) “you are anxious for a compliment, so I will tell you that you have improved her”.
(Jane Austen, Emma, Vol 1, Chap 8)
Some grammarians see these as complementizers; others see them as adverbs. With
the punctuation as in (44), the complementizer scenario is more obvious since so con-
nects the two sentences. However, so sometimes appears at the beginning of a sen-
tence, in which case it could be an adverb expressing the reason why something was
done. I leave it up to you to decide what to do with these. You may remember from
Section 1.2 that so can also be a degree adverb, as in so nice.
We can now formulate another rule, namely the one in (45):
(45) e Preposition-Complementizer-Adverb Rule
A Preposition introduces a noun (e.g. about the book);
a Complementizer introduces a sentence (e.g. because he le); and
an Adverb is on its own (e.g. She went out; and Unfortunatel y, she le).
ese categories are oen ambiguous in Modern English because prepositions and
adverbs can change to complementizers.
.
Pronouns
In this section, I discuss the dierent pronouns in English. Pronouns are a hybrid
category since they do not carry much lexical meaning but they can function on their
own, unlike articles and complementizers, which need something to follow them. is
makes them hard to classify as lexical or grammatical categories.
Personal pronouns, such as I, me, she, he and it, and reexive pronouns, such
as myself, yourself, and herself, are seen as grammatical categories by many (myself
included). e reason is that they don’t mean very much: they are used to refer to
phrases already mentioned. However, in this book, I label personal and reexive pro-
nouns the same way as nouns, since they function like full Noun Phrases as Subjects
and Objects (more on this in Chapter 4). us, a determiner such as the cannot stand
on its own, but she, as in (46) from Shakespeare, can:
(46) Twere good she were spoken with,
For she may strew dangerous coniectures
in ill breeding minds. (Hamlet, IV, 5, 14)
Personal pronouns can be divided according to number into singular and plural and
according to person into rst, second, and third person. For example, I and me are rst
person singular, and we and us are rst person plural. e second person pronoun you
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
is used both as singular and as plural. ird person singular pronouns he/him, she/her,
and it are further divided according to gender; the third person plural pronouns are
they and them.
Pronouns look like the determiners we saw in the previous section. Almost all
determiners, except the articles, can stand on their own, e.g. demonstratives, such as
that in that is a problem. us, they are very much like pronouns, but they can in prin-
ciple have a noun following. erefore, I will label something a D if it can have a noun
following it but a pronoun if it can’t.
Apart from personal and reexive pronouns, there are some possessive pronouns
that occur on their own, and are therefore not determiners. Examples are mine, yours,
his, hers, ours, and theirs, as in (47a). ese pronouns appear when the noun they
specify has been le unspecied. us, (47a) could be rewritten as (47b), with mine
replaced by my e-mail:
(47) a. at e-mail is not mine, but it is yours.
b. at e-mail is not my e-mail, but it is your e-mail.
e result is awkward, however, and I will suggest that mine and yours are really inde-
pendent pronouns, not determiners with the noun le out.
e other determiners, namely interrogatives, quantiers, and numerals can occur
independently too, as in (48). It will be up to you as the reader to decide whether these
are independent pronouns or are really determiners preceding nouns that have been
le out:
(48) What would be solved if all chose two?
Indenite pronouns, such as anyone, anybody, everyone, someone, something, and noth-
ing, occur frequently and are in many ways similar to personal pronouns. ere are
many other indenites that are similar to adverbs, e.g. anywhere, nowhere, sometime,
and somewhere, or to degree adverbs, e.g. somewhat. ey are pro-forms and can stand
in for an adverb. When I label them in the answers to the exercises, I will indicate that
they are pronouns as well adverbs.
In this section, we discussed several types of pronouns. Some occur indepen-
dently (I, you, and mine) and others occur with a noun and are Ds (my and whose).
ere are other pronouns we will encounter, mainly relative ones and adverbs such as
then and there.
.
What new words and loanwords tell us!
Some of the new words of the late 20th and early 21th century are geocache, sudoku,
podcast (from the Oxford English Dictionary online), spyware, mouse potato, agritourism
Chapter 2. Categories 
(from the Merriam Webster online), and facebook, google, kindle, twitter, sustainability,
pwned, and texting, to name but a few, and they are all lexical categories! Some of the
new words or expressions are loanwords (e.g. sudoku is from Japanese, pwn possible
from Welsh), some are extensions of other meanings (bookmark a site from bookmark
a book), some are clipped (weblog becomes blog), others come from special (pwned
from internet gaming) but all are lexical, rather than grammatical categories.
Lewis Carrolls Jabberwocky includes a number of nonsensewords. As an exer-
cise, at the end of the chapter, you’ll be asked what category each of these is. For now,
it is enough to point out that they are all lexical:
‘Twas brillig, and the slithy toves
Did gyre and gimble in the wabe:
All mimsy were the borogoves
And the mome raths outgrabe.
“Beware the Jabberwock, my son!
e jaws that bite, the claws that catch!
Beware the Jubjub bird and shun
e frumious Bandersnatch!
He took his vorpal sword in hand:
Longtime the manxome foe he sought -
So rested he by the Tumtum tree
And stood a while in thought. (…)
ere are other phenomena that the lexical/grammatical distinction sheds light on.
For instance, children learn lexical categories before grammatical ones, and people
with aphasia can have diculties with either lexical or grammatical categories (see
Exercise E below). So there is empirical (from the outside world) evidence for the dis-
tinction made in this chapter.
.
Conclusion
e lexical categories discussed in this chapter are dened in semantic, morphological,
and syntactic terms, i.e. according to meaning, word form, and position in the sen-
tence. e main factor determining the category (in English) is the position in relation
to other words. You could also try to nd a synonym and that might help you decide
on the category.
Grammatical categories can mainly be dened (as their name implies) in terms
of their grammatical function and it is oen hard to nd a synonym. Pronouns have
characteristics of both. A summary table is provided as Table 2.6, but review Tables 2.1
to 2.4 and Rules (12), (37), and (45) as well.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Table 2.6. e categories in English
Lexical N cloud, sun, love, kitchen, house
V know, see, paint, swim
Adj good, nice, friendly
Adv actually, now, there, sometimes,
where
P to, from, on, in front of
Grammatical D the, that, my, one, whose
AUX may, have, be
C and, that, because
Pronouns Pron I, yourself, who, mine, someone
e key terms in this chapter are lexical category (Noun, Verb, Adjective, Adverb,
Preposition and Pronoun) and grammatical category (Determiner, Auxiliary, Coor-
dinator and Complementizer), or open as opposed to closed.
Chapter 2. Categories 
Exercises
A. Identify each word in the short text below.
We found this place accidentally and have been returning almost weekly. We just love
it. It is so simple and yet so wonderful and the sta is great. They are always smiling and
just so nice. There is almost always a line. A must!
(review of the Fry Bread House in Phoenix, slightly adapted)
B
. Compose some sentences (a) where an adjective modies a noun,
(b) where an adverb modies an adjective, and
(c) where an adverb modies another adverb and the
two together modify a verb.
C. Find the prepositions, coordinators, and complementizers in the text below.
MUnicycling is the act of riding a one-wheel bike o road. It is also known as Rough Terrain
or All Terrain Unicycling and, in the past decade, has become the hottest trend around in
the unicycling community. O road terrain is, of course, uneven and mountains have gra-
dients, rocks and other obstacles to get in the way of the intrepid unicyclist. Whilst jump-
ing over rocks provides the fun element, cycling uphill on a unicycle demands incredible
strength and lung capacity. Additional balancing skills, including the ability to back-pedal,
are also needed to safely negotiate a downhill section on a unicycle. As a result, above
average tness levels are required. (from http://mountain-biking.suite101.com/article.cfm/
mountain_biking_on_a_unicycle)
D. To what categories do the nonsense words belong in Lewis Carroll’s Jabberwocky”,
given in Section 4 above? Which arguments did you use to decide on these?
E. Choose ve words that are grammatical categories and look them up in a dictionary.
How do dictionaries deal with them?
F
. Broca’s aphasia results in a loss of grammatical categories, such as determiners
and auxiliaries, but not in a loss of lexical categories, such as nouns and verbs. It is
sometimes called agrammatism. Wernickes aphasia results in a loss of meaning, but not
in a loss of grammatical categories. Which sentence exemplies which aphasia?
I. I could if I can help these like this you know … to make it.
II. Well front soldiers campaign soldiers to shoot well head wound
… and hospital … and so … (from O’Grady et al. 1987: 280; 278)
G. The excerpt below is from D.H. Lawrences Snake. Find the adjectives and adverbs in
the two excerpts below and see how they are used. What do they modify? Are there
compounds?
Snake
A snake came to my water-trough
On a hot, hot day, and I in pyjamas for the heat,
To drink there.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
In the deep, strange-scented shade of the great dark carob-tree
I came down the steps with my pitcher
And must wait, must stand and wait, for there he was at the trough before me.
And voices in me said, If you were a man
You would take a stick and break him now, and nish him o.
But must I confess how I liked him,
How glad I was he had come like a guest in quiet, to drink at my water-trough
And depart peaceful, pacied, and thankless,
Into the burning bowels of this earth?
H. Most people, if asked to provide or repeat the rst line of Dylan Thomas poem, partially
given below, will say ‘Do not go gently . . . with gently as an adverb modifying the verb.
Why is gentle grammatical as well?
Do Not Go Gentle into That Good Night,
Do not go gentle into that good night,
Old age should burn and rave at close of day;
Rage, rage against the dying of the light.
Though wise men at their end know dark is right,
Because their words had forked no lightning they
Do not go gentle into that good night
Good men, the last wave by, crying how bright
Their frail deeds might have danced in a green bay,
Rage, rage against the dying of the light.
Wild men who caught and sang the sun in ight,
And learn, too late, they grieved it on its way,
Do not go gentle into that good night.
Look at the cartoon in Figure 2.2. Explain why gently is used rather than gentle.
Figure 2.2. Gently into that … (© 2008 Jan Eliot. Reprinted with the permission of Universal
Press Syndicate. All rights reserved.)
Chapter 2. Categories 
I. First, circle the verbs (and auxiliary verbs) in Wallace Stevens’ Anecdote of the Jar and
then identify the categories of the other words, i.e. N, Adj, Adv, P, Det, C, and Pronoun.
Are there any words that you are unsure about? Make an educated guess as to their
category.
I placed a jar in Tennessee,
And round it was, upon a hill.
It made the slovenly wilderness
Surround that hill.
The wilderness rose up to it,
And sprawled around, no longer wild.
The jar was round upon the ground
And tall and of a port in air.
It took dominion everywhere.
The jar was grey and bare.
It did not give of bird or bush,
Like nothing else in Tennessee.
J. Identify all the words in the following sentence taken from Shakespeare. Are there any
that look unusual?
(49) Ile serve thee true and faithfully till then. (Loves Labors Lost, V, 2, 840)
K. Look at the rst 10 instances of fast in the BNC or COCA and see how many are adjectives
and how many are adverbs.
Class discussion
L. In class, it has been argued that lexical categories can be borrowed from one lan-
guage into another (e.g. karaoke, taco, sauerkraut) or ‘invented’ (e-mail, chat-room, web
navigator). Can you think of a preposition or a pronoun that has been borrowed or
made up? What does your answer mean for the status (lexical/grammatical) of these
categories?
M. Morris Bishop wrote the following in The New Yorker (27 September 1947, p. 30). How
do you like the prepositions in the last line?
I lately lost a preposition
It hid, I thought, beneath my chair
And angrily I cried, ‘perdition!
Up from out of in under there.
Correctness is my vade mecum,
And straggling phrases I abhor,
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
And yet I wondered, What should he come
Up from out of in for?”
N. Identify all the adjectives in (50), the completed (19), and discuss some aspects, e.g. the
category of capital:
(50) Relaxing in the living room of his unpretentious red stone house in an
upper-middle-class section of his capital city, Tegucigalpa, Callejas spoke
about his plans.
Keys to the exercises
A. The dierence between verb (V) and auxiliary (AUX) will only become completely clear in
Chapter 6. Note that I classify pronominal adverbs as Adv/Pro(-form). See the last part of
Section 3 on this. Are there other questions that came up?
We (Pron) found (V) this (D) place (N) accidentally (Adv) and (C) have (AUX) been
(AUX) returning (V) almost (Adv) weekly (Adv). We (Pron) just (Adv) love (V) it (Pron). It
(Pron) is (V) so (Adv) simple (Adj) and (C) yet (Adv) so (Adv) wonderful (Adj) and (C) the
(D) sta (N) is (V) great (Adj). They (Pron) are (AUX) always (Adv) smiling (V) and (C) just
(Adv) so (Adv) nice (Adj). There (Adv/Pro) is (V) almost (Adv) always (Adv) a (D) line (N).
A (D) must (AUX used as N)!
B. a. The cute kitten slept soundly; That was not pleasant;
He is this very abrasive politician.
b. He is this very abrasive linguist; That computer was extraordinarily irritating; The
extremely unpleasant judge was impeached.
c. I can see [very well] from here; He went [extremely quickly];
He said that she drove [too fast].
C. The prepositions are: of, o (unless you consider o road as a compound), as, in, (around is
an adverb), in, O, (of course is one adverb), (to in to get is an innitive marker), in, of, over,
on, including, on, As, and above. The coordinators are one or and four instances of and.
There is one complementizer, namely whilst.
D. ‘Twas brillig (Adj), and the slithy (Adj) toves (N)
Did gyre (V) and gimble (V) in the wabe (N):
All mimsy (Adj) were the borogoves (N)
And the mome (Adj) raths (N) outgrabe (V).
“Beware the Jabberwock (N), my son!
The jaws that bite, the claws that catch!
Beware the Jubjub (N) bird and shun
The frumious (Adj) Bandersnatch!”
Chapter 2. Categories 
He took his vorpal (Adj) sword in hand:
Longtime the manxome (Adj) foe he sought -
So rested he by the Tumtum (N) tree
And stood a while in thought. (…)
E. Five grammatical categories: all (D), my (D), have (AUX), though (ADV and C), since (C and
P). If you look some up in a dictionary that is historically based (e.g. the Oxford English
Dictionary), you will run into trouble because there is so much information. For instance,
the is listed as demonstrative’, pronoun, and ‘article’. A less historical dictionary might just
give ‘article’.
F. I is Wernicke; II is Broca.
G. The adjectives, adverbs, and compounds are listed below. Most of these are tricky and
probably hard to do until you have reached Chapters 5 and 10. So don’t worry!
Adjectives: hot, deep, great, dark, glad, peaceful, pacied, thankless, burning.
Adverbs: there (twice), now, how. Compounds: water-trough (twice), strange-scented,
carob-tree.
A short analysis: Lawrences Snake is about reflection and lack of action. It describes
a still, beautiful scene, which is emphasized by the use of adjectives such as hot (l. 2)
and deep, strange-scented, great, and dark (l. 4). There is also a conflict between the
peace of the moment (and nature) and the voices (of education, etc.). The conflict
is emphasized by the use of the adjectives peaceful, pacified, thankless as opposed
to burning. It is the snake that is seen as peaceful, hence, depart peaceful and not
depart peacefully.
H. Grammatically speaking, having an adverb such as gently modify go is correct but Dylan
Thomas chose gentle. Suddenly, another interpretation becomes available, one where
the person addressed in the poem should not ‘become gentle’. Now, because of its form,
gentle modies the implied you. The eect is very dierent. The Stone Soup cartoon
plays on the distinction nicely. The kids are not going gently, i.e. in a gentle manner; they
are o with BBQ tools and much energy.
I. I (Pron) placed (V) a (D) jar (N) in (P) Tennessee (N)
And (C) round (Adj) it (Pron) was (V), upon (P) a (D) hill (N)
It (Pron) made (V) the (D) slovenly (Adj) wilderness (N)
Surround (V) that (D) hill (N).
The (D) wilderness (N) rose (V) up (Adv) to (P) it (Pron)
And (C) sprawled (V) around (Adv), no (Adv) longer (Adv) wild (Adj)
The (D) jar (N) was (V) round (Adj) upon (P) the (D) ground (N)
And (C) tall (Adj) and (C) of (P) a (D) port (N) in (P) air (N).
It (Pron) took (V) dominion (N) everywhere (Pron/Adv).
The (D) jar (N) was (V) grey (Adj) and (C) bare (Adj).
It (Pron) did (AUX) not (Adv) give (V) of (P) bird (N) or (C) bush (N).
Like (P) nothing (Pron) else (Adj) in (P) Tennessee (N).
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Possible dicult words: no longer, wild, nothing, and else. The Oxford English
Dictionary labels else an adverb. I think it is an adjective since it is a synonym
of other’ and the order of adjectives and indenites is often inverted, as in
someone strange.
J. (49) Ile (Pron and AUX) serve (V) thee (Pron) true (Adv) and (C) faithfully (Adv) till (P)
then (Adv).
K. I checked the BNC and the rst 10 instances of fast that I found are listed here:
Adv: … the cold spreading too fast, of my not being able to live with it.
I have never had to run so fast in a job in my life
Following fast on its heels is Ko Samui
He turned quickly, and left the station as fast as he could.
They go fast enough as it is.
Adj: … take a fast step forwards
… progression from fast to slow to fast again
The prototype fast reactor at Dounreay
… a fast new locomotive.
… a fast enough speed of about 60 miles an hour.
Special topic: Adverb and adjective
The rule stated in (12) above is often ignored by native speakers. In its simple form, it reads:
an adjective modies a noun; an adverb modies a verb, adjective, or adverb. The reason that
the rule is not always followed is that English is changing. For instance, real is being used as
a degree adverb and is becoming more like other degree adverbs such as too, so, and very
that lack the -ly ending. In (14) to (17) above, examples of the correct’ use of adjectives and
adverbs are given. Some additional prescriptively correct uses are listed here in (51) to (55),
where the adjective modies a noun:
(51) She waited impassive while they made it in safe.
(52) I list them separate.
(53) He tested positive.
(54) In an article on nails: Color them unusual!
(55) Headline: 911 system stretched thin.
Explain what the adjectives in (51) to (55) modify. What happens if you add a -ly and
make the adjective into an adverb? The meaning changes since now the adverb modies
the verb.
Examples of ‘incorrect use’ are listed in (56), (57), and (49) above, all from an earlier
variety of English. One of the reasons for this is mentioned in Section 1.2, namely that in Old
English adverbs have no endings:
Chapter 2. Categories 
(56) Tis Noble Spoken. (Anthony and Cleopatra, II, 2, 99)
(57) Thou didst it excellent. (Taming of the Shrew, I, 1, 89)
There may be other reasons. Explain why (58) to (61) are prescriptively incorrect:
(58) In formal speech:
You did that real good.
(59) ‘because if she doesn’t do good in school, then … (Judge in Texas, quoted in a
newspaper)
(60) It looks beautifully.
(61) Does the clutch feel any dierently? (The Tappet Brothers on ‘Car Talk’)
These sentences illustrate three problems speakers encounter. First, as mentioned, really
is losing its ending when it is degree adverb, as in (58). As Swan (1980: 12) writes:
“In informal conversational English (especially American English), real is often used instead
of really before adjectives and adverbs”. Note that nobody uses real in (62). Why might that be
the case?
(62) Really, you shouldn’t have done that.
Secondly, the adverb counterpart to the adjective good is not good, as in (58) and (59), or
goodly, but well, as in (63), the rewritten version of (59). Well is also used as adjective, as in (64).
It is no wonder speakers become confused! In (64), good can replace well. Please explain why:
(64) You did that really well.
(65) I am well, thank you.
Thirdly, speakers tend to overreact when they see an adjective next to a verb and hypercorrect
themselves. Hypercorrection occurs when speakers are so unsure that they think about the
prescriptive rule too much and confuse themselves. They think that if an adjective is next to a
verb, it has to modify the verb and be an adverb, as in (60) and (61). The poem by Dylan Thomas
cited above shows, however, that this is not always necessary.
As a last point, a comment on hopefully is necessary. Swan (1980: 296–7) mentions
that there are two uses: one is ‘full of hope’, as in (65), and the other use, as in (66), “shows the
speakers attitude”, and means ‘it is hoped’. According to Swan, “[s]ome people consider the
second use ‘incorrect’. Both functions will be dealt with in Chapter 5:
(65) They waited hopefully for a positive response.
(66) Hopefully, that concert is worth going to.
It is not clear why hopefully should have attracted all this attention. There are several other
adverbs like it, e.g. those in (67) to (70):
(67) Naturally, I’d like you to stay with us for a few days.
(68) Amazingly, he arrived on time.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(69) Fortunately, the bus wasn’t late.
(70) Funnily enough, I’d been thinking about that.
In (67) to (70), the adverbs all express the speaker’s attitude and this is a legitimate use of an
adverb; they do not all have to modify the verb, although many used to in earlier varieties of
English. More on this in Chapter 5.
Chapter 3
Phrases
1. The noun phrase (NP)
2. The adjective phrase, adverb phrase, verb
phrase, and prepositional phrase
3. Phrases in the sentence
4. Coordination of phrases and apposition
5. Finding phrases and building trees
6. Conclusion
Sentences can be divided into groups of words that belong together. For instance, in the
nice unicorn ate a delicious meal, the, nice, and unicorn form one such group and a, deli-
cious, and meal form another. (We all know this intuitively). e group of words is called
a phrase. If the most important part of the phrase, i.e. the head, is an adjective, the phrase
is an Adjective Phrase; if the most important part of the phrase is a noun, the phrase is
a Noun Phrase, and so on. Indicating the phrases renders the structure of the sentence
clearer and less ambiguous, as we’ll see.
One can indicate phrases by putting brackets around them and we will occasionally
do so. However, brackets are (visually) confusing and, as an alternative, trees’ are used
with branches connecting parts of phrases. e grammatical categories Determiner
and Coordinator do not form phrases of their own but function inside a Noun Phrase
(NP), Verb Phrase (VP), Adjective Phrase (AdjP), Adverb Phrase (AdvP), or Prepositional
Phrase (PP). e grammatical category Auxiliary functions inside a Verb Group (see
Chapter 6) and the Complementizer connects one sentence to the other and is head of
a CP, as we’ll see in Chapter 7.
In Sections 1 and 2, the structure of phrases is examined. e head of a phrase is
important, but oen this is intuitively understood. e trickiest part of this chapter concerns
the intermediate categories N (N-bar) and V
(V-bar). In Section 3, the structure for a
full sentence and its phrases is discussed. Phrases are very oen coordinated by means
of and or or
and a structure for this is given in Section 4. In Section 5, more precise
rules are given on how to identify phrases and on how to construct trees.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
. The noun phrase (NP)
An NP such as the nice unicorn is built around a noun, namely, unicorn. is noun (or
N) is called the head of the NP. We can nd the head in a simple way by thinking how
wed shorten the phrase and still keep the essential part, as in a telegram. For instance,
we might shorten (1) to (2):
(1) [e nice unicorns from that planet] are visiting us regularly.
(2) Unicorns visit regularly.
More will be said on heads below.
In addition to the head, NPs can contain determiners (e.g. the) and adjectives
(nice) as well as other elements (e.g. from that planet). A tree structure for a simple NP
is given in (3). e lines, called ‘branches, indicate how the phrase is divided up, and
branches come together in ‘nodes’:
(3)
NP
D
the
Adj
nice
N
unicorn
A dierent structure for (3) looks like (4):
(4)
A structure such as (4) expresses the relationships more accurately than (3). In (3), it
is unclear whether the is more closely connected to the adjective or the noun, but from
(4), it is clear that the species nice unicorn. A structure as in (3) with more than two
branches is a at structure since the hierarchies are not clear. Using this book, you will
learn to draw structures such as (4) and to avoid trees such as (3).
ere are a number of things to note. First, the top node of (4), i.e. where the branches
come together, is an NP because the head of the phrase is an N. Shortening the NP would
tell us that. Secondly, the node in between the NP and N is called N (pronounced N-bar).
It is an intermediate node and some people call it NOM and students in my grammar
classes have called the N the small NP or the placeholder. ird, note that nice in (4) is
itself the head of an Adjective Phrase (see 1.3 as well) and we could indicate that as in (5):
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(5)
One way to go about constructing this tree is (a) to nd the head unicorn, (b) to label
the entire phrase as NP, and (c) to draw branches from the NP down to D on the le
and, if there is more than one word le, to Nʹ on the right. e Nʹ functions as place-
holder until you can put the N down. More step by step suggestions on how to draw
trees can be found in the last section.
On occasion, it may be hard to nd the head of an NP, or to identify the entire NP. For
instance, the initial group of words in (6), adapted from one of Dr. Seuss’ books, is centered
around a noun. Which noun do you think is the head and how extended is the NP?
(6) [e pleasant wocket in my pocket that I adore] loves cranberry chutney.
e right answer is that wocket is the head because if you had to shorten the sentence,
you might say the wocket loves chutney. us, pleasant and in my pocket and that I adore
add additional information. Another way to shorten the sentence is to use a pronoun,
as in (7). is is called pronominalization. If the group of words in the pleasant wocket
in my pocket that I adore can be replaced by one pronoun, it has to be a phrase:
(7) It loves cranberry chutney.
You can also nd the entire phrase by examining which parts say something about the
head, i.e. modify it. For instance, in (6), both pleasant and in my pocket have no other
function in the sentence than to modify the head wocket.
An important function of the head is to determine the agreement with the verb.
is will be more obvious in the next chapter though. I have repeated (1) as (8) with
brackets indicating that the head of the subject NP is unicorns. e singular and plural
number underneath the nouns and verb show that the head of the NP, unicorns, agrees
with the auxiliary verb are in (plural) number, not the closer noun planet:
(8)
[e nice [unicorns] from that planet] are visiting us regularly.
|||
PLURALSINGULARPLURAL
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
We could represent (6) as (9), where I have le the that I adore out for simplicity:
(9)
NP
DAdj NPDN
the pleasant wocket in my pocket
is structure indicates that the NP is composed of six words, but it does not say
whether in is more connected to my pocket or to wocket. is is again a at structure
since we dont see what goes with what. It is even worse than (3). erefore, as men-
tioned, we will avoid this kind of tree.
More hierarchical structures for this sentence are given in (10ab). To draw those
trees, you could start by grouping what goes together, e.g. the PP in my pocket, and by
circling the head. Since the head is an N, you have to put down the NP and then go to
the D on the le (and put the underneath D) and the N on the right. You need an N
because you have more than just the head wocket. en, if you put pleasant on the le,
you need another N on the right and you get (10a). If you rst want to put the PP on
the right, you need an N on the le to be a placeholder for the adjective and the noun
and the results is (10b). Well do a lot of practice with this:
(10)
b.a. NP
D
N
e
AdjP
Adj
N
pleasant
NPP
wocket
PNP
in
D N
my pocket
NP
D
N
e
AdjP
Adj
N
pleasant
N
PP
wocket
PNP
in
D N
my pocket
In (10a), in my pocket goes together with wocket. In a structure, this close connection is
expressed by having the line, i.e. ‘branch, that goes upwards connect to the same point,
i.e. ‘node. is means they are ‘sisters’ in the structure. In (10b), pleasant and wocket are
put closer together, i.e. are sisters. Both structures are possible. e meaning dierence
between (10a) and (10b) is minimal, but this is not always the case as sentences such
as (20) and (22) below show. Note again that pleasant is itself the head of a phrase and
that I indicate that by means of and Adj head inside an AdjP. e ultimate tree is not
as important as understanding why you represent a tree in a particular way, as I have
just tried to do for (10).
Chapter 3. Phrases 
In (10), I am representing my pocket as an NP because it has a D and an N head. If
the NP had been a pronoun or a name, the structure would have been as in (11). Pro-
nouns and names such as Jennifer, Edward, Malacandra cannot have other elements
modify/specify them and therefore we will see them as full phrases, as in (11ab):
(11)
a. NP b. NP
she Edward
Under very special circumstances, proper names can be modied, as when there are
many persons called Edward and you want to make sure it is the nice Edward. is is
not common with names, and it is very uncommon with pronouns.
Some heads are trickier to identify than others. For instance, in one of those pages,
the head is one, and in a piece of paper, piece is the head. Frequently, a Relative Clause,
such as who wore that ugly hat in (12) is part of an NP, as shown by brackets, modifying
the head person:
(12) [e person [who wore that ugly hat]] is the queen.
A structure for (12) will be given in Chapter 10. For now, just understand that it is part
of the NP.
Structures such as (10) are called trees. As mentioned, the lines connecting parts
of the trees are called branches, and the points where the branches come together are
called nodes. e nodes are usually labelled, e.g. N, N, or NP. Remember that N is an
intermediate node between the top NP and the N. Such intermediate nodes allow one
to indicate which elements are grouped together and thus make trees less at. ey are
placeholders for a group of words that go together.
.
The adjective phrase, adverb phrase, verb phrase,
and prepositional phrase
. The adjective phrase (AdjP) and adverb phrase (AdvP)
AdjPs are built around adjectives, which indicate properties of nouns; AdvPs are
built around adverbs which indicate qualities of verbs, adverbs, and adjectives. Since
adjectives and adverbs have this qualifying function, they themselves are (optionally)
accompanied by a degree marker such as very, too, extremely, really. e latter are
adverbs of a special kind: they always modify another adverb or adjective and never
modify a verb. ey are comparable to the determiner in the NP, and more like gram-
matical than lexical categories. ey do not expand into an AdvP of their own since
degree markers such as extremely very do not occur.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
An example of an AdjP is given in (13a) and of an AdvP in (13b). e (D)Adv
indicates a degree adverb but, from now on, just Adv will be used:
(13)
a. AdjPb. AdvP
(D)Adv Adj (D)Adv Adv
so nice very quickl
y
In (13a), the head of the AdjP is the adjective nice, and this head is modied by a
degree adverb so; in (13b), the adverb quickly expands into a phrase and is modied by
the degree adverb very that does not form a phrase of its own. ats why I choose not
to make very the head of an AdvP.
An AdjP can be pronominalized, as in (14), but pronominalizing an AdvP, as in
(15), sounds slightly awkward:
(14) I was happy and so was she.
(15) He behaved nicely, and she behaved so/thus.
. The verb phrase (VP)
A VP is built around a verb and the latter can be in the present or past tense (they are
past in (16abc)). Some VPs include other obligatory material, i.e. words or phrases
that cannot easily be le out, such as the NP in (16a), the PP in (16b), and the AdjP
in (16c). ese obligatory parts are called complements and will be discussed in the
next chapters:
(16)
a. b. c.
VP
VNP
wrote
DN
the letter
VP
VPP
was
PNP
in
DN
the garden
VP
V AdjP
was
Adv Adj
very quiet
e VP can also include optional material that explains when, where, why, and how
the action or state that the verb describes took place. ese optional elements function
as adverbials and will be discussed in Chapter 5.
As in the case of the NP, a VP can be pronominalized. An example is given in (17),
where the (bolded) VP washed the dishes is replaced by do so. Some linguists call these
pro-VPs or pro-forms, since they do not stand for nouns. It is up to you whether you
call them pronoun or pro-form:
(17) Gijsbert washed the dishes and Mariken did so as well.
Chapter 3. Phrases 
. The prepositional phrase (PP)
A PP is built around a preposition. As mentioned in the previous chapter, prepositions
indicate relations in space and time. PPs include a P and an NP, as in (18):
(18)
PP
PNP
on
DN
the Moon
PPs can be replaced (pronominalized) by the adverbs then, when, how, there, etc.
In this section, it is necessary to jump ahead to Chapters 4, 5, 9 and 10 where
functions are discussed. Up to now, we have looked at the names of categories and
phrases, e.g. N and NP. Depending on where phrases are situated in the tree, they play
a particular function, such as subject and object. Functions will not be put in the tree
structure because it should be clear from the tree what they are.
With respect to PPs, it is not always easy to determine what role they play and
their function in a sentence is manifold. For instance, in the ambiguous (19), an oen
used sentence in linguistic circles, does the PP function inside the NP, or are the NP
and PP independent of one another?
(19) She saw the man with glasses.
e answer to both questions can be ‘yesbecause the sentence is ambiguous. In the
one case, the PP with glasses modies the man and functions inside the NP the man
with glasses; in the other case, the PP is independent of the NP since it modies the VP
and species how the seeing was done. e structure for the former reading is as in
(20a) and for the latter reading as in (20b):
(20)
a. b. VP
V PP
VNP
PNP
saw
with glasses
DN
the man
VP
VNP
saw
DN
the
NPP
man
PNP
with
N
glasses
us, a particular tree structure disambiguates the sentence. In (17a), the PP with
glasses is right next to the N man (i.e. PP is sister to N) and therefore modies man; in
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(17b), the same PP is right next to the V saw the man (i.e. PP is sister to V) and hence
says something on how the seeing of the man is done. For now, don’t worry about
(17b) too much. You may have noticed the use of V (pronounced V-bar) in (17b). A
V (like the N in an NP) is an intermediate category in the VP. In (17b), we need to
group the V and NP together so we need a label for that and we use a small VP’ or
placeholder until we can put down a branch for the V.
Groucho Marx uses structural ambiguity a lot, as in (21) below. Consider how the
PP in my pajamas in (21) is ambiguous, in at least two ways:
(21) I once shot an elephant in my pajamas. How he got in my pajamas I’ll never know.
When you read the rst line you think that the speaker was wearing pajamas, but the
second line makes that impossible and you have to rethink the sentence. Now, there
are two interpretations le: (a) the elephant was wearing the pajamas of the speaker
while being shot and (b) the elephant was shot inside the pajamas. I have represented
the two interpretations in (22). In (22a), the elephant is wearing the pajamas and, in
(22b), the elephant is shot in the pajamas:
(22)
a. b.
VP
V PP
VNPP NP
shot in
DNDN
an elephant my pajamas
VP
VNP
shot
DN
an
NPP
elephant
PNP
in
DN
my pajamas
As explained in Chapter 1, structural ambiguity is dierent from lexical ambiguity. With
lexical ambiguity, a word has two meanings, e.g. case in (9) in Chapter 1. Another instance
is (23), a well-known joke also attributed to Groucho Marx, where the preposition outside
is lexically ambiguous. Outside and inside look like each other’s opposites in expressing a
location, but in fact outside has an additional meaning, namely ‘in addition to; except for’:
(23) Outside of a dog, a book is a mans best friend; inside its too hard to read.
. Phrases in the sentence
Having provided a tree structure for all of the phrases whose heads are lexical catego-
ries, I will now show how to combine these into a sentence. e basic structure for a
sentence, i.e. S, is shown in (24):
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(24)
a. b.
S
NP VP
He
V
le
S
NP VP
DNVNP
e emu loved
DN
the kangaroo
Thus, the initial element in the sentence is generally an NP (and, as well see in
the next chapter, the function of this NP is subject). The NP is a daughter of the
sentence S (i.e. immediately below S and connected by a tree branch). e rest of the
sentence is the VP which can be more complex (as seen in (22b) above), as can the NP
(as seen in (10)).
e relationships that are relevant in a tree are sister and daughter/mother. In
(24ab), the NP and VP are sisters to each other and daughters of S. Sisters have a close
relationship. us, in (20a), the relationship between the V saw and the NP the man
with glasses is a direct one since they are sisters, but the relationship between glasses
and the V saw is an indirect one.
As well see in the next chapter, each phrase has a function to play in the sentence.
ese functions can be read o the tree. For instance, in (24ab), the NP is the subject
and the VP is the predicate; in (16a), the verb wrote is the head of the VP and its sister,
the NP the letter, is the object.
.
Coordination of phrases and apposition
Phrases and categories can be coordinated, as long as they are the same kind. For
instance, two NPs are coordinated in (24), two prepositions in (26), and two VPs
in (27):
(25) We see scorpions all the time in [the house] and [the garden].
(26) e dog went [under] and [over] the fence.
(27) I [read books] and [listened to music].
When the elements that are coordinated are not the same, e.g. an NP and a PP in (28),
the sentence becomes ungrammatical:
(28) *I read [a book] and [to Janet].
Coordination can be used to recognize phrases and categories. If you know one phrase
or category, then the other phrase or category will be the same.
e structure for coordinate constructions is controversial. A number of linguists
argue that the relationship between the coordinated phrases in, for instance, (29) is
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
completely equal and hence that a structure as in (30a) is appropriate. Others claim
that the rst phrase is somewhat more important and use (30b). Note that I have not
labelled the node above and in (30b) since its name is controversial:
(29) Books and magazines sell easily.
(30)
a. b.
N
NP
NP
NC NP
books and
magazines
NP
NP CNP
N and N
books magazines
Arguments can be found for either structure. For ease of representation, I’ll use (30a),
but feel free to use (30b). We can usually switch around the NPs in (29) and this seems
to be an argument in favor of (30a) since both NPs have the same status. However,
when we move part of the books and magazines, for instance, because the second NP
is a very long one, as in (31), and remains with the second NP and this speaks in favor
of (30b) since and magazines discussing political issues is a unit (indicated by brackets
in (31)), but books and is not:
(31) I read books yesterday [and magazines discussing political issues].
In Chapter 7, well look at coordinating sentences. e basic question about which
structure to pick is relevant there too. Two-part coordinators were mentioned in the
previous chapter, e.g. both Mary and John; I will not provide a structure for these.
Apposition diers from coordination. It occurs when two NPs are used side by
side but with the same reference, as in (32) to (34). In (32), the added information
is not crucial for the meaning of the sentence since many of us know who Napoleon
was, and I could have chosen another way to describe him. If we saw or heard (33)
and (34), however, without a context, we wouldnt know who was meant by ‘we’ or
my friend’:
(32) Napoleon Bonaparte, the past Emperor of France, went to war against most
of Europe.
(33) We the people of the United States, …, do ordain and establish this Constitution for
the United States of America. (from the Preamble to the US Constitution)
(34) My friend Bill sent a letter.
In appositives, the second NP can replace the rst, or could be rephrased by a relative
clause, as in (35). e structure could be as in (36), close to that of a relative clause:
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(35) We, who are the people of the United States, ….
(36)
NP
NP NP
We
D
N
the
NPP
people
of the United States
. Finding phrases and building trees
. Finding the phrase
A phrase is a group of words forming a unit and united around a head, e.g. a noun or a
verb. Since phrases are syntactic units, a number of rules apply to them. Some of these
have been discussed above, namely pronominalization and coordination. ree addi-
tional ones can be used, namely deletion, replacement by a wh-element, and movement.
ese ve rules are listed in Table 3.1. If I have a hunch that, for instance, to the store in
(37) is a phrase, how do I prove that? Let’s apply the rules and see:
(37) She ran to the store.
I can pronominalize to the store as there, coordinate it, delete it, replace it, and move
it, as shown in the Table.
Table 3.1. Finding a phrase
Rules Examples
a.
A phrase can be pronominalized: She ran [there].
b. It can be coordinated with a phrase
of the same kind:
She ran [to the bookstore] and
[to the library];
c. It can be deleted: She ran […].
d. It can be replaced by a wh-element: [Where] did she run?
e. It can be moved: [To the store] she ran.
e ve criteria in Table 3.1 conrm that to the store in (37) is a phrase. We know that
it is a PP because a Preposition, namely to, is the head.
All phrases can be pronominalized and coordinated. However, not all phrases can
be deleted. e initial NP is very important, and in English, sentences are ungrammat-
ical without it. us, changing (37) into (38) produces an ungrammatical sentence:
(38) *Ran to the store.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
In Chapters 4 and 5, we will discuss what kinds of phrases can be deleted and what
kinds cannot.
.
Building trees
We can build trees from top to bottom or from bottom to top. Experiment with this a
little to see what you personally like best. Lets do the phrase the boy with the red hat using
both ways. Either way, we rst need to decide what the head is. Well argue the head is
boy (e.g. because we pronominalize the phrase with he not it). is means the phrase is
an NP.
Starting from the top, lets put down the NP rst. If there is a determiner, the rst
branch to the le will always be a D, so D is the daughter of NP:
(39)
NP
D
the
Now, we have to be careful not to make the next branch go to N because then there
won’t be space for both the N boy and the PP with the red hat. Instead, well put
down an N which can be expanded. Remember that N is a placeholder for more
than one branch:
(40)
NP
D
N
the
Now, the branches coming down from Nʹ need to be put in, as in (41):
(41)
NP
D
N
the
NPP
boy
Aer this, nishing the tree is easy. Well draw branches from PP to P and NP, and then
have to start all over again with the NP. Aer the NP is nished, we make sure that we
have put all the words under the categories. e result will be as in (42):
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(42)
NP
D
N
the
N
PP
boy
PNP
with
DN
the
AdjP N
Adj
red
hat
Sometimes you will see the tree in (42) drawn as in (43). is makes it easier to see the
actual sentence. You have to wait until the end to put the words in. Either tree is ne,
though I usually use (42):
(43)
NP
D
N
e
N
PP
boy
PNP
with
DN
the
AdjP N
Adj
red hat
When a phrase is not a branching phrase, e.g. the AdjP red in (42) and (43), we still
indicate that it can have a head (and other elements) by having it go to the Adj. To save
space, I sometimes leave out the branches, as in (44):
(44) AdjP
Adj
red
It is also possible to draw a tree starting from the bottom. In this case, it is handiest to
put the category of each word on top of it, as in (45a). en, we need to nd what goes
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
with what. In this case, red and hat combine, so we’ll draw branches to connect them, as
in (45b):
(45)
a. DNPDAdj N
theboy with thered hat
b.
DNPDAdj N
theboy with thered hat
en, D has to be combined with it and then P, with the result of (46):
(46)
PP
NP
DNPDAdj N
the boy with the red hat
Now, the PP has to be connected to boy since it modies it, and then D has to be con-
nected. e result will be the same as that in (42) and (43). Well need to make sure all
nodes have labels, e.g. we need to put in the N. e disadvantage of this tree is that
the branches are not of equal length and that this becomes confusing in seeing what
goes together.
A few more trees to look at are given in (47) and (48). In (47), the PP near the park
shows where the tree was planted because it is the sister to the V planted an exotic tree. In
(48), the PP without leaves is sister to the N trees, so it is the tree that is without leaves.
(47)
S
NP VP
ey
PP
PNP
near
DN
the park
V
VNP
planted
DN
an
AdjPN
Adj tree
exotic
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(48)
S
NP VP
ey
VNP
planted
NPP
trees
PNP
without N
leaves
Notice that I am indicating that leaves is the head of the NP but that it could be modied
by an adjective; the pronoun they cannot.
Finally, one can use a computer to draw trees either by purchasing a tree-drawing
font or by just using the line-drawing feature thats part of most word processing pro-
grams. In the latter case, use tabs to space out the nodes and then draw the branches
aer the nodes are in. Experiment a little with this.
.
Conclusion
In this chapter, phrases and their tree structure are introduced. A lexical category such
as a noun typically has other elements around it that go with it (i.e. modify it). is
group of words and the head form a phrase. All lexical categories (N, V, Adj, Adv, and
P) head phrases and each of these phrases is discussed.
Phrases are combined into sentences (or S), as in (24) above. A sentence includes
an NP and a VP. In the next chapter, we discuss the functions of the NP and VP in the
sentence. A structure for phrases that are coordinated and appositives is also given
in (30).
e key terms in this chapter are phrases (NP, VP, AdjP, AdvP and PP); S; flat as
opposed to non-flat/hierarchical structures; ambiguity; pronominalization; coordi-
nation and apposition. e most important thing is to learn to draw trees.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Exercises
A. Draw the tree structures for (49) and (50):
(49) They saw the lights.
(50) The rabbit planted carrots.
B. What do trees express?
C. First identify the phrases in (51) to (53) by putting brackets around them. Then try to
draw trees for (51) and (53) but not for (52) (we get to that sentence in the next chapter):
(51) Dumbledore submits his tax-return on time every year.
(52) Kim’s painting made Voldemort extremely unhappy.
(53) Hagrid remained a lover of dragons during his life.
D. Draw brackets around all the NPs in the rst paragraph of the text below. Note that
names, pronouns, and nouns on their own also need to be marked as noun phrases.
Man rescued after 4 months at sea
A U.S. Navy frigate rescued a man off the coast of Costa Rica last week. The man says
his crippled sailboat was adrift at sea for almost four months. The 24-foot sailboat
appeared battered and broken, and the navy spokesperson said his crew was
stunned when Van Pham appeared and waved at the frigate.
Van Pham told crew members of the Navy frigate that he had set out for a brief trip
between Long Beach and Catalina Island when high winds broke his mast. His radio, he
said, failed to work and he found himself adrift. Van Pham survived because he ate the
sh that he caught in the water around him, as well as a few seagulls. He drank rainwater
collected in a bucket and he appeared to be in generally good health.
E.
Draw brackets around all the PPs in the second paragraph of the text in D.
F. Sentence (54) has a fairly complex initial NP. Try to draw the tree rst for this NP and then
t it into an S:
(54) The man with the monstrously ugly umbrella left the house.
G. Sentence (55) is ambiguous. Explain which of the two trees expresses that the Martian
has the telescope?
(55) She spotted the Martian with a telescope.
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(a)
OR: (b)
S
NP VP
She
V
VNP
spotted
DN
the Martian
PP
PNP
with
DN
atelescope
S
NP VP
She
VNP
spotted
DN
the
N PP
Martian
PNP
with
DN
atelescope
H. Draw a tree for (56). Is (56) ambiguous? If so, explain how:
(56) They like a house with a porch with rocking chairs.
I. Put brackets around all the phrases in (57) and (58). How would you draw these
sentences as trees?
(57) Tom and Jerry make very good ice cream.
(58) They washed dishes and cleaned the sink.
J. Give some reasons justifying your choice of some of the phrases in (51).
Class discussion
K. In Chapter 1, Section 1.2, two instances of structural ambiguity are given. The headlines
in (59) to (61) are likewise structurally ambiguous. Can you explain their ambiguity using
brackets indicating the structure?
(59) Complaints about NBA referees growing ugly.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(60) Enraged cow injures farmer with ax.
(61) Two sisters reunited after 18 years at check-out counter.
L. Discuss the structure of one of these, a piece of chalk, and all those arguments.
M.
Draw brackets around the phrases for the intended meaning of throw it away in a waste
basket’ and around those for the meaning PJ heard in Figure 3.1.
Figure 3.1. From inside or into? (family circus 2008 Bil Keane, Inc. king features
Syndicate)
Keys to the exercises
A.
S
NP
(49)
VP
They
VNP
saw
DN
the lights
S
NP VP
D
(50)
NV NP
The rabbits planted N
carrots
B. Trees indicate what goes with what, e.g. which phrases modify which head.
C. In (51), [Dumbledore] is an NP and [submits his tax-return on time every year] is a VP;
[his tax-return] is an NP; [on time] is a PP and [every year] is an NP. All put together,
it looks as follows: [[Dumbledore] [submits [his tax-return] [on time] [every year]]]. In
Chapter 3. Phrases 
(52), [Kim’s painting] is an NP and [made Voldemort extremely unhappy] is a VP; [Kim]
is another NP; [Voldemort] an NP; and [extremely unhappy] an AdjP. With brackets, it
looks like: [[Kim’s painting] [made [Voldemort] [extremely unhappy]]]. In the next
chapter, we will see a structure with an added bracket. In (53), [Hagrid] is an NP;
[remained a lover of dragons during his life] is a VP; [a lover of dragons] an NP; [of
dragons] a PP; [dragons] an NP; and [during his life] a PP. With brackets: [[Hagrid]
[remained [a lover [of [dragons]]] [during his life]]]
(51)
every year
on
N
S
NP VP
Dumbledore
V NP
V PP
DN
V
NP P
NP
submits
D Ntime
his tax-return
(53)
S
NP VP
Hagrid
PP
PNP
during
DN
his life
V
V NP
remained
D
N
a
NPP
lover
PNP
of N
dragons
D. The NPs: [Man] rescued after [4 months] at [sea]
[A U.S. Navy frigate] rescued [a man] o [the coast of [Costa Rica]] [last week]. [The man]
says [his crippled sailboat] was adrift at [sea] for almost [four months]. [The 24-foot
sailboat] appeared battered and broken, and [the navy spokesperson] said [his crew] was
stunned when [Van Pham] appeared and waved at [the frigate].
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
E. The PPs: Van Pham told crew members [of the Navy frigate] that he had set out [for a
brief trip [between Long Beach and Catalina Island]] when high winds broke his mast.
His radio, he said, failed to work and he found himself adrift. Van Pham survived because
he ate the sh that he caught [in the water [around him]], as well as a few seagulls. He
drank rainwater collected [in a bucket] and he appeared to be [in generally good health].
F. (54)
S
umbrella
NP
D
N
The
N PP
man
PNP
with
DN
the
AdjP N
Adv Adj
monstrously ugly
left
thehouse
VP
VNP
D N
G. In (a), the Martian has a telescope; in (b), the ‘she uses the telescope to spot the Martian.
We can gure this out from looking at who the PP is sister to in the tree.
H. If the structure for (56) is the one drawn in (a), they like a house with both a porch and
with rocking chairs and the rocking chairs can be anywhere in the house. If it is drawn as
in (b), they like a house with a porch that has rocking chairs and the rocking chairs have
to be on the porch:
a. S
NP VP
They
VNP
like
D
N
a
N
NPP
house
P NP
with
DN
a porch
PP
PNP
with
AdjP N
Adj chairs
rocking
Chapter 3. Phrases 
b. S
NP VP
They
VNP
like
DN
a
NPP
house
PNP
with
DN
a
NPP
porch
PNP
with
AdjPN
Adj chairs
rocking
I. Sentences (57) and (58) can be drawn with brackets as follows:
(57) [[Tom] and [Jerry]] [make [[very good] ice cream]].
(58) [They] [[washed [dishes]] and [cleaned [the sink]]].
As trees, they can be drawn as follows:
(57)
S
VP
VNP
make
AdjP
N
Adv Adj
ice cream
very good
NP
NP and NP
T
om Jerry
(58)
S
NP VP
T
hey
and
VP
VNP
washed N
dishes
VP
VNP
cleaned
DN
thesin
k
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
J. In (51), Dumbledore is an NP because it can be pronominalized, i.e. replaced by he; it can
be coordinated with another NP as in Dumbledore and his accountant submit the forms on
time. It cannot be deleted or moved because it is the subject as we will see in Chapter 4. It
can be questioned as in Who submits the forms on time?
Submits his tax-return on time every year is a VP since it can be pronominalized, i.e. replaced
by do so as in Dumbledore submits his tax-return on time every year and Hagrid does so too.
It can also be coordinated with another VP as in Dumbledore submits his tax-return on time
every year but neglects to renew his accident insurance.
His tax-return is an NP since it can be replaced by it, can be coordinated as in Dumledore
submits his tax-return and insurance claims on time every year, and moved as in It is his
tax-return that Dumbledore submits on time every year. And so on…
Special topic: Negative concord
Unlike most of the special topics, Negative Concord does not supplement the material covered
in the above chapter, but is an often debated issue. The prescriptive rule on multiple negation is
as follows:
(62) Two negatives in one sentence make the sentence positive.
Swan (1980: 182) says “[i]n standard English, nobody, nothing, never etc are themselves enough
to make the sentence negative, and not is unnecessary.
We use certain types of multiple negatives in our utterances all the time, e.g. in (63) and
(64b). In (63), the sentence expresses negation since the no is outside the clause and independent
of the I don’t want to go. In this sentence, the negatives do not cancel each other out, since the
negatives are independent of each other, and the sentence is prescriptively correct. In (64b),
an answer to (64a), nothing is negated and the sentence could be paraphrased as (65). In this
sentence, the negatives cancel each other out:
(63) No, I don’t want to go.
(64) a: I paid nothing for that.
b: Five dollars is not nothing.
(65) Five dollars is quite something.
Since (63) and (64) follow the rule, they are not objected to by prescriptive grammarians.
A sentence such as (66), however, is said to be incorrect if it means the same as (67) or (68):
(66) They don’t have no problems.
(67) They don’t have problems.
(68) They don’t have any problems.
Chapter 3. Phrases 
Although two negatives are supposed to make a positive, most speakers understand (66) as
a negative. This construction is referred to as Negative Concord, i.e. the two negatives work
together to emphasize the negation rather than cancel each other out. Sentences such as (66)
occur very frequently in spoken, informal English. However, the prescriptive rule is so well
known that people often deliberately break it for impact. That may have been the reason a
double negative is used in (69), a protest sign at the time that a student code of conduct was
being considered at the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor:
(69) WE DON’T
NEED NO
STINKIN
CODE
Figure 3.2. Multiple Negation
Changes involving negatives follow a certain path, sometimes called the negative cycle. Thus,
in Old English, there is a negative no or ne, as in (70). At some point, the negative weakens and
contracts with the verb. Thats the reason another negative is added in (71):
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(70) Men ne cunnon secgan to soðe ... hwa
Man not could tell to truth ... who
‘No man can tell for certain ... who’. (Beowulf 50–52)
(71) Næron
Š
e noht æmetti
Š
e, ðeah ge wel ne dyden
not-were you not unoccupied. though you well not did
‘You were not unoccupied, though you did not do well’. (Pastoral Care, 206).
At some point the noht/not itself weakens and wed expect another negative. The prescriptive
pressure of (62) stops this from happening, certainly in writing. There is the use of never, as in
(72), however, as you can nd when you google the sequence:
(72) I never see him much these days. (meaning ‘I don’t see him often these days’)
Review of Chapters 1–3
e rst chapter shows that we know quite a bit about language intuitively without
formal training and the second and third chapters make some of this knowledge more
explicit. Chapter 1 examines two types of ambiguity, lexical and structural ambiguity,
and the latter in particular has been helpful in showing the necessity for dierent tree
structures in Chapter 3. Chapter 1 also explains prescriptive rules, examples of which
are given in the special topics at the end of each chapter.
Chapter 2 lists the lexical (N, V, A, and P) and grammatical (D, AUX, C) catego-
ries we make use of in English. Tables provide the characteristics and examples of
many of these. Several rules are also formulated to distinguish adjectives and adverbs
in (12), determiners and adjectives in (37), and prepositions, complementizers and
adverbs in (45). Pronouns are complicated because so many kinds exist. Personal pro-
nouns function like entire phrases but have very little meaning. Other pronouns, e.g.
possessives, are determiners.
Chapter 3 shows how sentences can be divided into phrases, each of which is cen-
tered around a noun, verb, adjective, adverb, or preposition. e NP and VP are the
most complex since they can include an N or V. ese are intermediate categories,
not quite full phrases (they lack something) but bigger than heads. Chapter 3 also sug-
gests the S to represent the entire sentence and gives trees for coordinated phrases.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Exercises relevant to these chapters:
A. List the lexical and grammatical categories in (1) and (2). Draw a tree for (1):
(1) The tortoise from Jupiter ate his food.
(2) Do not go gentle into that good night,
Old age should burn and rave at close of day; …
B. List the categories in (3), both lexical and grammatical. Give two reasons why painted
is a verb:
(3) Zoya painted the chairs in the rain.
C. Provide the tree for (3). The intended meaning for (3) is unambiguous: Zoya painted
actual chairs while it was raining and she was out in it’.
D.
Draw trees for the phrases in (4) and (5) and for the sentences in (6) and (7):
(4) That sensitive poet from Shiraz
(5) noticed a hopeless sadness
(6) Vincent and his brother wrote many letters.
(7) We suggested those solutions quickly.
E. Draw a tree for (8):
(8) The trees in the park are unhappy.
F. How is the following sentence ambiguous?
(9) Outside of a dog, a book is a mans best friend. Inside a dog, it is too dark to read.
G. Is there anything prescriptively incorrect in (10) and (11)? If so, say what?
(10) That seemed logical enough.
(11) ne durste þer na man speken; leste þe king hit wolde awreken
not dared there no man speak lest the king it wanted to-avenge
‘No one there dared to speak in case the king wanted to punish them.
(Layamon, Brut 12425)
Class discussion
H. Please comment on ‘You look real nice. When would you say this; when might you say
something else.
I.
Explain the dierence between linguistic and non-linguistic knowledge.
Review of Chapters 1–3 
J. Briey discuss the poem We Real Cool’ by Gwendolyn Brooks in the light of either
chapter 1 or 2 or both:
We real cool. We
Left school. We
Lurk Late. We
Strike straight. We
Sing sin. We
Thin gin. We
Jazz June. We
Die soon.
Keys to the exercises
A. (1) The (D) tortoise (N) from (P) Jupiter (N) ate (V) his (N) food (N)
(2) Do (AUX) not (Adv) go (V) gentle (Adj) into (P) that (D) good (Adj) night (N), Old
(Adj) age (N) should (AUX) burn (V) and (C) rave (V) at (P) close (N) of (P) day (N)
(1)
S
VP
VNP
ate
DN
hisfood
NP
D
N
The
NPP
tortoise
PNP
from Jupiter
B. Zoya: N C. (3)
S
NP VP
Zoya
V
PP
VNPP NP
painted in
DNDN
the chairs the rain
painted: V
the: D
chairs: N
in: P
the: D
rain: N
Painted is a verb because it shows past tense (morphological) and indicates an action (semantic).
Sentence (3) is not ambiguous since in the rain is independent of chairs; it says something about
where you painted them.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
D.
(4) (5)NP
DN
That
AdjP
N
Adj
sensitive NPP
poet
PNP
from Shiraz
VP
VNP
noticed
DN
a
AdjPN
Adj sadness
hopeless
(6)
S
NP VP
NP CNPV
Vincent and wrote
DN
his brother
NP
DN
many letters
(7)
S
NP VP
We
V AdvP
Adv
VNPquickly
suggested
DN
thosesolutions
E. (8)
S
NP
D
N
The
NPP
trees
PNP
in
DN
the park
VP
V AdjP
are Adj
unhappy
F. See the end of Section 2 (Chapter 3).
G. Sentence (10) is correct because logical is used as an adjective here. The multiple
negation in (11) would not be correct in Modern English, but was in the 13th century.
Review of Chapters 1–3 
Example of an exam/quiz covering Chapters 1 to 3
A. Why is (a) prescriptively ungrammatical? What would you say in a formal situation
instead of (a)?
(a) I would like to rst make the point that bureaucrats don’t think that way.
B. Please list the category (N, V, etc) of each word in (b):
(b) The coyote evolved in North America alongside the wolf and expanded its range
since they readily reproduce in metropolitan areas.
C. Give two reasons why evolved in (b) is a verb.
D.
Draw tree structures (as in Chapter 3) for the phrases in (c) and (d):
(c) That careful driver in his Volvo
(d) heard a strange sound.
E. Please draw tree structures (again as in Chapter 3) for the sentences in (e) and (f):
(e) Zoltan and Zoya read three books in a week.
(f) We rarely suggest useful solutions.
Keys to the exam/quiz
A. Split innitive (and contraction). Alternative: … rst like to make …(do not) …
B. The (D) coyote (N) evolved (V) in (P) North (Adj) America (N) alongside (P) the (D) wolf (N)
and (C) expanded (V) its (D) range (N) since (C) they (Pron) readily (Adv) reproduce (V) in
(P) metropolitan (Adj) areas (N).
C. Evolved has an -ed ending and could be preceded by an auxiliary, e.g. has.
D.
NP
DN
That
AdjP
N
Adj
careful
NPP
driver
PNP
in
DN
his Volvo
(d)(c)
VP
VNP
heard
DN
a
AdjPN
Adj sound
strange
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
E.
(e)
S
NP VP
NP CNP
Zoltan andZoya
PP
PNP
in
DN
aweek
V
VNP
read
DN
three books
(f)
S
NP VP
We
AdvP
V
Adv
rarely VNP
suggest
AdjP N
Adjsolutions
useful
Chapter 4
Functions in the sentence
1. Subject and predicate
2. Complements
3. Verbs and functions
4. Trees for all verb types
5. Light verbs (optional)
6. Conclusion
In Chapter 3, groups of words that go together were called phrases and labelled as
NP, VP, AdjP, AdvP, and PP depending on what headed them. Phrases (and pronouns
since they replace phrases) have functions in the sentence, e.g. subject, direct object,
indirect object, and subject and object predicate. e name, label, or realization (e.g.
NP) and the function itself (e.g. subject) should be kept separate.
As mentioned, we will not be putting functions in the tree structures since (most
of) the functions follow from the tree structure. Certain functions such as subject and
direct object occupy specic positions in the tree (daughter of S and sister of V respec-
tively), and to label them would be redundant.
e four basic functions are subject, predicate, complement, and adverbial (see
next chapter for adverbials). As explained in Section 1, a subject and predicate are
needed in every sentence. Most verbs need complements as well, as Section 2 dis-
cusses. Complements come in dierent varieties; the ones dealt with in this chapter
are direct object, indirect object, subject predicate, and object predicate. Some people
equate object and complement, but technically complement is a broader category than
object. In Section 3, verbs will be named depending on the type of complement they
appear with. Section 4 provides trees for the dierent verbs, and Section 5 explores
one additional verb type, the light verb.
.
Subject and predicate
Every complete sentence has a subject and a predicate. e subject is usually realized
by an NP (sometimes by a clause, see Chapter 7), and the predicate is always realized
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
by a VP. In (1), the moon is the subject and has risen in the sky is the predicate. e
predicate says something about the subject:
(1) [e moon] [has risen in the sky].
Other examples of subjects and predicates are given in (2). Note that subjects can be
more than one or two words, as their long-term survival in Florida shows!
(2) [Manatees] [are large, marine mammals]. [ey] [can live up to 60 years and can
weigh up to 1200 pounds]. [eir long-term survival in Florida] [is uncertain,
however].
Typically, subjects start o a sentence, as in (1) and (2), but there are a number of
constructions where they dont. For instance, in (3), the Adverb Phrase fortunately for
us precedes the subject; in questions such as (4), the auxiliary verb does; and in (5),
the sentence is a complex one and there are multiple subjects. (Well go into complex
sentences in more detail in Chapters 7, 8, and 10).
(3) Fortunately for us, [she] managed to join the government.
(4) Do [those people] like anything?
(5) [He] made no answer, and [they] were again silent till [they] had gone down the
dance, when [he] asked her if [she and her sisters] did not very oen walk to Meryton.
(Jane Austen, Pride & Prejudice, Chapter 18)
Since the subject is not always the initial word or phrase of the sentence, we need other
ways to determine the subject. Table 4.1 lists three diagnostics for determining what
the subject is.
Table 4.1. Subject tests (subject is in italics; verb is in bold)
a. Inversion with the AUX in Yes/No questions
e pig from Malacandra will want to eat soon.
Will the pig from Malacandra want to eat soon?
b. Agreement with the Verb/AUX
e prigg is nice.
e priggs are nice.
c. Tag questions
e hross is nice, isn’t he?
Let’s apply these tests to (1). e rst test of Table 4.1 shows that the moon is the
subject since it can change places with has, as in the question (6):
(6) Has [the moon] just risen in the sky?
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
e second test involves subject verb agreement. We discussed this rule in Chapter 3
because it is helpful in nding the head of an NP. e NP that determines agreement
on the verb is the subject. In English, this marking is fairly limited. In (7) and (8), some
instances of subject verb agreement are marked, which most of you know already. In
English, the verb be shows the most inection, as seen in (7), but most other verbs just
show the singular third person, as in (8b), and leave the other subjects unmarked, as
in (8a):
(7) a. I am happy. (rst person singular subject I with rst person
singular am)
b. You are happy. (second person subject you with second person are)
c. He/Matthew is happy. (third person singular subjectwith third person
singular is).
d. We/they are happy. (plural subject with third person plural are).
(8) a. I/you/we/they walk regularly. (unmarked walk).
b. She/Emma walks regularly. (third person singular subject with third person
singular verb)
us, to nd the subject in (1), we could change its number (singular to plural or plu-
ral to singular) and see if that changes the form of the verb as well. In (1), the subject
the moon is singular and, if we pluralize it to the moons, as in (9), the verb becomes
plural as well (i.e. loses the third person singular ending):
(9) e moons have just risen in the sky.
is shows that the subject in (1) is indeed the moon (and in (9), it is the moons of
course). Sentence (9) is a bit strange since there is only one moon surrounding earth.
However, if we were on Jupiter, (9) would be appropriate. Hence, the strangeness is not
caused by the grammar, but by our knowledge of the world.
e third test for determining the subject involves adding a tag question and see-
ing what the pronoun in the tag replaces. In (10), the it in the tag refers to the moon
and not to the sky and thats why the former is the subject:
(10) e moon has just risen in the sky, hasnt it?
Having discussed three criteria for identifying subjects, I turn to a kind of subject that,
at rst, does not look like a subject, namely, there in (11):
(11) ere are ve unicorns in the parking lot.
If we apply the three tests of Table 4.1 to sentence (11), there and ve unicorns each pass
some, but not all, of the tests for subject. For instance, in a question there and are switch
places; the tag will be formed with there, as in aren’t there; but the agreement on the
verb is determined by ve unicorns. To account for this, we’ll assume that both there and
ve unicorns function as the subject. ere is called a dummy or pleonastic or exple-
tive subject. It is used when no other subject occupies the position in the beginning
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
of the sentence. A variant of (11) is (12), where ve unicorns is in subject position and
there is not needed:
(12) Five unicorns are in the garden.
Turning to the predicate, we will just dene it as everything in the sentence that is not
the subject. In the tree, the predicate is always the VP that is under the right branch
right below the S and the subject is the NP right below the branch o the S on the le
side, as in (13).
(13)
S
NP VP
. Complements
A complement is what has to follow the verb. Complements come in dierent avors,
as direct and indirect objects and as subject and object predicates.
.
Direct and indirect object
A common function in the sentence is the direct object, usually realized as an NP, as in
(14) (see Chapter 7 for the use of a clause as direct object):
(14) a. Harry Potter played [a game].
b. He read [the letter from Hogwarts].
Objects occur as sisters to the verb, as in (15), and can be turned into subjects in a pas-
sive construction, as in (16):
(15)
S
NP VP
He
VNP
read
DN
the
NPP
letter
PNP
from Hogwarts
(16) [e letter from Hogwarts] was read by him.
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
In (16), the letter from Hogwarts functions as the subject because, for instance, in a
question it would switch with the auxiliary verb was. Make the subject in (16) plural
and see what that shows!
Passive sentences are variants of non-passive or active ones and come about by
switching the subject and the object and by adding a form of to be as in (17b), the
passive variant of (17a). e subject of the active sentence (17a) becomes optional in
the passive and, if expressed at all, is preceded by by:
(17)
a. I saw him. (active)
b. He was seen (by me). (passive)
Passives are useful when we don’t know who the agent of the action is and they oen
occur when the object is more denite than the subject, as in (18), but this is a complex
matter:
(18) e decision was made by a small group of people.
(Talk of the Nation, 1998, COCA)
Passivization is a way to distinguish between objects (both direct and indirect) on the
one hand and subject predicates, object predicates, and adverbials on the other, as well
see in the next chapter.
e indirect object expresses the goal (Santa in (19a)) or the beneciary of the
action (Harry in (19b)):
(19) a. I gave Santa a letter.
b. I made Harry some soup.
Indirect objects can be passivized as well, and in a sentence with both a direct and
indirect object, it is the indirect object that becomes the subject. For instance, (20) is
the passive counterpart of (19a), and the indirect object Santa becomes the subject,
not the direct object a letter:
(20) [Santa] was given a letter by me.
Indirect objects can be preceded by the prepositions to, in the case of the goal, and for,
in the case of a beneciary, as in (21) and (22):
(21) I gave a letter to Santa.
(22) I made some soup for Harry.
When to and for are added the order of indirect and direct object switches, as you
can see by comparing (18) with (21). Some grammarians call the PPs to Santa and for
Harry indirect objects; others call them adverbials since they seem less important to
the sentence (e.g. some can be le out) and they cannot become the subject of a passive
sentence, as the unacceptable (23) shows:
(23) *Santa was given a letter to.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
I will call the PPs in (21) and (22) indirect objects but am happy to listen to other views.
A list of verbs that take a direct object and those that take both a direct and indi-
rect object is given in Table 4.2. In Section 3, we will label the former transitive verbs
and the latter ditransitive. Many verbs have optional indirect objects. Table 4.2 is based
on a limited search of the British Nation Corpus and the Corpus of Spoken American
English; see Exercise N for more on this issue. You will have to decide each time you
see them in a sentence if they have a direct or a direct and an indirect object.
Table 4.2. Verbs with direct and indirect objects
Verbs with only direct objects with direct and mostly
obligatory indirect objects
with direct and less
obligatory indirect objects
see, eat, love, hit, hear,
watch
give, teach, oer, tell, show, ask, lend
provide, send, hand, promise, grant,
award, begrudge, mail, throw
buy, bring, bake, read,
pay,earn, build, cook, knit,
prepare
Making this table, I was very surprised how few verbs only have a direct object.
e reason is that we can imagine doing almost anything for the benet of others, e.g.
running a mile for the ASPCA and reading someone a book.
.
Subject and object predicate
e subject predicate is usually realized as an AdjP. It makes a claim about the subject,
as in (24), and can also be an NP, as in (25), or a PP, as in (26) (see Chapter 7 for the
use of a clause as subject predicate):
(24) He is [pleasant].
(25) He is [a nice person].
(26) He is [in the garden].
e verb used in sentences with a subject predicate is usually either be or become or
can be replaced by it. us, in the rst line of the poem by Dylan omas, discussed
in exercise H from Chapter 2, the adjective gentle goes with the unexpressed subject
and the verb go could be replaced by become. Other verbs that typically occur with a
subject predicate are feel, look, grow, and smell, when used as in (27):
(27) a. is silk feels nice.
b. at problem looks hard.
c. e kitten grew tired.
d. ose raspberries smell nice.
In (27abd), the verb can be replaced by a form of be and in (27c) by a form of become,
with some loss of specic meaning. If you replaced the transitive verb in (13) and (14)
with be or become, the results would be strange, to say the least.
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
As mentioned in the special topic to Chapter 2, many speakers overreact or panic
when they produce an adjective right next to a verb, as in (27). e combination is
correct, however, since the adjective modies a noun (functioning as subject). It need
not be changed to an adverb and in many cases it cant. A list of verbs that may have a
subject predicate in English appears in Table 4.3.
Table 4.3. Examples of verbs with subject predicates
act, appear, be, become, get, go, grow, fall, feel, keep, look, remain, seem, smell, sound, stay, taste, turn
Most of the verbs in Table 4.3 can be used in other ways too and thats why it is
important to think about the entire sentence and not just to look at the verb. For instance,
each of the verbs in (27) can be used without a subject predicate, as (28) shows:
(28) a. He felt his pulse.
b. ey looked around.
c. She grew strawberries in her garden.
d. I smell trouble.
In (24) and (27), the adjective functioning as a subject predicate says something about
the subject, but an adjective can also say something about a direct object. e adjective
then functions as an object predicate. ere are relatively few verbs that take a direct
object and an object predicate, so don’t overuse the function in your analysis! It is safe
to say that, if you see a verb such as consider, you need to think about the possibility of
an object predicate, but not with verbs such as see, read, and go.
e object predicate is usually an adjective phrase, as in (29), but can also be an
NP, as in (30), or a PP, as in (31):
(29) e students found the exam [dicult].
(30) Jane considers Pride and Prejudice [a classic].
(31) She put the cup [on the table].
A few examples are given in Table 4.4.
Table 4.4. Verbs with direct objects and object predicates
consider I considered Sabina very smart.
think I thought Timber (to be) nice.
nd ey found Einstein interesting.
know I know Chandra to be nice.
put She put snails on the table.
place ey placed a jar upon a hill
call ey called the ship e Lauderdale.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Here too, it sometimes depends on your analysis whether you consider a phrase
an object predicate or a direct object. For instance, a good chairperson in (32) can be an
object predicate to the direct object him, in which case to be can occur between them,
as in (33), and him is the same person as a good chairperson. Alternatively, him can be
an indirect object and a good chairperson a direct object, in which case for can precede
him, as in (34), and him and a good chairperson are not the same person. Hence, the
verb nd is ambiguous:
(32) ey found him [a good chairperson]. (ambiguous)
(33) ey found him to be [a good chairperson]. (Object Predicate)
(34) ey found for him [a good chairperson]. (Direct Object)
e terms for the two functions discussed in this section are much debated. Some
grammarians call them subject and object complements; others subject and object
predicatives; yet others call them subject and object attributives. I have chosen subject
and object predicate to show that their function is similar to that of the VP predicate.
It is as if the AdjP is more important than the verb in these constructions. at is the
reason the verb in (24) to (26) can be le out in many languages and, in English, no
verb appears to link object and object predicate, even though to be can be included in
(33) and in (35):
(35) Jane considers Pride and Prejudice to be [a classic].
Four suggestions on identifying the object predicate are as follows. (a) Only use this
label if you have a (direct) object; (b) if you see the verb consider, it is a good candidate
for having a direct object and object predicate; (c) if you leave out the object predi-
cate, the sentence is incomplete or has a dierent meaning; and (d) dont overuse the
function!
In short, some of the major functions of phrases in the sentence are subject, predi-
cate, direct and indirect object, subject predicate and object predicate. ere are spe-
cial objects such as prepositional objects and objects of phrasal verbs. ese will be
dealt with in Chapter 5 together with the adverbial function.
.
Verbs and functions
Verbs are distinguished depending on what objects or object predicates they select.
Verbs that select objects are called transitive verbs and those that dont, as in (36)
below, intransitive. If the verb selects one object, as in (13) and (14) above, it is (mono)
transitive; if it selects two objects, as in (19), it is ditransitive. Verbs that select a subject
predicate, as in (24) to (27), are called copula verbs or linking verbs and those that have
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
both an object and an object predicate, as in (29) to (31), are called complex transi-
tive. Two more types of verbs will be discussed in the next chapter: prepositional and
phrasal verbs. Since adverbials can always be added to any verb, they do not play a role
in the classication of the verb. I will now provide examples of each kind of verb.
Examples of intransitives are swim, walk, arrive, cough, sleep, and sneeze. ey do
not need a complement:
(36) He sneezed and sneezed.
(37) He slept during the meeting.
As mentioned before, you should look at the entire sentence before you can be com-
pletely sure of the classication of the particular verb. us, walk in I walk the dog is
transitive, but in I walked for hours it is not. (In the next chapter, Section 5, I give some
reasons why during the meeting in (37) is not an object but an adverbial).
Examples of (mono-)transitives are eat, read, see, hear, plant, write, compose,
paint, love, hate, drink, hit, and hug, as in (38). ey have a direct object complement:
(38) He hugged the ball.
As also seen in Table 4.2, give, tell, bake, cook, and play are ditransitives. A typical
ditransitive appears in (39). However, many transitive verbs have optional indirect
objects as mentioned before. e example given in (40) contains a fairly optional indi-
rect object him:
(39) ey told the public a lie.
(40) I played (him) a tune.
If a verb selects a subject predicate, it is called a copula or linking verb. A number
of copula verbs are given above in Table 4.3, namely be, become, go, feel, look, grow,
seem, and smell. Complex transitives are verbs such as consider, know, elect, keep,
prove, deem, judge, reckon, make, and regard. ey have direct objects and object
predicates as their complements. ey are similar to regular transitives except that
their object needs some modication. Please notice (again) that many verbs belong
to more than one category. For instance, make can be a transitive, as in I made a
sweater, or a complex transitive, as in She made them happy, or a ditransitive, as in
She made them a cake.
Distinctions such as transitive and intransitive are useful to explain when to use
verb forms such as lay/lie, set/sit, and fell/fall. e rst verb in these three sets is the
transitive one and the second is the intransitive. e verbs are irregular in that the
normal rules for past tense (add -ed) and participle (add -ed or -(e)n) do not apply.
Sentences (41a) and (42a) are in the present tense, (41b) and (42b) in the past tense,
and (41c) and (42c) in the present perfect. (We’ll discuss these terms in Chapter 6):
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(41) a. is chicken lays an egg every day. (transitive irregular)
b. He laid that book on the table yesterday.
c. I have laid the table like this for years.
(42) a. I lie down regularly. (intransitive irregular)
b. I lay down yesterday.
c. I have lain here for hours.
One problem with these verbs lies in the past tense of the intransitive being the same
as the present of the transitive. Both of these are irregular verbs since they are not
predictable. In addition, there is an intransitive lie, meaning not telling the truth, that
is regular in form, as (43) shows:
(43) a. ey always lie under oath. (intransitive regular)
b. ey always lied under oath.
c. ey have always lied.
is makes them really dicult to use!
To nish this section, I’ll just again list some intransitive, transitive, and ditransi-
tive verbs, as well as copulas and complex transitives with their complements. Seeing
all together might give a better picture.
Table 4.5. Examples of the verb classes so far with their complements
Intransitive no complement
walk, go, arrive, sneeze, go, lie (as in both lie to congress and lie down), sit, die, and swim
Transitive one (direct) object
see, eat, love, hit, hug, drink, break (as in break the vase), and paint.
Ditransitive one direct and one indirect object
give, teach, oer, tell, show, ask, lend, buy, bring, bake, read, provide, send, hand, promise, grant, cook,
prepare, award, begrudge, mail, and throw
Copula one subject predicate
be, become, seem, appear, look, remain, keep, stay, fall, turn
Complex Transitive one (direct) object and an object predicate
consider, nd, know, name (as in name the ship the Albatros)
. Trees for all verb types
As I have mentioned before, the tree structure reects what the function of each phrase
is. us, the subject and the predicate are the daughters of S, and the objects and sub-
ject predicate are sisters to V. e object predicate is a bit more complex but can be
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
argued to be a sister of V too. e adverbial elements, as we’ll see in the next chapter,
are not sisters to V, but the prepositional objects and objects to phrasal verbs are.
Intransitives may occupy the entire VP, as in (44):
(44)
S
NP VP
I
V
laughed
A structure for the (mono)transitive verb of (38) above is (45), and for the copula verb
of (27a) above, it is (46):
(45)
S
NP VP
He
V
NP
hugged
DN
the ball
(46)
S
NP VP
at
V AdjP
feels
Adj
nice
In general, we try to make trees show hierarchies, i.e. we seek to avoid triple branches
in (47). However, to show that both the direct and indirect object in (47) are objects, I
have drawn them as sisters to the V:
(47)
S
NP VP
She
VNPNP
gave him
N
books
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
ere are ways of expressing this in a non-at/hierarchical structure but they are com-
plicated and still controversial. Hence, this book will use (47), noting the problem of
the atness of the VP.
The other complement where flatness is a problem is the one to the complex
transitive verb, as in (29) to (31) above. Since the object and predicate in some way
form a unit (unlike the direct and indirect object), I’ll represent it as in (48a), labeling
the node above NP and AdjP a small clause (SC), i.e. a clause with the verb deleted.
If the verb is present, the structure will look like (48b). More on (48b) in Chapter 8,
however:
(48)
a. b.S
NP VP
She
VSC
found
NP AdjP
it
Adj
nice
S
NP VP
She
VS
found
NP VP
it
V AdjP
to be
Adj
nice
If the small clause in (48a) really has a be verb le out, we can think of the object predi-
cate as a sister to V as well.
.
Light verbs (optional)
is is an introductory grammar text, and hence not all kinds of verbs can be dealt
with. To give an example of such a group, well look at light verbs, an interesting set of
verbs in English that combines with mainly indenite nouns (and sometimes preposi-
tions also). Examples of light verbs with nouns are given in (49):
(49) have a look, take a look, take a rest, take a tumble, take the initiative, take heart, take
measures, give advice (on), make a decision (on), do a translation (of), do harm,
give a hand, make trouble (for).
e verb and noun together have the meaning of a verb, e.g. have/take a look is similar
to the verb look, give advice to the verb advise, and make a decision to the verb decide.
With some, e.g. do a translation, the noun is still a real object and can be passivized, as
in A translation of Homer was done by that famous writer although the indenite sub-
ject sounds unusual; with some, e.g. take a look, the passive sounds ungrammatical, as
in A look was taken by me. We won’t draw trees for these or analyze them further.
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
. Conclusion
In this chapter, we have discussed six major functions for which phrases are used:
subject, predicate, direct and indirect object, subject predicate, and object predicate.
Particular functions are realized by particular phrases, e.g. the subject is oen an NP.
In Figure 4.1, a schematic representation for the functions of the phrases NP, VP and
AdjP is given. Apart from VP, which is always a predicate (and the other way round),
there is no one-to-one relationship between a phrase and a function. In Chapter 5, PPs
and AdvPs will be discussed.
Subject ([the driver] laughed)Subject Predicate (is [a student])
Direct Object (see [the problem])NPObject Predicate (consider him [a fool])
Indirect Object (give [the dog] food) Adverbial (le [this week], see Chap 5)
Predicate (He [saw the clock])VP
Subject Predicate (is [nice]) AdjPObject Predicate (consider him [foolish])
Figure 4.1. A schema of the functions of NPs, VPs, and AdjPs
e classication of verbs is dependent on the kinds of objects and predicates they
have. e obligatory elements following the verb are called complements. We have
seen ve classes of verbs. Intransitives have no objects, (mono)transitives have one,
and ditransitives have two objects, a direct and an indirect. Copula verbs have a sub-
ject predicate and complex transitive verbs have an object and an object predicate. See
what you nd easier to recognize: the verb (as transitive or ditransitive) or the func-
tions. Tree structures are also provided for each of these verbs with the complements
as sisters to the V.
Key terms are the six functions (subject, predicate, direct and indirect object,
subject predicate and object predicate); and the classification of five verb types
(intransitive, (mono)transitive, ditransitive, copula, and complex transitive). Be
careful to keep function and phrase separate!
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Exercises
A. Provide an example of each of the six functions we have discussed so far.
B. Examples of subjects are given in brackets in (50), which is adapted from a wikipedia
entry on javelinas. Do you agree with this selection of subjects?
(50) [Peccaries] are medium-sized animals, with a strong supercial resemblance to
pigs. Like pigs, [they] have a snout ending in a cartilagenous disc, and eyes [that]
are small relative to their head. Also like pigs, [they] use only the middle two digits
for walking, although, unlike pigs, [the other toes] can be altogether absent.
[Their stomach] is non-ruminating, although [it] has three chambers, and is more
complex than [that of pigs] is. (http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Peccary)
C.
Identify the subjects in the text used in Chapter 2, repeated here:
At last, we had begun lming. Should I say ‘we’? I was living in the house and extremely
curious about everything connected with the lm. Fortunately, they let me hang around
and even gave me a job. As an historian, I kept an eye on detail and did not allow the
lmmakers to stray too far from the period of Louis Philippe. The project was to make an
hour-long lm about Houdin and it was decided to shoot the picture in Switzerland. This
may have been a bad idea. It certainly mixed professional and domestic aairs.
D
. Identify the functions of the phrases in brackets in the sentences below:
(51) [I] gave [him] [the ticket].
(52) [They] [planted a dogwood].
(53) [The trees in the park] are [unhappy].
E. Identify the dierent kinds of complements (e.g. direct object, subject predicate) in (54)
to (60):
(54) They sold us their furniture.
(55) Tom submits his tax-returns.
(56) She seemed very happy.
(57) He found it easy.
(58) He took the early train.
(59) The politician considered that argument invalid.
(60) That sounds terrible.
F. Provide the labels of the verbs in (54) to (60) (e.g. copula, ditransitive).
G. Draw trees for sentences (54) to (60).
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
H. List all the functions and names/labels of the phrases in (61) to (64). To what categories
do the following words belong: helpful, from, and hard-working?
(61) I considered the book very helpful.
(62) He baked Joan a cake.
(63) The pig from Mars left.
(64) The hard-working students seemed exhausted.
I. Read the rst part of Mavis Gallants short story About Geneva”:
Granny was waiting at the door of the apartment. She looked small, lonely, and patient,
and at the sight of her the children and their mother felt instantly guilty. Instead of
driving straight home from the airport, they had stopped outside Nice for ice cream.
They might have known how much those extra twenty minutes would mean to Granny.
Colin, too young to know what he felt, or why, began instinctively to misbehave,
dragging his feet, scratching the waxed parquet. Ursula bit her nails, taking refuge in a
dream, while the childrens mother, Granny’s only daughter, felt compelled to cry in a
high, cheery voice, Well, Granny, here they are, safe and sound!”
What kinds of verbs are wait, look, feel, and drive in this text? What is the function of those
extra twenty minutes, the waxed parquet, and small, lonely, and patient?
J. Identify the subjects in (65) to (68). Provide two reasons why in each case:
(65) In the rain, it is sometimes hard to see.
(66) Only one of these people is happy.
(67) The book Chomsky wrote when he was young was reissued last year.
(68) Were the Wizard of Oz and Catweazle preparing to go to Alabama?
K. We reviewed the rule for lay, lie, etc in Section 3. State it in simple terms and then discuss
what is happening in the cartoon?
Figure 4.2. Lie ahead. (Used with the permission of GEC, Inc. and United Media in conjunction
with the Cartoonist Group. All rights reserved)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
L. Sentence is (69) is quite complex. What kinds of verbs are grow and look? What are their
complements?
(69) We must expect to see her grown thin, and looking very poorly. (Jane Austen,
Emma, Vol 2, Chap 1).
M.
Dicult. The excerpt below is from Roethkes Villanelle ‘I wake to sleep and take my
waking slow, of which only the rst six lines are given. Discuss the types of verbs that are
used (intransitive, transitive, etc). If you are interested in literature, you may also look at
the function of the adjectives and adverbs.
The Waking
I wake to sleep and take my waking slow.
I feel my fate in what I cannot fear.
I learn by going where I have to go.
We think by feeling. What is there to know?
I hear my being dance from ear to ear.
I wake to sleep and take my waking slow
Class discussion
N. Find two intransitive verbs and two copula verbs (without looking directly in the book).
Also, provide two sentences with only a direct object and two sentences with a direct
and indirect object.
O. Use the British National Corpus (BNC at http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/) or the Corpus of
Contemporary American English (COCA at http://www.americancorpus.org/) to see if any of
your copula, transitive, and ditransitive verbs can be found with the complement you
selected in the previous question.
Keys to the exercises
A. Subject and predicate in [He][left]; direct object and direct object in I gave [myself]
[flowers]; subject predicate in Flowers are [nice]; and object predicate in I thought that
[stupid].
B. Peccaries can be inverted in a question, as in Are peccaries medium-sized animals, with
a strong supercial resemblance to pigs? It is a pretty obvious subject and if you made it
singular, the verb would change, as in A peccary is a medium-sized animal. The third test
would give you: Peccaries are medium-sized animals, aren’t they?
The next subject they does not appear immediately at the beginning, but making they
singular would have an eect on the verb, as in Like pigs, it has a snout ending in a
cartilagenous disc.
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
The third subject is trickier since we haven’t talked about relative clauses yet. So, not to
worry if you didn’t see that. It wouldn’t be on an exam at this point. The object eyes in
this sentence is modied by that are small relative to their head. In that relative, that is
the subject.
The next they is pretty obvious again. You could make it singular, as in Also like pigs, it
uses only the middle two digits for walking.
The other toes and their stomach are obvious intuitively although the tests are a little
hard to apply. You would have to change the sentences to make them into a single
sentence question: Can the other toes be altogether absent? and Is their stomach
non-ruminating?
The next subject is it which if plural would cause the verb to be have.
The last subject is again tricky. The original text didn’t have the is following the NP that
of pigs. After than, NPs on their own can be analyzed as subjects if you think there is a
verb left out, and that’s what I did.
C. we, I, I, they, I, the lmmakers (this will be clearer after Chapter 8), the project, it, this, it.
D. [I] gave [him] [the ticket]: S, IO, DO
[They] [planted a dogwood]: S, Pred
[The trees in the park] are [unhappy]: S, SuPred
E. (54) They sold us their furniture: IO DO
(55) Tom submits his tax-returns: DO
(56) She seemed very happy: SuPred
(57) He found it easy: DO, ObPred
(58) He took the early train: DO
(59) The politician considered that argument invalid: DO, ObPred
(60) That sounds terrible: SuPred
F. The verbs are ditransitive, transitive, copula, complex transitive, transitive, complex
transitive, and copula.
G. (54)
S
NP VP
They
VNPNP
sold us
DN
their furniture
(55)
S
NP VP
Tom
VNP
submits
DN
his tax-returns
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(56)
S
NP VP
She
V AdjP
seemed
Adv Adj
very happy
(57)
S
NP VP
He
VSC
found
NP AdjP
it Adj
easy
The tree for (58) is similar to the one for (55); the tree for (59) is similar to the one for (57);
and the tree for (60) is similar to (56).
H. Su:NP, Pred:VP, DO:NP, ObjPred:AdjP
Su:NP, Pred:VP, IO:NP, DO:NP
Su:NP, Pred:VP
Su:NP, Pred:VP, SuPred:AdjP
And the categories are: Adj, P, Adj
I.
wait: intransitive; look: copula; feel: copula; drive: intransitive (debatable).
those extra twenty minutes: Su; the waxed parquet: DO; small, lonely, patient: SuPred.
J. In (65), it; in (66), only two of those people; in (67), The book Chomsky wrote when
he was young; in (68), the Wizard of Oz and Catweazle. I’ll give some reasons.
In (68), Were the Wizard of Oz and Catweazle preparing to go to Alabama, the subject and
auxiliary have already inverted since it is a question. Thus, without having to invert the
sentence yourself, you can see what the subject is, namely the phrase after the auxiliary.
The agreement on were is plural which ts if the subject is the coordinated the Wizard
of Oz and Catweazle. Notice that if you changed it to just the Wizard, the agreement
becomes singular: Was the Wizard preparing to go? The tag question test doesn’t work in
a sentence that is already a question.
In the three remaining sentences, you could use tag-questions in some:
In the rain, it is sometimes hard to see, isn’t it?
The book Chomsky wrote when he was young was reissued last year, wasn’t it?
And Inversion would work as follows in the non-questions:
In the rain, is it sometimes hard to see?
Is only one of these people happy?
Was the book Chomsky wrote when he was young reissued last year?
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
If you changed the subject, the following would show the subject (this is hard to do with
it in (65) though):
Two of these people are happy.
The books Chomsky wrote when he was young were reissued last year.
K. The funny aspect involves hypercorrection. The initial use of lies was correct. Since the
future always lies ahead, the content is expressed in a redundant way that we often use.
Some people pick on this.
L. In this sentence, the verbs grow and look are used as copulas since the adjective thin
is used as subject predicate to modify her and poorly is used as subject predicate as
well since it modies the left out her. So, even though poorly looks like an adverb, it is
an adjective (the Oxford English Dictionary says that the use of poorly as an adjective is
somewhat rare, but it is quite frequent in Jane Austen). Grow can also be a transitive
verb and look a prepositional object verb, but not in this sentence.
M. Many of the verbs are intransitive (wake, sleep, fear, go, dance) or used as intransitive
(learn, think, know). This contributes to the apparent simplicity. The verb take is twice
used as a complex predicate (although one could also argue it is light verb). There are
two transitives (feel, hear) and one copula (be). There is only one adjective (slow), used
twice. The adverbs are -not, where, and there.
An analysis:
Roethke’s ‘I wake to sleep and take my waking slow’ is dominated by verbs. There is
symmetry in the two sentences in that both start with similar sounding verbs (wake
and take) and the rst verb is repeated as a noun (waking). This focuses our attention
on the waking and yet the author purports to be interested in sleeping.
As to the use of adjectives, only one is used (slow) and, on rst reading, we might
think this is incorrect and that it has to be an adverb (slowly). It is not incorrect and,
moreover, using the adjective rather than the adverb focuses our attention on waking
rather than on the verb take. Both the use of the verbs and the adjective contribute
to making the poem puzzling since, if the poet really wanted to sleep, he should not
want to be slow in falling asleep.
Special topic: Case and agreement
In this special topic section, we’ll discuss case and agreement. Most of these rules have changed
in the last 500 years and native speakers of English have lost their intuitions on case endings
and agreement. Prescriptive rules die hard, however, so we’ll discuss those and see how subject
and object can be helpful here. One prescriptive rule can be formulated as (70):
(70) Case in English:
Subjects have nominative case. Direct and indirect objects have accusative or
objective case. Prepositional objects also have accusative or objective case. Pos-
sessive nouns have genitive case.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
In Modern English, cases are only visible on pronouns. For instance, in (71), the subject she is
nominative and the direct object him accusative. Me has objective or accusative case because
of the preposition towards. With full NPs, it is not obvious what the case is. Notice the lack of
obvious case on the garden.
(71) She saw him come towards me in the garden.
In (72), the demonstrative this doesn’t show case, but you and me have accusative case since they
are objects of the preposition between. In coordinates such as these, however, the rule is often broken
in all stages of English. Thus, in (73) and (74), the nominative I is used rather than the accusative me
and in (75), the nominative he is used where an accusative would be expected (prescriptively):
(72) This (matter) is between you and me.
(73) all debts are cleared between you and I. Shakespeare, Merchant of Venice III, 2, 321
(74) If you are sick and tired of the way it’s been going, …, you give Al Gore and I a chance
to bring America back. (Bill Clinton, as reported in the New York Times in the 1990s)
(75) In his speech, Mr. Giuliani said that one of the main dierences between he and
Mrs. Clinton was that “I’m in favor of reducing your taxes … (again as reported in
the New York Times, 8 April 2000).
In (76), the accusative thee (a special form for the second person, no longer used in Modern
English) is used rather than the nominative thou (again no longer in use). The nominative would
be expected since the Diuell and thee are the subject:
(76) How agrees [the Diuell and thee] about thy soule?
(Shakespeare 1 Henry IV I, 2, 126)
Notice that in (76), the agreement on the verb is singular as well even though the subject is the
plural the Diuell and thee. This ‘mistake happens often in coordinated subjects.
With wh-questions, the case rule is also often broken. Thus, in (77), the use of whom in
the rst frame would sound very articial even though, as the accusative or objective form, it is
the correct form. Whom cares is not correct but completely appropriate in Figure 4.3.
(77) Who shall I say is calling?
Figure 4.3. Who or whom? (Used with the permission of King Features Syndicate in conjunction
with the Cartoonist Group. All rights reserved.)
Chapter 4. Functions in the sentence 
Similarly, after copulas, many people insist on using the nominative. Before 1600 or so, in
sentences with the copula verb to be, both subject and subject predicate have nominative case.
Nowadays, this sounds overly formal.
The genitive case is used in cases such as (78) and (79). If the word does not end in s, an
apostrophe and s are added, as in (78), but if it ends in an s, as in (79), either an apostrophe and
s or just an apostrophe is added. Many people consider the ending in (79) pedantic and hence
it often disappears altogether:
(78) Shakespeare’s works
(79) Employees’(s) cafeteria
Turning to agreement, we can formulate the rule, as in (80). We have used it above to nd the
subject and the head of the NP that functions as subject:
(80) Agreement in English
The subject of a sentence agrees in person and number with the nite verb.
In Modern English, there is little agreement left on the verb. In standard English, apart from the
verb to be (I am, you are, s/he is, we are, you are, and they are etc.), there is only a third person
singular -s ending on verbs in the present tense (e.g. I walk, you walk, s/he walks, we walk, you
walk, and they walk). Note that in some varieties of English, words such as police and government
are singular, whereas in others, they are plural. In general, as long as you are consistent, either
should be ok.
The diculties with agreement usually occur with long subjects, as in (81), or with dummy
subjects, as in (82):
(81) [One of the problems that they worried about continuously] were solved rather
quickly.
(82) There’s some problems that they could not solve.
Prescriptively, (81) should have singular agreement, i.e. was, and (82) should be plural, i.e. are.
In earlier varieties of English, e.g. in 16th century English, there is much more agreement.
For instance, in (83), the verb agrees with the second person singular thou. In some varieties of
English, no agreement is left, as in (84), and in some, both singular and plural are possible, as in
(85), from Hiberno-English:
(83) Caes. What sayst thou to me now? Speak once againe.
Sooth. Beware the Ides of March. (Shakespeare, Julius Ceasar, I, 2, 18)
(84) The dog stay outside in the afternoon.
(85) The boys is/are leaving.
Chapter 5
More functions, of prepositions
and particles
1. Adverbials
2. Prepositional verbs
3. Phrasal verbs
4. Phrasal prepositional verbs (optional)
5. Objects and adverbials
6. Conclusion
is chapter deals with adverbials, i.e. the optional elements in the sentences that pro-
vide background information on when, where, why, and how the event described by
the verb and its objects takes place. It is important to be aware that adverbials are not
always realized as Adverb Phrases, but can also be realized as PPs or NPs (or as clauses,
see Chapter 7). Note that the term ‘adverb’ refers to the category that heads the Adverb
Phrase (AdvP) and that ‘adverbial’ refers to the function.
Prepositional objects are also discussed since they look like adverbials, but can
be argued not to be. Objects to phrasal verbs are regular direct objects. ey are dis-
cussed here rather than in Chapter 4 because they are easily confused with preposi-
tional objects and include a preposition-like element called a particle. Finally, two
other kinds of verbs are discussed involving particles and prepositions: the intransitive
phrasal verb and the phrasal prepositional verb.
e main point of this chapter is to learn to distinguish between adverbials and
objects. I’ll provide some tests for this. When a sentence is passivized, the functions of
subject and object may be reversed, i.e. an object then functions as a subject, whereas
an adverbial can never function as a subject.
.
Adverbials
When adverbials modify verbs, they express when, where, how, and why the action
takes place. So, they give background information on time, place, manner, and cause
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
of the event. In the tree structure, we make a distinction between direct and indirect
objects, subject predicates, and object predicates on the one hand (all referred to as
complements) and adverbials on the other: objects, subject predicates, and object
predicates are closer to the verb than adverbials. Even if in the tree the functions of the
phrases are not indicated, you should be able to tell from the tree which phrase is the
object and which is the adverbial, e.g. in (1):
(1)
S
NP VP
She
AdvP
Adv
quickly
V
VNP
wrote
DN
the story
In (1), the NP the story is sister to the V wrote and is therefore the object; the AdvP
quickly is sister to the intermediate V wrote the story, and therefore modies that.
Quickly tells you how the story was written and is therefore an adverbial. Since the V
that represents wrote the story is intermediate inside the VP, we call it a V (‘V-bar’).
When you draw the tree, perhaps look back to Chapter 3 (Section 5) and remem-
ber possible ambiguities, e.g. in (20) of Chapter 3. Well go over how to construct the
tree in (1) quickly (from top to bottom). First, you start with S, whose daughters are
always NP and VP, as shown in (2a). e NP happens to be a pronoun, so nothing
else needs to be done to the NP in terms of branches. e VP consists of a V wrote,
an object the story, and an adverbial quickly. Be careful not to make the rst branch
to the le into a V because then you wont have space for all three. Instead, use the V,
as in (2b), and then think what should be closest to the V, and t them in, as in (2c).
Remember the V is a placeholder for lots of branches, a little VP so to speak, but still
with the V as its head:
(2)
a. b. c. S
NP VP
She
V AdvP
VNP
S
NP VP
SheShe
V AdvP
S
NP VP
Now add the words to V and branches and words to the NP and AdvP and you have
the nished tree of (1).
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
ere is a dierence between a VP-adverbial, e.g. quickly in (1), and a sentence
adverbial, e.g. fortunately, actually, indeed, and of course. Sentence adverbials (or
S-adverbials) do not modify the action of the VP but express the views and the mood
of the speaker. Trees for a sentence-initial and sentence-nal S-adverbial are given
in (3). Duplicating the S intends to show that the adverbial is really outside the core
sentence:
(3)
a. Sb.S
AdvP S AdvP
AdvNPVP Adv
Unfortunately they unfortunately
V
lied
S
NP VP
ey
V
lied
Certain adverbs can be both VP-adverbials and S-adverbials. For instance, happily
has two interpretations in (4). One is where painting the pictures was a happy event,
in which case happily is a VP-adverbial modifying painted those pictures, and the
comma is less appropriate. A second interpretation is where the speaker expresses
an opinion about the entire sentence (perhaps because the pictures turned out to
be good):
(4) Happily, they painted those pictures.
e same ambiguity exists for adverbs such as wisely and clearly. For most speakers of
English, hopefully too is both a VP-adverbial and an S-adverbial, although for unclear
reasons some people object to its use as an S-adverbial (see special topic to Chapter 2).
PPs that function as adverbials are typically VP-adverbials. ey oen provide
background information regarding place, as in (5), and time, as in (6):
(5)
S
NP VP
She
V PP
VNPP NP
ate in
DNDN
that food the dark
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
(6)
S
NP VP
NV PP
Javelinas
VNPP NP
roam
in
DN DN
the deserta the evenin
g
A sentence can have many adverbials (depending on the speaker’s or hearer’s patience).
For instance, in (7), the speaker’s feelings (the AdvP unfortunately), the time (the NP
that morning), and place (the PP to work) of driving the car are given, as well as the
reason for this action (the sentence the bus had broken down) and the way in which the
action occurred (the PP without glasses):
(7) [Unfortunately], he drove the car [to work] [that morning] [without his glasses]
[because the bus had broken down].
It is possible to add more adverbials to this sentence, e.g. quickly or recklessly.
As can be seen in (7), adverbials are not only realized as AdvPs such as quickly, but
also as NPs (that morning), PPs (to work and without his glasses), and clauses (because
the bus had broken down, see Chapter 7). is means NPs function not only as sub-
jects, indirect and direct objects, subject predicates and object predicates (see previous
chapter), but also as adverbials. AdvPs on the other hand only function as adverbials.
PPs function mainly as adverbials and subject predicates but, as we’ll see in the next
section, they also function as objects to certain verbs, namely prepositional ones (and,
as we saw in Chapter 3 and will see in more detail in Chapter 9, they can also be modi-
ers inside a phrase). Identify the adverbials in Figure 5.1.
Figure 5.1. Adverbials. (Used with the permission of King Features Syndicate in conjunction with
the Cartoonist Group. All rights reserved.)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
All adverbials used by Beetle are VP-adverbials, but one could imagine an
S-adverbial, such as unfortunately, being used! eir realization is through an AdvP
(thoroughly, completely, and expeditiously) and through a PP (with gusto and verve,
without delay, and with determination).
We’ll now turn to PPs that sometimes resemble adverbials but are actually objects.
.
Prepositional verbs
Prepositional verbs are verbs such as abide by in (8), refer to in (9), glance at, lean
against, add to, allow for, approve of, care for, insist on, resort to, apply for, account for,
reply to, absolve from, long for, yearn for, result in, argue about, and defer to. e P with
the NP functions as an object:
(8) ey abided [by the contract].
(9) He referred [to that article].
ese verbs require a PP, i.e. (10) and (11) are ungrammatical, and thats why the PP is
considered an object rather than an adverbial. e contract in (8) and that article in (9)
can also be passivized, as in (12) and (13), and this test shows that they are real objects,
as shown in (14), where the PP is sister to V:
(10) *He abides.
(11) *He refers all the time.
(12) e contract was abided by.
(13) at article wasnt referred to by him.
(14)
S
NP VP
He
VPP
referred
PNP
to
DN
that article
Native speakers of English know that verbs such as refer are combined with a certain
preposition. Non-native speakers must learn the meanings of these verbs or look them
up in a dictionary, e.g. refer with, refer about, refer at are not possible.
.
Phrasal verbs
Phrasal verbs must be distinguished from prepositional verbs and from verbs with
an adverbial. Like prepositional verbs, they are listed separately in a dictionary
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
since their combinations are somewhat idiosyncratic. Examples of phrasal verbs
are call up, bring up, cover up, look over, take away, turn in, put down, take o, put
on, switch on/o, hand in, make out (as in decipher’). Some example sentences are
given in (15) to (19):
(15) ey called up the president.
(16) ey covered up the scandal.
(17) Helen turned in her homework.
(18) She put down the nasty people.
(19) She switched on the light.
e prepositions up, in, down, and on accompanying these verbs have become par-
ticles rather than prepositions or adverbs since they no longer always express place
or direction. e structure of a sentence such as (15) is therefore one of a verb with a
particle, as in (20):
(20)
S
NP VP
ey
VNP
called up
DN
the president
us, in (20), the verb and particle are placed in V together, whereas the object is a
separate NP. We could represent the verb in (20) as a compound verb if you dont like
two words under one category.
One of the easy (but not so well understood) criteria for determining if a verb is
phrasal is whether the (pronominalized) object can be put between the verb and the
particle, as in (21) to (25):
(21) ey called him up.
(22) ey covered it up.
(23) She turned it in with many mistakes.
(24) She put them down.
(25) She switched it on.
is is not possible with prepositional verbs, as the unacceptable (26) shows:
(26) *ey abided it by.
e basic distinction, clear from (20), is that the V and particle form a unit and that the
object is an NP, not a PP. is is so because (a) a pause can occur between the verb particle
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
complex and the NP object, as in (27), but not between the V and the unit which is not a
phrase as in (28); (b) the NP objects of a phrasal verb can be coordinated, as in (29), but the
particle and NP cannot be coordinated with another particle and NP, as (30) shows; and
(c) moving the NP object to the le by itself, as in (31), is ok, indicating the NP is a unit,
but moving the particle and the NP together is not ok, as (32) shows, indicating they do
not form a phrase:
(27) She put down — the customers.
(28) *She put — down the customers.
(29) She put down the customers and the owner.
(30) *She put down the customers and down the owner.
(31) It was the customers she put down.
(32) *It was down the customers she put.
In (33) and (34), examples are given of phrasal verbs without an NP object:
(33) His career is taking off.
(34) ey nally gave in.
Because the verb and particle have lost their independent meanings in (33) and (34),
just like the verbs in (15) to (25) above, they are referred to as phrasal verbs. Unlike
the phrasal verbs in (15) to (25), the ones in (33) and (34) lack objects. Some other
examples are sleep in, and turn in, as in (35):
(35) Even though I turned in early last night, I slept in.
Figure 5.2 exemplies a few more phrasal verbs. Calvin uses them intransitively, but
some of these are also possible as transitive ones (shut them up and mix them up).
Figure 5.2. More Phrasal verbs (Reprinted with the permission of Universal Press Syndicate.
All rights reserved)
In Table 5.1, I am listing some phrasal verbs that just came to mind starting with
the letter ‘a, but there are thousands. For this reason, there are dictionaries of phrasal
verbs in English (and some are online).
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
Table 5.1. Examples of phrasal verbs
Intransitive Transitive
add up mean something add up calculate/add
drop out stop participating back up put it in reverse
break down experience a crisis bring o accomplish
catch on (begin) to understand bring out publish
carry on continue as before bring up raise (a child)
die out diminish in intensity drop o deliver
Phrasal verbs are in general used in less formal styles; the synonyms in Table 5.1
are much more formal vocabulary choices.
.
Phrasal prepositional verbs (optional)
Constructions with phrasal prepositional verbs combine a verb, a particle, a preposi-
tion, and an NP. e object of such a verb is a prepositional object, as indicated with
brackets in (36) and (37) for the verbs put up with and come up with:
(36) Orrmm will not put up [with that noise].
(37) Benji came up [with a new solution to Fermats eorem].
e reason the verbs are phrasal is that the verb and the particle have lost their inde-
pendent meaning. ey are, however, not very prepositional since the prepositional
object cannot be passivized very well, as the awkwardness of (38) shows (and I indicate
the awkwardness by means of a question mark):
(38) ?at noise will not be put up with.
Other examples are cut down on, catch up on, get away with, stand up for, face up to, and
check up on. Like phrasal verbs, phrasal prepositional verbs are very colloquial and are
oen avoided in formal writing. Could you think of a single verb that can replace the
phrasal prepositionals in examples (36) and (37)?
.
Objects and adverbials
As a possible help in distinguishing the dierent functions, Table 5.2 is provided:
Table 5.2. Dierences among objects, su/obj predicates, and adverbials
Objects Su/Obj Predicates Adverbials
Obligatory yes yes no: optional
info on time, place, manner, etc.
Passive yes no no
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
I have already mentioned that adverbials are optional but that objects and predi-
cates are not. us, in contrast to prepositional objects, such as those in (10) and (11)
above, an adverbial PP can be le out, as (39ab) shows.
(39) a. He slept [during the meeting].
b. He slept.
A second criterion for distinguishing the dierent functions is passivization. As men-
tioned, direct and indirect objects and the NP in the prepositional object can be pas-
sivized, e.g. (40), (41), and (42) respectively:
(40) Emma was seen. (active: Someone saw Emma.)
(41) Walter was given a book. (active: Someone gave Wal te r a book.)
(42) e article was referred to. (active: Someone referred to the article.)
Aer the direct, indirect, and prepositional objects are passivized, they of course func-
tion as subjects.
Not yet mentioned above is that objects of phrasal verbs can also be passivized, as
expected if they are objects, as can objects of complex transitives. Respective examples
are (43) and (44), where the scandal and that math problem are now the subjects:
(43) e scandal was covered up immediately.
(active: Someone covered up the scandal immediately.)
(44) at math problem is considered unsolvable by many great minds.
(active: Many great minds consider that math problem unsolvable.)
In (40) to (43), I have le the original subject unspecied (hence the ‘someone’ in the
active); in (44), I have added by many great minds. Many great minds is the subject of
the active sentence, but is optional in the passive. Hence, I would argue that the func-
tion of by many great minds is adverbial.
e NPs in adverbials, subject predicates, and object predicates cannot be passiv-
ized, as is shown for adverbials in (45) and object predicates in (46):
(45) *e meeting was slept during.
(46) *e chair was elected him. (passivized from the active We elected him (to be) chair)
As expected, the direct object in (46) can be passivized namely as He was elected
the chair.
In the previous chapter, we discussed intransitive verbs such as sleep, sneeze, go,
and swim. Now that we know there are PP objects as well as PP adverbials, how can
we tell which is which using the criteria from Table 5.2, e.g. in sentences such as (47)
and (48):
(47) I went [to the library].
(48) I swam [in the pool].
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
Some speakers regard the information contained in the PP as essential and others
consider it less so. If the goal of the going is seen as obligatory in (47), one might call
the PP an object, a prepositional object in this case; if the goal is seen as optional, the
PP would be an adverbial. Hence, for sentences such as (47) and (48), there are two
dierent analyses: the verbs can be intransitive ones with the PPs functioning as adver-
bials or the verbs can be prepositional ones with the PPs functioning as prepositional
objects. Notice that these sentences dier as to whether or not they can be passivized,
as shown in (49) and (50):
(49) *e library was gone to.
(50) ?e pool was swum in.
e results of passivization provided in (49) and (50) make the adverbial analysis plau-
sible for (47) and the object analysis for (48). ose of you for whom (49) and/or (50)
are ok consider both or one of the adverbials more like objects.
It could be that (50) sounds awkward because speakers feel ill at ease with the par-
ticiple of the verb swim. Let’s therefore try two other sentences and their passives:
(51) He walked on the grass.
(52) Washington slept in this bed.
(53) e grass was walked on.
(54) is bed was slept in.
Sentences (53) and (54) provide evidence that the grass and this bed are real objects in
(51) and (52).
Two other frequently asked questions are (a) how the object predicate, as in (55)
and (56), repeated from Chapter 4, diers from a modier to a noun, e.g. from Mars
in (57), or (b) from an adverbial in (58). I have indicated the most likely analyses by
means of brackets:
(55) Jane considers [Pride and Prejudice] [a classic].
(56) She put [snails] [on the table].
(57) I saw [a man from Mars].
(58) I saw [a man] [in the garden].
e answer is that, in (57), from Mars forms part of the direct object (as indicated by
the brackets) which can be replaced by a single element, as in (59). In a sentence such
as (56), on the table is not part of the direct object since they cannot both be replaced
by one element as the ungrammatical (60) shows:
(59) I saw him.
(60) *She put it.
e same is true for (55), since (61) has quite a dierent meaning than (55):
(61) Jane considers it.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
. Conclusion
As a conclusion, I list instances of the eight types of verbs we have discussed in Chapters
4 and 5:
Table 5.3. Verb types and their complements
Name Example Complement Sentence
intransitive swim, arrive She arrived (early).
(mono)transitive see, eat, read, love Direct Object She saw me.
ditransitive give, tell Direct and Indirect Object I gave him owers.
copula be, become Subject Predicate She is nice.
complex transitive consider, know Direct Object and Object
Predicate
I consider her nice.
prepositional refer, glance Prepositional Object He glanced at the book
phrasal switch on/give in Direct Object/ – She turned o the light
He gave in.
(phrasal prepositional get down to Prepositional Object He got down to business).
Typically, the direct and indirect objects are realized as NPs and the subject and
object predicates as AdjPs, but as was indicated above, there are other possibilities.
e prepositional object is always a PP, but the reverse is not true since PPs can have
many functions.
Adverbials are not relevant for the classication of verbs since they can always be
added or deleted. As mentioned above, they are typically realized as PPs and AdvPs
even though NPs and clauses are also possible. As an addition to Figure 4.1 where the
functions of NP, VP, and AdjP are given, Figure 5.3 does the same for PP and AdvP:
Indirect Object (give it [to him])
Subject Predicate (is [in the garden]) PP Prepositional Object (insist [on it])
Object Predicate (put it [in the car]) Adverbial (swim [during the day])
AdvP Adverbial (swim [carefully])
Figure 5.3. e functions of PPs and AdvPs
Passives are relevant since they allow us to nd direct, indirect, prepositional, and
phrasal objects. Aer being passivized, these objects of course function as subjects.
Key terms are adverbial, prepositional verb, and phrasal verb. Don’t worry too
much about phrasal prepositional verbs. Also relevant are VP- and S-adverbials.
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
Exercises
A. Identify all the functions in (62) to (67). Draw trees for (62) and (65):
(62) Fortunately, she found it easily.
(63) I separated it carefully.
(64) She found it easy.
(65) He baked Zoya bread last night (i.e. he baked it for her).
(66) Wisely, the pig from Mars left relatively early.
(67) The hard-working students seemed exhausted after three weeks of classes.
B. In the short text below, identify the underlined verbs (e.g. intransitive, complex transitive)
and the function of the phrases in brackets:
I have [a shocking news item]. [This little-known tidbit] will stun some of you and put the
rest [in a catatonic haze]. This is why I am warning you to brace yourselves. The Olympics
are going on [right now].
(adapted from a piece by Steve Galindo in the State Press)
C.
Do the same in the text below. How would you analyze deal in the last sentence?:
Underground nitrogen leak shuts down roads in the city
A worker from T&T Construction punctured [a high-pressure nitrogen line] at about 7 a.m.,
shutting down [trac] on the northeast side of town [all day]. The Police Department
blocked o a large area because [it] didn’t [initially] know what was leaking. “[It] could
have been natural gas, so we had to be [careful], [Tena Ray, a spokesperson], said.
“Fortunately, we don’t have to deal with things like this very often.
(adapted from a piece by Michelle Beaver in the State Press)
D.
Underline the PPs in the text below, and say which ones are adverbials.
In Rapid City, S.D., a bualo escaped from an auction and ended up in a dressing room. It
spent a couple of hours staring into a mirror at the Rushmore Plaza Civic Center. The bualo
jumped over a steel panel during the Black Hills Stock Show & Rodeo on Sunday morning,
went down an alley and got into the dressing room reserved for visiting sports teams, said
Brian Maliske, the civic center’s general manager. The door happened to be unlocked and he
pushed the door open and went in, Maliske said. The crew conducting the Black Hills Classic
Bualo Sale decided to keep the animal locked in the dressing room for the rest of the auc-
tion. During its two hour stay, it reportedly became fascinated with the image it saw in a big
mirror. When the sale ended, a rodeo crew member coaxed the bualo out and into captivity.
(adapted from the East Valley Tribune, January 2005)
E.
Take a verb and combine it with dierent prepositions and explain what kind of verb is the
result. For instance, take sleep and combine it with in, during, o, around, over, or outside.
F.
Make a sentence containing the verb complain about. What kind of verb is it? Do the
same with resort to, comment on and catch up with.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
G. Explain the ambiguity in the cartoon in Figure 5.4.
Figure 5.4. Glasses. (Used with the permission of the Thaves and the Cartoonist Group.
All rights reserved)
H.
How would you describe the dierence between to visit with somebody’ and to visit
somebody’? Speakers of English use both. What would you say?
I.
Find the adverbials in the text below (adapted from an Amnesty International document).
How are they realized, i.e. what kind of phrases are they? Be careful not to list the phrases
that modify nouns (of the failure of justice) or adjectives. This is a dicult text, I found, so
don’t get discouraged!
Human Rights and the Punjab
The organization provides a number of instances of the failure of justice in this report.
The government authorities have failed to address the problem of disappearances’
in Punjab. The government has not responded to any of the cases documented since
December 1993. The practice of ignoring petitions continues.
The Supreme Court found the police guilty of abducting and killing people but grave
concerns remain unaddressed. The report expresses concern about recent allegations
in the press that hundreds of people have been killed in Punjab. Continuing allegations
of disappearances are indicative of the absence of a serious commitment by the state
authorities.
J.
Are there prepositional objects in the text of B? Are there phrasal verbs?
K. (Dicult) Please circle and label (transitive etc.) all lexical verbs in the text below.
Streets of Athens
My friends next door are some of the few Athenians who have not moved into a concrete
block or sold local building contractors their family home for a handsome prot. The
sprawling morass of concrete that spills into the Attica Plain surrounding the suburbs
of Athens makes Europes southernmost capital one of the world’s most polluted cities.
(adapted from The Guardian, June 1990)
Then, bracket and label all the direct/indirect objects, subject/object predicates, and
adverbials of the text in K.
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
Class discussion
L. What do (68) and (69) tell you about the type of verbs switch on and look up are
respectively? One way to look at this is to focus on the light being preposed in (68)
without taking on along. Up the word is not a unit in (69) since it cannot be coordinated
with a similar unit:
(68) It was the light he switched on.
(69) *I looked up the word and up the quote.
M. In connection with phrasal verbs, we discussed intransitive phrasal verbs, such as take o
and give in, repeated here as (70) and (71). Take o can also be a transitive phrasal verb,
as in (72):
(70) His career took o.
(71) They nally gave in.
(72) She took o her glasses.
How would you analyze (73)? If you looked up away in a dictionary, it would tell you it
derives from the PP on way and is now an adverb or preposition, just like o and in:
(73) He went away.
One of the ways to solve this is to see if you can question where he went. If you could, that
would mean away would be an adverb, and not a particle. It is not a likely P since it has no object
(which we suggested in Chapter 2 was a way to dierentiate between prepositions on the one
hand and adverbs and particles on the other).
N.
A garbage collection company used (74) as one of its slogans. Explain the ambiguity in
terms of verb type:
(74) Our business is picking up.
O. Identify the prepositions and particles in Figures 5.5 and 5.6.
Figure 5.5. Put o until after
(The Born Loser: © Newspaper Enterprise Association, Inc.)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Figure 5.6. Back up over
(Family Circus © 2005 Bil Keane, Inc. King features Syndicate)
P. In the text in tree (20), I provide a tree for the phrasal verb when the object follows
the verb and particle. Discuss some possible trees for sentences where the object
intervenes between the verb and the particle.
Keys to the exercises
A. (62) Adv-ial Su Pred do Adv-ial
(63) Su Pred Do Adv-ial
(64) Su Pred Do ObjPr
(65) Su Pred Io Do Adv-ial
(66) Adv-ial Su Pred Adv-ial
(67) Su Pred SuPred Adv-ial
(62)
(It doesn’t matter whether or not you connect AdvP and
Adv with a line, as (62) shows)
S
AdvP S
Adv
Fortunately NP VP
She
V AdvP
VNP Adv
found it easily
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
(65)
S
NP
VP
He
V NP
VNPNPD N
baked Zoya N last night
bread
B. I have: transitive [a shocking news item: DO]. [This little-known tidbit: SU] will stun:
transitive some of you and put: complex transitive the rest [in a catatonic haze: ObjPr].
This is: copula why I am warning: transitive you to brace: transitive yourselves. The
Olympics are going on: intransitive phrasal [right now: Adverbial].
C. A worker from T&T Construction punctured: transitive [a high-pressure nitrogen line: DO]
at about 7 a.m., shutting down: (transitive) phrasal [trac: DO] on the northeast side
of town [all day: adverbial]. The Police Department blocked o: (transitive) phrasal a
large area because [it: Su] didn’t [initially: Adverbial] know: transitive what was leaking:
intransitive. “[It: Su] could have been: copula natural gas, so we had to be [careful: SuPr],
[Tena Ray, a spokesperson: Su (appositive)], said. “Fortunately, we don’t have to deal
with things like this very often”.
Deal is a verb taking a prepositional object.
D. In Rapid City, S.D., a bualo escaped from an auction and ended up in a dressing room.
It spent a couple of hours staring into a mirror at the Rushmore Plaza Civic Center. The
bualo jumped over a steel panel during the Black Hills Stock Show & Rodeo on Sunday
morning, went down an alley and got into the dressing room reserved [for visiting sports
teams], said Brian Maliske, the civic center’s general manager. The door happened to be
unlocked and he pushed the door open and went in, Maliske said. The crew conducting
the Black Hills Classic Bualo Sale decided to keep the animal locked in the dressing room
for the rest [of the auction]. During its two hour stay, it reportedly became fascinated with
the image it saw in a big mirror. When the sale ended, a rodeo crew member coaxed the
bualo out and into captivity. The adverbials are all of the underlined or bracketed PPs
EXCEPT of hours (goes with an N couple) and of the auction (goes with the N rest).
E. Sleep in would be an intransitive phrasal, with in a particle, since in does not have it
original (locational) meaning. Sleep around is similar. Sleep o is a transitive phrasal since
one can say sleep o a hangover and sleep it o; o is a particle, because you would have
to look up the meaning in a dictionary. Sleep during consists of an intransitive verb sleep
and a preposition during. During is not a particle since sleep it during is ungrammatical,
and it is not an adverb since it cannot occur independently of an NP, as in I slept during.
Sleep outside contains an intransitive verb sleep and an adverb outside.
F. She complained about the government; He resorted to violence; They commented on
the book. They are all prepositional object verbs.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
A sentence with the phrasal prepositional verb catch up with’ is: They caught up
with him.
G. The question ‘How do they look?’ contains a copula verb, but the joke is that look can also
be used in another way, namely as a prepositional verb. In this case, ‘How do they see?’
plays on that meaning.
H. To visit with somebody is said to be American English, whereas to visit somebody
is said to be British English. The dierence is that in the former case, visit with is a
prepositional verb, whereas in the latter case, visit is a (mono)transitive verb.
I. Adverbials in the rst paragraph: in this report: PP and since December 1993: PP; in the
second paragraph: in Punjab: PP.
J. Respond could be argued to be prepositional; no phrasal ones.
K. My friends next door
are
some of the few Athenians who have not
moved
into a
concrete block or
sold
local building contractors their family home for a handsome
prot. The sprawling morass of concrete that
spills
into the Attica Plain
surrounding
the suburbs of Athens
makes
Europes southernmost capital one of the world’s most
polluted cities.
The names of the verbs are: copula, intransitive or prepositional verb, ditransitive,
intransitive (possibly prepositional), transitive, and complex transitive.
The minimum you can do is as follows but that doesn’t catch all complements:
My friends next door are SuPr [some of the few Athenians who have not moved into
a concrete block or sold local building contractors their family home for a handsome
prot]. The sprawling morass of concrete that spills into the Attica Plain surrounding the
suburbs of Athens makes DO [Europes southernmost capital] ObjPr [one of the world’s
most polluted cities]. You could look inside these complements, as in:
[some of the few Athenians who have not moved Adv-ial or PO [into a concrete block]
or sold IO [local building contractors] DO [their family home] Adv-ial [for a handsome
prot]]. The sprawling morass of concrete that spills Adv-ial [into the Attica Plain
surrounding DO [the suburbs of Athens]].
Special topic: The passive and dummies’
I consider the use or non-use of the passive a matter of style not of grammar (not even an
issue of prescriptive grammar). In certain kinds of writing, the use of the passive can have
advantages and, in others, it is better to be direct and to use the active. Sometimes, it is
irrelevant to know who performed the action and then the passive is more appropriate.
Let me start o with some quotes against the use of the passive. George Orwell, in his
1946 essay ‘Politics and the English Language, is perhaps the strictest:
(70) “Never use the passive where you can use the active.
Chapter 5. More functions, of prepositions and particles 
Orwell was of course worried about political propaganda. Style books include similar statements:
(71) “Use the active voice. The active voice is usually more direct and vigorous than the
passive (Strunk The Elements of Style 1918; later Strunk & White 1959[2000]).
The Princeton Writing Center cautions against the use of the passive as recently as 2009 though
it doesn’t suggest getting rid of it altogether:
(72) “Remember: to use the passive voice eectively, use it sparingly. Otherwise, your
writing may well evince the absurdity of this famous example …
`It was midday. The bus was being got into by passengers. They were being
squashed together. A hat was being worn on the head of a young gentleman ....
(http://web.princeton.edu/sites/writing/Writing_Center/handouts/html/
passivevoice.htm)
The passive has of course been in the English language since its beginning, as (73) shows, with
the passive auxiliary and verb in bold:
(73) Ða wæs gylden hilt gamelum rince
then was golden hilt old man
harum hild-fruman on hand gyfen
grey war-chief in hand given
‘Then was the golden hilt given into the old mans, the grey warriors, hand.
(Beowulf 1677–78)
Since passives use the auxiliary ‘to be, rules such as (74) seem to include them:
(74) Avoid the verb ‘to be’. “One of the most common stylistic mistakes aspiring writers
make is to rely too much on the verb to be. To be” is the most basic verb in the
English language, and writers can all too easily nd themselves using it in almost
every sentence. (http://www.essayforum.com/13_5678_0.html)
I think most of the time the copula ‘to be’ is meant when this rule is stated, but this rule may
also contribute to the unpopularity of the passive.
Finally, the dummy subjects ‘there and ‘it’, also called pleonastics or expletives, are
cautioned against by many style guides, as in the following quote from the Purdue University
writing center:
(75) Avoid overusing expletives at the beginning of sentences.
(http://owl.english.purdue.edu/owl/resource/572/04).
Many writers use these of course. Adam Smiths Wealth of Nations, a long text, contains
596 instances of there, many at the beginning of the sentence, and 4676 instances of
is. Charles Dickens’ Bleak House, a much shorter text, has 173 occurrences of there and
413 of is.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
As with the passive, the frequent use of to be, it, and there is really a matter of style. Some
authors like them, as reading the following adaptation from Ernest Hemingway’s Hills Like White
Elephants shows. I have put the forms of to be and the dummy subjects in bold:
(76) The hills across the valley of the Ebro were long and white. On this side there was
no shade and no trees and the station was between two lines of rails in the sun.
Close against the side of the station there was the warm shadow of the building
and a curtain, made of strings of bamboo beads, hung across the open door into
the bar, to keep out ies. The American and the girl with him sat at a table in
the shade, outside the building. It was very hot and the express from Barcelona
would come in forty minutes. It stopped at this junction for two minutes and went
on to Madrid.
Chapter 6
The structure of the verb group (VGP)
in the VP
1. Auxiliary verbs
2. The ve types of auxiliaries in English
3. Auxiliaries, ax hop, and the verbgroup (VGP)
4. Finiteness
5. Relating the terms for verbs (optional)
6. Conclusion
In this chapter, the verbs that can appear together in a VP are discussed in more detail.
Most of the sentences we have talked about so far have contained one (nite and lexical)
verb. In English, a VP can (in principle) have four auxiliary verbs and one lexical verb.
English is quite unusual in this respect, compared to other languages that typically do
not have this many auxiliaries. is complex of auxiliaries and the lexical verb will be
called the Verb Group, abbreviated in the tree as VGP (and used when auxiliaries are
present). English is also unusual in that if an auxiliary is not present and the sentence
is negative or a question, a ‘dummy’ auxiliary do is needed.
In Section 1, the auxiliaries are dened and characterized in general terms. In that
section, I also include auxiliaries as part of the (at, non-hierarchical) Verb Group.
We will label the auxiliaries as modal, perfect, progressive, passive, and dummy in
Section 2. Auxiliaries are associated with a particular ending, i.e. ax, that appears on
the verb immediately to their right. is process is called ax-hop and is discussed in
Section 3. Section 4 provides rules for identifying nite verbs and for distinguishing
them from non-nite ones. Section 5 is an optional section that reviews the terminol-
ogy that is relevant to verbs and tries to justify the dierent classications.
.
Auxiliary verbs
Verbs can be divided into lexical and auxiliary verbs. A VP contains one lexical verb
and (optionally) up to four auxiliaries. Most of the VPs dealt with in the previous
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
chapters consist of a single verb, and then they automatically are lexical verbs. Lexical
verbs can be further divided into intransitive arrive, walk, copula be, transitive see,
eat, etc, as we’ve seen. ese verbs carry a real meaning and are not dependent on
another verb. In addition to a lexical verb, the VP may contain auxiliaries which are
then grouped together with the lexical verb in a Verb Group. Auxiliaries depend on
another verb and add grammatical information. ey are divided into dierent kinds
in Section 2.
Auxiliaries are also sometimes called helping verbs since they help out other
verbs. For instance, in (1), has does not mean ‘possess’; it merely indicates that the
action of the lexical verb see was in the past. In (2), on the other hand, have has a lexi-
cal meaning (‘to possess’) and there is no other lexical verb present. Its classication is
transitive since it has a direct object (a book on sentences):
(1) e Malacandran has seen the hross. (perfect auxiliary have with lexical verb see)
(2) I have a book on sentences. (transitive verb have)
Unlike lexical verbs, auxiliaries invert in questions, as in (3), can precede the negative
n’t (i.e. the common form of not), as in (4), can be used in tag questions, as in (5), and
can be used to emphasize that the action did indeed take place, as in (6):
(3) Has she gone yet? (perfect auxiliary have)
(4) She hasn’t done that yet. (perfect auxiliary have)
(5) She hasn’t done that yet, has she? (perfect auxiliary have)
(6) She HAS actually said that! (emphatic auxiliary)
If n’t/not appears, as in (4) and (5), this adverb will be included in the Verb Group as
well and will be abbreviated as neg’ to save space in the tree. Table 6.1 provides some
ways to recognize auxiliaries.
Table 6.1. Characteristics of auxiliary verb
a. ey must be used with a lexical verb (unless the verb is elided).
b. ey have little meaning; rather, they express tense, mood, and aspect.
c. ey invert in questions, as in (3).
d. ey occur before n’t , as in (4).
e. ey are used in tags, as in (5).
f. ey are used for emphasis, as in (6).
e Verb Group will be represented as a at tree structure, as in (7). As men-
tioned in Chapter 3, grammatical categories such as the auxiliary do not head their
own phrase (in this book) and hence do not function at sentence level. Grammatical
categories function inside a phrase or, in this case, as heads inside the Verb Group (if
auxiliaries are present, VGP will be used; otherwise, V will suce):
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
(7)
VP
VGPNP
modal pf progr VD N
may have been seeing the hross
I use abbreviations for the auxiliaries for practical reasons: the full names are hard to t
in the tree otherwise. Lets now look at each of the auxiliaries in more detail.
.
The ve types of auxiliaries in English
In this section, well discuss the auxiliaries as they appear if all are present in a sen-
tence, namely modal, perfect (pf), progressive (progr), and passive (pass). When no
auxiliaries are present, we need do in certain cases.
.
Modals
English is exceptional in the numbers of auxiliaries it has and the combinations it
allows. Each auxiliary has its own name and position with regard to the others. Modals
do not have agreement or tense endings (hence *he cans; *I am canning to go); they are
the rst to occur in a sequence of auxiliaries; and do not require an ending, i.e. ax,
on the verb following them (He can walk, but not: *He can walked). us, (8a) and
(8b) are typical instances: the modals might and could are rst and the verb following
it does not have an ax:
(8) a. It might rain. (modal auxiliary might)
b. Rigobertha could be going tomorrow. (modal auxiliary could)
ere are nine modals in English: can, could, may, might, shall, should, will, would, and
must. Modal auxiliaries express uncertainty, as in (8), necessity (must, should), ability,
as in (9), or permission, as in (10):
(9) I can swim. (modal auxiliary can)
(10) You may go now. (modal auxiliary may)
Modals are also used where some languages would use the subjunctive mood. e
Modern English subjunctive is very restricted and examples are given in (11a) and
(12a). Alternatives using modals are provided in (11b) and (12b):
(11) a. ey insisted that he go. (subjunctive mood)
b. ey insisted that he should go.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(12) a. I wish it were Friday. (subjunctive mood)
b. I wish it would be Friday
Since subjunctives are not common in Modern English, I will not go into this more
deeply.
Modals are oen used when we ask a favor of someone, as in (13), or when
we want to be polite. e ‘pastform (could) in (13) is seen as more polite than the
present’ form (can) in (14). Modals have lost the ability to express present and past
tense, but they are nite. us, the dierence between (13) and (14) is not related to
when the action happened, but to how likely the event is to happen. Could is more
polite since it expresses a more remote possibility; can is more direct and hence seen
as less polite:
(13) Could I borrow some money? (modal auxiliary could)
(14) Can I borrow some money? (modal auxiliary can)
In English, the modal will (and shall in some varieties of English) is used to express
future, as in (15) and (16), the latter of which is the contracted form:
(15) He will go to Mars next year. (future expressed by will)
(16) She’ll walk on Jupiter in two years. (future expressed by ‘ll)
ere are special modals, called semi-modals: dare (to), need (to), have to, and ought
to. ey are seen as modals since they express obligation, ability, and necessity. Used
to is sometimes added to this group, but it is much more a regular auxiliary expressing
habituality. Semi-modals are in ux between auxiliary and lexical verb status. In (17),
T.S. Eliot does not invert dare in a question (see test (c) of Table 6.1) and it therefore
looks like a lexical verb, but in (18), acceptable for some speakers, dare is inverted and
more of an auxiliary:
(17) Shall I part my hair behind? Do I dare to eat a peach? (semi-modal dare)
(e Love Song of J.A.P, l 122)
(18) Dare I eat a peach? (semi-modal dare)
In English, only auxiliaries move, and if the sentence contains just a lexical verb,
a dummy do will be used (see Section 2.5). Since do is used in (17), it is usually
thought that dare in (17) is a lexical verb. e other semi-modals allow a variety of
constructions as well. For instance, ought in (19) is very much an auxiliary since it
moves, but in (20), it is not. Both occur in 19th century texts (see the Oxford English
Dictionary):
(19) How ought I address thee, how ought I revere thee? (semi-modal ought)
(Robert Browning, Agamemnon 796)
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
(20) You did n’t ought to have received ‘em. (semi-modal ought)
(Charles Dickens, Martin Chuzzlewit 34, 403)
. Perfect have (pf)
Have follows the modal if there is one. It is called the perfect auxiliary, and abbreviated
as ‘pf, though it does not make the meaning perfective or nished. It is used to indicate
that a past action still has relevance and that mixture of tense and aspect is called the
present perfect. For instance, in (21), the speaker still lives ‘here, whereas in the simple
past tense, as in (22), the speaker no longer does:
(21) I have lived here for ages. (perfect auxiliary have, used in present perfect)
(22) I lived here in the nineties. (simple past)
ere is currently a shi between British English and American English in that the
former prefers the present perfect, as in (23), for the recent past, whereas the latter
does the simple past, as in (24):
(23) Well I’ve, I’ve seen her today but she said shed er get me some socks and that…
(BNC – informal conversations)
(24) in fact, I saw him today at the airport (COCA – FOX TV)
When have is used, the verb following it is marked with an -ed ending (if it is regular),
e.g. lived in (21). e form of the verb that is the result of ‘ax-hopis called the past
participle, or -ed participle. Ax-hop is so called because the ax appears on the verb
to the immediate right of the auxiliary it goes with: the ax ‘hops’ onto the next verb.
In (25), the ending related to have appears on be, which is an irregular verb (like see,
go, do, etc.) and therefore has an -en ending:
(25) Zoltan may have been playing a terrible game. (perfect auxiliary have)
e term past participle is perhaps somewhat confusing since the presence of the past
participle does not make the entire sentence past tense. In fact, (21), (23), and (25) are
in the present tense, and hence the name present perfect. ere is a past perfect, as in
(26), with the form of the auxiliary have in the past but otherwise similar. Its meaning
is completion by a certain point in the past (in this case ‘by ve’):
(26) He had done it by ve. (perfect auxiliary had)
In Figure 6.1, I provide (simplied) timelines for the dierent tense and aspect
combinations, where S represents the time of speech and E the event. e present
tense is used when the time of the event and the utterance are the same; the past is
used when the event is at an earlier time; and the future is used when the event is at
a later time. e present perfect is used when the event (represented by the arrow)
started earlier but includes the time of speech and the past perfect is used when there
is a reference time in the past, such as ve oclock, and the event occurred before that
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
time. e future perfect has a time reference in the future by which time the event will
have occurred.
Timeline Example
Present ______S____ I know the answer
E
Past ____E___S___ I knew the answer yesterday
Future ____S___E___ He’ll do it
Present perfect ___E__S_________ I have lived here for ages
Past perfect __E_______S_____ He had done it by 5 a.m.
5 a.m.
Future perfect __S____E________ He’ll have done it by 5 a.m.
5 a.m.
Figure 6.1. Timelines for tense and aspect
e exact order of S and E doesn’t matter in the future perfect.
In Figure 6.2, we will list three progressives. If you are a native speaker, you know
how to use these, but you might want to know terms such as present and past perfect
since these are used. I don’t think that you need to memorize the terms in Tables 6.1
and 6.2. I just want you to have a familiarity with the terms.
.
Progressive be (progr)
e progressive, abbreviated as ‘progr, indicates that the action is or was in progress.
is is the aspect of a verb, as opposed to the tense of a verb which tells you whether
the action took place in the present, past, or future. In (27) to (29), the aspect is pro-
gressive, but the tense is present in (27), past in (28), and future in (29). Since the
progressive indicates that an action is or was in progress, it is incompatible with verbs
that express a state, as shown in (30) and (31):
(27) Zoya is walking. (progressive be)
(28) Zoltan was playing the piano, when a noise disturbed him. (progressive be)
(29) He will be walking the dog. (progressive be)
(30) *He is knowing the answer. (progressive be)
(31) *e book is being blue. (progressive be)
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
To form the progressive, a form of to be is used, as in (27). e verb that follows this
auxiliary has an -ing ending through ax-hop. It is called a present participle, or
ing-participle. Again, as in the case of past participles, the term is confusing since the
present participle need not make the sentence into the present tense, as (28) shows.
In Figure 6.1, I showed simplied timelines for some simple tenses and the per-
fects. Figure 6.2 adds timelines for progressives. e present progressive expresses that
the event is taking place for some time at the time of the utterance; the past progressive
that it was taking place for some period in the past; and the future progressive that an
event will be taking place in the future. A few more combinations can be added but I
will leave that for the class discussion section (see Exercise G, Figure 6.5).
Timeline Example
present progressive SI am working
E
past progressive E __S__ I was working
future progressive ___S__ EI will be workin
g
Figure 6.2. ree progressives
Some people argue that certain forms of be are not auxiliary verbs but lexical ones,
and that the -ing forms are adjectives. I mention it here as a possible analysis in some
cases. For instance in (27) above, one could argue walking is like nice, since like nice, it
can be used to modify a noun in (32):
(32) My nice walking shoes are very light. (present participle used as adjective)
My own feeling is that walking in (27), where it refers to an action, is very dierent
from walking in (32), where it describes the qualities of a noun. If we considered the
distinctions made in Chapter 2, walking would be a verb in (27), but an adjective in
(32). e same ambiguity occurs with passives, as will be shown next.
.
Passive be (pass)
As seen in Chapters 4 and 5, passive constructions, as in (33b), are made from active
ones as in (33a) by switching the subject and the object around and by adding a form
of to be. is passive auxiliary is abbreviated as ‘pass’ in the tree. e verb immediately
following this be has a past participle ending, in this case -en, because of the ax ‘hopping
from the auxiliary to the next verb:
(33) a. I see him. (active)
b. He is seen by me. (passive auxiliary be)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
e stylistic eects of passives were discussed in Chapter 5. Here, we just discuss the
form. In the active (34a), the Verb Group consists of a modal, a perfect, and a lexical
verb. Because of the perfect have, the form of the verb see is a past participle. In (34b),
the passive be is added and now its form is that of past participle (namely been) because
it follows have. Seen appears as past participle as well because it follows the passive be.
If this sounds too complex, just look at the ending of the verb on the immediate right
of the auxiliary and Table 6.3 below:
(34)
a. Zoya may have seen Zoltan. (modal may and perfect have)
b. Zoltan may have been seen by Zoya. (modal may, perfect have, and passive be)
Passive participles can oen be analyzed as adjectives (known, mixed, written) and are
then not part of the Verb Group. en, the form of be is not an auxiliary either, but a
copula. It is up to the reader to decide whether delighted in (35) is a passive participle
or an adjective. Most linguists would argue that (35) is not a passive construction since
(a) adding a by-phrase, as in (36), is awkward, and (b) delighted appears aer copula
verbs such as seem, as in (37), which is typical of adjectives:
(35) She was delighted to get chocolate. (copula be and adjective)
(36) *She was delighted by Edward to get chocolate. (by-NP not possible)
(37) She seemed delighted to get chocolate. (delighted aer seem)
e regular passive is constructed with the auxiliary be and that is the one you should
probably use in formal writing. ere is another passive auxiliary that I mention here
but wont include in the Table or summaries, namely get, as in (38). According to the
Oxford English Dictionary, the get-passive is rst used in 1652. It seems more forceful
than the be-passive:
(38) en he got knocked out. (passive auxiliary get)
(BNC, ction)
As well see in the next section, if there are two be auxiliaries in a row, the rst is the
progressive and the second one is the passive auxiliary. Note that the passive auxiliary
gets the ax of the preceding auxiliary through ax-hop, in this case that of the pro-
gressive. Missed is a past participle because of the preceding passive be:
(39) Treatable chronic liver disease may be being missed in primary care …
(google – medical site)
. The dummy’ do
Lexical verbs, such as know and think, cannot be used in questions and negative sen-
tences in Modern English, as (40a) and (41a) show for know. Instead, a dummy do is
used in (40b) and (41b):
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
(40) a. *Knows he the answer?
b. Does he know the answer?
(41) a. *He knows not the answer.
b. He doesn’t know the answer.
Do does not appear together with the other auxiliaries but is only inserted in questions,
as in (40b), or negative sentences with n’t/not, as in (41b), or for emphasis, as in (42):
(42) Oh, but I DID know the answer.
In earlier English, dummy do does not appear in this way. In Shakespeares time, for
instance, it is optional, as (43) to (45) indicate:
(43) Or if it were, it not belongs to you. (2 Henry IV, IV, i, 98)
(44) What meanes your Lordship? (Hamlet, III, i, 106)
(45) What does this meane my Lord? (Hamlet, I, vi, 7)
Leaving the do out has an archaic eect, or just a playful one, as in Figure 6.3.
Figure 6.3. I think not. (© 2008 Jan Eliot. Reprinted with the permission of Universal Press
Syndicate. All rights reserved.)
. Auxiliaries, ax hop, and the verbgroup (VGP)
e auxiliaries dealt with in the previous section occur in a particular order: modal,
perfect, progressive, and passive. Since dummy do only occurs if no other auxiliary
is present, I will ignore it here. As mentioned, the verb that immediately follows a
particular auxiliary bears the ending, also called ax, of that auxiliary. Since the
ax associated with a particular auxiliary does not appear on the auxiliary but on
the next verb, this process is called ax-hop. e auxiliaries and lexical verb go
together in a verbgroup or VGP. As a summary of the auxiliaries and axes, I list
them in the table below.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Table 6.2. Auxiliaries and their axes
Name of AUX AUX affix on the
next verb
sentence name of verb
with affix
modal: may, might, can,
could, etc
He may go innitive
perfect: have -ed/-en ey have walked
/seen/gone
past participle
progressive: be -ing I am going present participle
passive: be -ed/-en ey are loved/seen past participle
A sentence that includes all four types of auxiliaries sounds a little contrived. Note
the strict order (e.g. have may would be ungrammatical:
(46) e woman made a 911 call from the trunk of a car [that] police believe she may
have been being held in. (google – local news site)
In (46), there is a modal may, a perfect have, a progressive be marked with -en because of
have, a passive be marked with -ing because of the progressive immediately to its le, and
a lexical verb held that bears the ax of the passive auxiliary immediately to its le.
As shown in (7) above, the structure of a sentence with a number of auxiliaries is
not very insightful, i.e. it is very at, since all the auxiliaries are part of the Verb Group.
e negative adverb not in English must be included in the Verb Group as well since it
is an ax on the nite auxiliary. A structure for (47) is (48):
(47) He hasn’t been doing his homework.
(48)
S
NP VP
He
VGPNP
pf-neg progr VD N
hasn’t been doing hishomewor
k
Other structures have been suggested with a less at structure but they are still contro-
versial and would lead us into a new set of arguments.
.
Finiteness
e sentences we have discussed so far have been complete sentences, not sentence frag-
ments. A complete sentence consists of a subject and a nite verb. A nite verb in English
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
agrees with the subject (in the present tense) and indicates present or past. Its subject
has nominative case, which can only be seen in the case of pronouns in Modern
English, i.e. the subject pronoun of nite verbs must be nominative I, you, he, she, it,
we and they, not accusative me, him, her, us or them (you and it are both nominative
and accusative).
Finite sentences have a Verb Group with a nite verb as its rst (or only) member.
In (49), have is the nite verb that makes the entire Verb Group nite. As a result, the
entire sentence is also nite:
(49) I [have been going] there frequently. (nite have in a VGP)
Have is nite because it shows subject agreement (have rather than has, as in (50)),
indicates present tense (have rather than had, as in (51)), and has a nominative subject
(I rather than me, as in the ungrammatical (52)):
(50) He has been going there frequently.
(51) He had been going there frequently.
(52) *Me have been going there frequently.
Note that in some varieties of English, sentences such as (52) are grammatical.
Modals, as in (53), are nite even though (for historical reasons) they never dis-
play subject-verb agreement:
(53) I might have done that.
Only finite sentences are complete sentences. Most of us, however, use fragments
in informal speech, in poetry, e.g. John Keats in the excerpt in (54), or even in
formal writing:
(54) Ode on a Grecian Urn
ou still unravishd bride of quietness!
ou foster-child of silence and slow time,
What mad pursuit? What struggle to escape?
What pipes and timbrels? What wild ecstacy?
Nevertheless, incomplete sentences are generally frowned upon in formal writing.
Sentence (55) below is not a complete sentence but is a sentence fragment. How can
it be xed?
(55) Mentioning that point about nite sentences yesterday.
Sentence (55) can become a full sentence by adding a subject and a nite verb as in (56):
(56) I was mentioning that point about nite sentences yesterday.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
As will be shown in a later chapter, non-nite sentences can only be part of other sen-
tences. It is always a good idea to count the number of lexical verbs. For instance, how
many lexical verbs are there in (57)?
(57) I have heard her sing too oen.
In (57), there are two lexical verbs, heard and sing, but only the rst Verb Group is
nite since have is nite (e.g. the subject of have is nominative I). e Verb Group that
sing is the sole member of is non-nite since its subject is accusative her.
Other sentences that include a non-nite Verb Group are (58) and (59), with the
non-nite Verb Groups in bold. Note that the innitive marker to is part of the Verb
Group, and is abbreviated as ‘inf ’:
(58) Seeing the ordinary as extraordinary is something we all like to do.
(59) She forgot to google them.
In (58), seeing, is, like, and do are lexical verbs, but only is and like are nite. In (59),
forgot and google are the lexical verbs, but only forgot is nite.
A sentence can contain many Verb Groups, a (potentially) indenite number
if, as mentioned in Chapter 1, the speaker had enough energy and could continue.
Sentences such as (60), containing more than one Verb Group, are discussed in
Chapters 7, 8, and 10:
(60) I noticed that she mentioned that he was saying that she should tell him …
Imperatives are used to order someone to do something. ey oen lack a subject, as
in (61), but this need not be the case, as (62) shows:
(61) Draw the trees for these sentences.
(62) You, draw trees for this.
Imperatives are complete sentences and not sentence fragments.
.
Relating the terms for verbs (optional)
In this section, I’ll review all the terms we have used for verbs. First, we will discuss
regular and irregular forms. ese are relevant when forming a past tense or using a
participle. When we use a participle, we may label its Verb Group as present, past, or
future perfect. en, we will see that the classication of verbs into e.g. transitive and
of auxiliaries into e.g. modal belongs to a dierent type of classication. Finally, we add
niteness to the mix. is is quite a lot! So just try to read this section and dont worry
too much about connecting all the terms.
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
We have briey talked about regular and irregular verbs (end of Section 2 of
Chapter 4). Regular verbs have a predictable form, as in I walk, you walk, s/he walks,
we walked, and they have walked. If you are a native speaker of English, you know that
third person singular present tense has -s, the past tense has -ed, and the past participle
has -ed too. If you invent a new verb, you will use these endings without hesitation.
Irregular verbs are unpredictable; they were once more regular but because of changes
in the language, they now need to be learned, as Figure 6.4 shows.
Figure 6.4. Drawed and drew. (Used with the permission of the Baby Blues Partnership and King
Features Syndicate in conjunction with the Cartoonist Group. All rights reserved.)
Some people make a distinction between weak verbs (such as walk), strong verbs
where the vowel changes in the past (such as draw), and irregular ones (such as go,
with its past went). Many others just distinguish regular (the weak ones) from irregular
verbs (the strong and irregular ones).
e ending is used to build the tense and aspect forms of English that we have
seen in Figures 6.1 and 6.2. us the present perfect uses a present tense auxiliary
(and have is irregular) and the past participle ending of another verb. e latter can be
regular, as in the case of walked, or irregular, as in the case of gone. I list a few of the
regular ones in (63a) and the irregular forms in (63b):
present past past participle
(63) a. walk(s) walked walked (regular)
love(s) loved loved
hike(s) hiked hiked
b. go(es) went, gone (irregular)
4
begin(s) began begun
sing(s) sang sung
write(s) wrote written
put(s) put put
4.  Consult a dictionary (or the internet) for longer lists of irregular verbs.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
I have suggested to not worry too much about the names of all the tense and aspect
combinations (see Figure 6.5 below for even more names).
Whether a verb is regular or irregular or used as past or present is not con-
nected to its classification as intransitive or transitive. We have divided verbs in
Chapters 4 and 5 into intransitive, transitive, ditransitive, copula, complex tran-
sitive, phrasal and prepositional verbs. These can be either regular or irregular
and most can be used in all the tense and aspect combinations discussed. Auxilia-
ries are irregular: modals only have one form, the progressive and passive auxiliary
be is the most irregular, as is shown in (64a), and so is perfect have, as shown
in (64b):
present past past participle
(64) a. am, is, are was, were been
b. has, have had had
e copula be and transitive have work the same way of course.
Finally, we have used nite and non-nite to label verbs. e present and past
forms of verbs (am, was, sings, etc) are always nite and the past participle (been) and
present participle (being) always non-nite. Sometimes, it is hard to distinguish the
simple past (e.g. had) from the past participle (e.g. had) and you have to look at the
sentence. Remember that as long as one nite verb is present, the entire VGP is nite,
as is the entire sentence.
.
Conclusion
In summary, this chapter has classified the different kinds of auxiliary verbs: the
modal, perfect, progressive, and passive which occur in this order; do is added in
questions and negative sentences when an auxiliary is not available. Several names
for tenses, such as the present perfect and past progressive, are also provided. I
would suggest that, unless you are going to teach them, to just ignore the names
and have some sense for these terms and what they mean on timelines (see also
Exercise G).
Finiteness is also discussed: a verb is nite if it agrees with the subject and if this
subject bears nominative case. Since nite verbs and their relationship to lexical and
auxiliary verbs are oen challenging, I end with a list of examples in Table 6.3. e clauses
are indicated by brackets but their use will not be explained till the next chapter. For now,
remember that if you have a lexical verb, there is a clause.
Notice that each sentence has at least one nite verb in Table 6.3.
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
Table 6.3. Some nite (in bold), lexical, and auxiliary verbs (underlined)
1. [ose people could have been goong o].
AUX AUX AUX lexical
modal perfect progressive phrasal
2. [He has been wanting [to go there for ages]].
AUX AUX lexical lexical
perfect progressive transitive intransitive
3. [I mentioned [that it had been said [that she wished [to leave]]]].
lexical AUX AUX lexical lexical lexical
transitive perfect passive transitive transitive intransitive
4. [I saw [him giving her a present]].
lexical lexical
transitive ditransitive
5. [[Feeling ne], he le early [to put dinner on the stove]].
lexical lexial lexical
copula intransitive complex transitive
Key terms are auxiliary and lexical verb; affix; participle; modal, perfect, progressive,
and passive; regular and irregular verbs; finite and non-finite; nominative case, and
tense.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Exercises
A. Identify the auxiliary/ies in (65) to (68), e.g. are they passive, or modal? List the nite
verbs as well:
(65) Rigobertha has been meeting Carlos.
(66) Belo and Horta were awarded the Nobel Peace Prize.
(67) Indonesia was not too happy with the decision.
(68) They may be bringing about a peaceful solution in East Timor.
B. Identify the auxiliaries (e.g. modal, passive) in the passage from Chapter 2, repeated here.
Again, list the nite verbs as well:
Granny was waiting at the door of the apartment. She looked small, lonely, and patient,
and at the sight of her the children and their mother felt instantly guilty. Instead of
driving straight home from the airport, they had stopped outside Nice for ice cream.
They might have known how much those extra twenty minutes would mean to Granny.
C.
Think up a sentence with a perfect and a passive auxiliary.
D. Add a progressive auxiliary to: He might go. Now add a perfect as well.
Take out the perfect in He could have been going.
E.
Read the two poems below. Then, compare the use of the verbs: lexical as opposed to
auxiliary, and nite as opposed to non-nite. What is the eect of this dierent verb use?
As the cat Fire and Ice
climbed over
the top of Some say the world will end in re,
Some say in ice.
the jamcloset From what I’ve tasted of desire
rst the right I hold with those who favor re.
forefoot
But if it had to perish twice,
carefully I think I know enough of hate
then the hind To say that for destruction ice
stepped down Is also great
And would suce.
into the pit of
the empty Robert Frost (1874–1963); www.bartleby.com
owerpot
william carlos williams (1883–1963)
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
Optional (Section 5)
F. In the following list of verbs, identify the irregular ones:
wait, become, see, look, take, lead, grow, hang, light, run, garden, paint, and drive.
Class discussion
G. In Sections 2.2 and 2.3, I have given simplied timelines to the dierent tenses and
aspects. There are a few more possibilities that I have added together with the previous
ones in Figure 6.5. Discuss these in class and perhaps draw some of your own timelines.
H.
If you have access to the internet in class, search for some sites with irregular verbs. They
vary enormously in number. What is the largest number you can nd.
Timeline Example
Present ______S______I know the answer
E
Past ____E___S_____I knew the answer yesterday
Future ____S___E____ He’ll do it
present progressive ___ S ___ I am working
E
past progressiveE__S__I was working
future progressive ___S__ EI will be working
Present perfect ___E__S_________I have lived here for ages
Past perfect __E_______S_____He had done it by five
5 a.m.
Future perfect __S____E________He’ll have done it by 5 a.m.
5 a.m.
Progressive past perfect __E______S__ I had been working up to that point
Progressive future perfect ___S__E_____ I will have been working by then
Progressive present perfect __E__S_____ I have been working
Figure 6.5. Timelines for tense and aspect (nal version)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Keys to the exercises
A. has (perfect) and been (progressive) in (65); were (passive) in (66); no auxiliaries in (67);
may (modal) and be (progressive) in (68).
Finite verbs: in (65), has; in (66), were; in (67), was; in (68), may.
B. Granny was (progressive) waiting at the door of the apartment. She looked small, lonely,
and patient, and at the sight of her the children and their mother felt instantly guilty. Instead
of driving straight home from the airport, they had (perfect) stopped outside Nice for ice
cream. They might (modal) have (perfect) known how much those extra twenty minutes
would (modal) mean to Granny.
Finite are: was, looked, felt, had, might, would
C. He has been seen.
D. He might be going.
He might have been going.
He could be going.
E. In the rst poem, there are 2 nite lexical verbs; in the second, there are 12 lexical and 4
auxiliary verbs, and only 1 Verb Group is non-nite. Note also that in the second poem,
a number of nouns are somewhat verbal, e.g. hate, destruction, re, and desire. They are
either based on a verb or can be used as a verb. Discuss the eects of the verb use on the
tone of the poems.
F. Irregular are: become, see, take, lead, grow, hang (can also be regular), light (can also be
regular), run, and drive. You can tell this by thinking of the past and participle forms,
e.g. become has became and become respectively.
Special topic: Reduction of have and the shape of participles
The prescriptive rule against contraction can be formulated as follows:
(69) In formal writing, do not contract auxiliaries and negatives.
Most people do not fully spell out the auxiliaries in speech or informal writing. Thus, have in (70)
becomes ‘ve or a, as in (71), or even of, as in (72):
(70) I should have done that sooner.
(71) Now, that’s someone who they shoulda kept out the sun.
(COCA-Fiction 2008)
(72) He should of said something.
(COCA-ction 1994)
Reduction of have is typically done by speakers when have is in fact an auxiliary as in (71) and
(72), not when it is a main verb, as in (73), formed from (74):
(73) *He shoulda books in his oce.
(74) He should have books in his oce.
Chapter 6. The structure of the verb group (VGP) in the VP 
Reduction of auxiliaries has occurred since medieval times. Sentences (75) to (79) are from
the 15th and 16th centuries, where the reduced forms of have are in bold:
(75) it xuld a be seyd
‘It should have been said’. (Paston Letters, #131 year 1449)
(76) 3e wold a be plesyd
‘You would have been pleased’. (Paston Letters, #176 year 1464)
(77) there xuld not a be do so mykele
‘There should not have been done so much. (Paston Letters, #205 year 1469)
(78) So would I ha done by yonder Sunne.
(Shakespeare, Hamlet, IV, 5, 65 First Folio Edition 1623)
(79) I know you ha practised vpon the easie-yeelding spirit of this woman.
(Shakespeare, 2 Henry 4, II, 1, 126)
Hence, even though the reduction of have to of and -a is common in speech
nowadays (and was common in writing in earlier times), it is ‘not done’ in formal
writing. Since perfect have is weakening, we nd sentences such as (80):
(80) I feel that American Express should of not have paid once they received my call
and emails of the merchandise not working. (google – complaints site)
As mentioned, the perfect auxiliary have and the passive auxiliary be are followed by a past
participle. This rule is often violated. Remember the discussion of lie and lay (with participles
as lain and laid respectively) in Chapter 4? Other instances are the past participles bitten
and gone. They are often replaced by the past tense, as in (81) and (82), but this use is not
prescriptively correct in Modern English even though it occurs in writers such as Milton, as in
(83), Dryden, Pope, Addison, and Swift:
(81) Some mosquito has bit me.
(82) I should have went to Medical School at the U of A. (overheard on ASU campus)
(83) According to his doom: he would have spoke,
But hiss for hiss returnd with forked tongue. (Milton, Paradise Lost, X, 517–8)
Two other comments can be made about the forms of the participle. First, in earlier stages
of English, the ax was often not present, as in (84) and (85). It is not clear what caused that
absence:
(84) What have I do? (Chaucer, Millers Tale, 3739)
(85) If I so ofte myghte have ywedded be. (Chaucer, Wife of Baths Prol. 7)
Secondly, the present participle is preceded by the auxiliary be, as we have seen in Section 2.3, but
in many varieties of English, this participle has a prex, as in (86). Most frequently, the verb
preceding it is go, as in (87) from an English folksong, or keep:
(86) He was a-working
(87) A Frog he went a-courting.
The a-prex is the remnant of an older on that marked the progressive.
Review of Chapters 4–6
I’ll start with Chapter 6. In this chapter, the Verb Group (VGP) is examined more care-
fully: a Verb Group contains at least a lexical verb but can also contain one or more
auxiliaries. Verbs (and Verb Groups) are either nite or non-nite. If verbs express
tense and have a nominative subject, they are nite; if not, they are non-nite. Some
verbs are irregular in form. at aects the shape of, for instance, their simple past and
present perfect.
In Chapters 4 and 5, functions at sentence level are discussed: subject, predicate,
direct object, indirect object, phrasal object, prepositional object, subject predicate, and
object predicate. ese are obligatory parts of the sentence. Verbs are classied in terms
of whether or not they have obligatory complements: intransitives do not but transi-
tives, ditransitives, copulas, complex transitives, phrasal and prepositional verbs do.
In contrast to complements, adverbials function to add background and can be
added to a sentence optionally and without limitation (except for the speaker’s and
hearers level of patience and memory). e dierence between direct object, indirect
object, phrasal object, and prepositional object on the one hand and subject predicate,
object predicate, and adverbial on the other is that the former can be passivized. As a
reminder, I’ll provide a list of the major verb types with a simple tree for each. Review
the Tables of Chapters 4 and 5 as well.
(1) Intransitive: no objects
S
NP VP
Manatees
V
swim
(2) (Mono)transitive: one direct object
S
NP VP
ey
VNP
ate
DN
an apple
Review of Chapters 4–6 
(3) Ditransitive: one direct and one indirect object
a. b. S
NP VP
Amy
VNPPP
gave N
toys PNP
to Chandra
S
NP VP
Sabina
VNPNP
told Timber N
stories
(4) Copula: one subject predicate
S
NP VP
Aster
V AdjP
is
Adv Adj
very nice
(5) Complex Predicate: one direct object and an object predicate
S
NP
VP
Soly
VSC (= small clause)
found
NP NP
it
DN
anuissance
(6) Prepositional object verb: one PP object
S
NP VP
She
VPP
insists
PNP
on
DN
an answer
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(7) Phrasal: no object if intransitive, as in (7a); one direct object if transitive, as in (7b).
a. b.S
NP VP
Jona
V
logged o
S
NP
VP
Hermione
VNP
turned o
DN
the light
Review of Chapters 4–6 
Examples of midterm exams covering Chapters 4 to 6
Example 1
A. In Text A, list (or underline) the lexical verbs and label them as e.g. transitive, complex
transitive, or phrasal verb.
B.
List the adverbials, subjects, and direct objects in paragraph 1 of Text A.
C. List the auxiliary verbs in paragraph 2. Are they modal, perfect, progressive, or passive?
D. Circle the nite verbs in the complete text.
E. Are any of the verbs irregular?
F. Draw a tree for:
(8) Spain was the target at the end of the century
Text A (adapted from: The Good Neighbor, by G. Black).
Ever since the US Civil War, the countries of Central America and the Caribbean have
occupied a special place in the American psyche. Cuba, Nicaragua, Panama and their
neighbors have been a magnet for adventurers and pioneers, a proving ground for grand
abstractions of democracy and freedom, and frequently they have given scoundrels a
refuge. For most of the twentieth century people knew them as “banana republics”; by
the 1980s, a chain of clothing stores serving auent customers in today’s travel-mad
world had adopted that name.
This was frontier territory, a land where the whim of the adventurer was often the
only law, where Americans had limitless prerogatives, and where people considered
outside intruders malicious. Senator Hannegan of Indiana saw something else. He saw
Britain hastening ‘with race-horse speed’ to seize all of Central America. Spain was
the target of similar suspicion at the end of the century, but was succeeded in turn by
Germany, Mexico and the Soviet Union. Each of these foreign powers was charged with
importing ideologies alien to the natural order of the region.
Example 2
A. Find all the lexical verbs and classify them (monotransitive, phrasal, etc.) in Text B, a text
from a few years ago, but in many respects still relevant.
B.
Find all the complements and classify them (direct object, indirect object, subject
predicate, etc.). How are they realized (NP, PP, AdvP, etc.)?
C.
Point out the auxiliary verbs and classify them. Also circle or list the nite verbs.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
D. Are result, kill, and carry regular or irregular verbs?
E.
Draw a tree for:
(9) Continued hostilities have resulted in terrible abuses inside Afghanistan.
Text B. Afghanistan troubles
Continued hostilities have resulted in catastrophic human rights abuses inside Afghanistan.
All warring factions have carried out attacks against residential areas. The factions have
targeted civilians. They have killed tens of thousands of people in various parts of the
country. The vast majority of the victims have been Kabul residents. Previous attacks
against Kabul stopped when the Taleban forces entered Kabul about ve months ago.
Now, Taleban has threatened a bombardment. This will leave many people dead and
many more wounded.
(adapted from an Amnesty International document)
Example 3
A. List or circle the nite verbs in the second paragraph of Text C.
B. List and identify the lexical verbs (transitive, intransitive, etc.) and the auxiliary verbs
(passive, perfect, etc.) in the rst paragraph.
C.
Draw a tree for (10). First, indicate the functions and names/labels by means of brackets.
(10) They met in Paris at the beginning of the 20th century.
D. What is the function and name/label (i.e. realization) of the following phrases in the
sentences in which they occur:
(11) a brilliant success (rst paragraph).
(12) Picassos arrival (second paragraph).
Text C (adapted from an article in Arizona State Universitys State Press)
Imagine if Steve Martin wrote a comedic concept play with the entirely possible idea that
Pablo Picasso and Albert Einstein could have met in Paris at the beginning of the twentieth
century in a small bistro. He has succeeded, and the Arizona Theatre Company’s production
of Picasso at the Lapin Agile is a brilliant success. Martin has created a hilarious and thought
provoking look at two geniuses.
The play begins with Einstein and several other patrons discussing the probability
that Picasso would venture into the bistro. Einstein is anticipating Picassos arrival.
The players discuss everything from physics to the letter ‘E. The play abounds with
Steve Martin’s bizarre philosophies and even stranger sense of humor.
Review of Chapters 4–6 
Questions that are not related to the text:
E. Explain (using terminology used in class and in Chapter 5) why the following sentence is
ungrammatical:
(13) *Down the president she ran.
F. Add passive auxiliaries to the following sentences (and make the appropriate changes):
(14) Picasso may have played a part.
(15) Einstein is looking at Picasso.
Key to example 1
A. The lexical verbs in the rst paragraph are occupied (transitive), been (copula), given
(ditransitive), knew (complex transitive), serving (transitive; this is tricky), adopted (transi-
tive) and in the second paragraph: was (copula), was (copula), had (transitive), considered
(complex transitive), saw (transitive), saw (transitive), hastening (intransitive), seize (transi-
tive), was (copula), succeeded (transitive, but here passivized), charged (complex transi-
tive, but passivized), and importing (transitive).
B. The adverbials are: Ever since the US Civil War, (in the American psyche), frequently, For most
of the twentieth century, by the 1980s, (and in today’s travel-mad world). The PPs in paren-
theses could also modify place and customers respectively.
The subjects are: the countries of Central America and the Caribbean, Cuba, Nicaragua,
Panama and their neighbors, they, people, and a chain of clothing stores serving auent cus-
tomers (in today’s travel-mad world). The direct objects are: a special place (in the Ameri-
can psyche), a refuge, them, and that name and the sole indirect object is scoundrels. The
only subject predicate is long: a magnet for adventurers and pioneers, a proving ground
for grand abstractions of democracy and freedom and the object predicate is: as “banana
republics”.
C. The auxiliaries are had (perfect), was (passive), and was (passive).
D. The nite verbs are: Ever since the US Civil War, the countries of Central America and the
Caribbean
have
occupied a special place in the American psyche. Cuba, Nicaragua,
Panama and their neighbors
have
been a magnet for adventurers and pioneers, a
proving ground for grand abstractions of democracy and freedom, and frequently they
have
given scoundrels a refuge. For most of the twentieth century people
knew
them as banana republics”; by the 1980s, a chain of clothing stores serving auent
customers in today’s travel-mad world
had
adopted that name.
This
was
frontier territory, a land where the whim of the adventurer
was
often the only law, where Americans
had
limitless prerogatives, and where people
considered
outside intruders malicious. Senator Hannegan of Indiana
saw
something else. He
saw
Britain hastening with race-horse speed’ to seize all of
Central America. Spain
was
the target of similar suspicion at the end of the
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
century, but
was
succeeded in turn by Germany, Mexico and the Soviet Union.
Each of these foreign powers
was
charged with importing ideologies alien to the
natural order of the region.
E. The irregular verbs are auxiliaries have (three times), had, was (twice), and copula
be (four times), the ditransitive give, the complex transitive know, the transitive see
(twice).
F.
S
NP VP
Spain
V PP
VNPPNP
was at
DN DNP
the target the
NPP
end
PNP
of
the century
DN
Key to example 2
A. resulted: prepositional verb; carried out: phrasal; targeted: transitive; killed: transitive;
been: copula; stopped: intransitive; entered: transitive; threatened: transitive; and leave:
complex transitive.
B. in catastrophic human rights abuses (inside Afghanistan): prepositional object/PP;
attacks against residential areas: phrasal object/NP; civilians: direct object/NP; tens of
thousands of people: direct object/NP; Kabul residents: subject predicate/NP; Kabul:
direct object/NP; a bombardment: direct object/NP; many people: direct object/NP;
dead: object predicate/AdjP; many more: direct object/NP; and wounded: object
predicate/AdjP.
C. Auxiliaries: ve instances of have (perfect), one has (perfect), and one will (modal). The nite
verbs are ve instances of have, one stopped, entered, has and will.
D. regular.
Review of Chapters 4–6 
E.
ORS
NP VP
AdjPNVGP
Adj hostilities
Continued
pf V
have resulted
PP
PNP
in
AdjP
N
Adj
terrible
NPP
abuses
PNP
inside Afghanistan
S
NP VP
V PP
VGP PP in Afgh.
pf Vin terrible abuses
have resulted
Key to example 3
A. The nite verbs are: begins, would, is, discuss, and abounds.
B. Imagine: transitive, wrote: transitive, met: (here) intransitive, succeeded: intransitive, is:
copula, and created: transitive.
C.
S
NP VP
T
hey
PP
PNP
at
DN
the
NPP
beginning
PNP
of
DN
the
AdjPN
Adj century
20th
V
VPP
met
P NP
in Paris
D. SuPr: NP; DO: NP
E. The run down in She ran him down is a phrasal verb and the particle cannot be separate
from the verb.
F. A part may have been played by Picasso; Picasso is being looked at by Einstein.
Chapter 7
Finite clauses
Embedded and coordinated
1. Sentences and clauses
2. The functions of clauses
3. The structure of the embedded clause:
The Complementizer Phrase (CP)
4. Coordinate sentences:
The Coordinator Phrase (CP)?
5. Terminological labyrinth and conclusion
So far, the sentences we have focussed on have included one lexical verb and one or
more auxiliaries. ese are simple sentences. is chapter gives examples of sentences
that include more than one lexical verb, which means that they are composed of more
than one clause. Sentences that are part of another sentence, i.e. that have a function in
that sentence, are oen referred to as embedded clauses, where the embedded clause is
seen as subordinate to the other.
ere are also coordinated clauses. ey involve at least two clauses (and two
lexical verbs) that are joined by and (or another coordinator) and both coordinated
clauses are of (almost) equal importance. Both embedded and coordinated construc-
tions enable us to make very long sentences (innite if we have the energy) and ones
we had never heard before.
Section 1 provides some examples of complex clauses and Section 2 discusses
functions, such as subject, object, and adverbial, as they relate to clauses. In Section 3,
I introduce the structure of the embedded clause, making use of the grammatical
category complementizer and the Complementizer Phrase (abbreviated as CP).
In Section 4, I do the same for coordinated clauses by using the coordinator to
form a Coordinator Phrase (also abbreviated as CP). Section 5 reviews some of the
terminology.
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
. Sentences and clauses
A simple clause contains one lexical verb. Hence, if there are two lexical verbs, there
are two clauses. For instance, in (1), the lexical verbs are noticed and like and hence,
there are two clauses: the main clause (I should have noticed that Zelda doesn’t like
Zoltan) and the embedded one (Zelda doesn’t like Zoltan). is can be indicated by
means of brackets:
(1) [I should have noticed [that Zelda doesn’t like Zoltan]].
In determining the clauses in a text, it will be helpful to rst identify the lexical verbs
and then to draw the brackets around the clauses.
e embedded clause in (1) is part of the main clause. We could split it up into two
clauses, as in (2), but that is awkward:
(2) I should have noticed it. Zelda doesnt like Zoltan.
Auxiliaries, such as should and does in (1) and (2), are not relevant for determining the
number of clauses or sentences; only lexical verbs are. How many main verbs are there
in the middle frame of Figure 7.1?
Figure 7.1. A pony (Used with the permission of the Baby Blues Partnership and King Features
Syndicate in conjunction with the Cartoonist Group. All rights reserved.)
If I counted right, there are ten. If you really want a pony, that may not be so many!
In a coordinated sentence, there are also two lexical verbs (or more, if more clauses
are coordinated or if one of them contains an embedded clause) but they are joined by
a coordinator. us, in (3), we have the main verbs arrived and ate and the coordina-
tor and:
(3) [e food arrived] and [they ate].
(COCA – ction 1995)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
As we’ll see, it is easier to divide (3) into two separate clauses than (1) and that is
because coordinated clauses have a looser connection.
Some linguists call the larger sentence in (1) the sentence or main clause and the
smaller sentence the embedded sentence, dependent, or subordinate clause. In Section 5, I
list some of these terms. I will use both clause and sentence interchangeably to indicate
a unit that contains a lexical verb. e complementizer that in (1) functions to link the
embedded sentence to the main clause, but can oen be le out in English. Try that in
(1). For a list of complementizers, look back to Chapter 2, Table 2.5.
In (1), both clauses have a VP containing a nite verb, i.e. should and does (remember
auxiliaries can be nite), but embedded sentences can be non-nite as well. In this chapter,
I discuss the clauses with nite VPs and in the next chapter those with non-nite VPs.
Be careful not to confuse nite verbs, such as should or does, with lexical verbs, such
as noticed and like: each clause must have a lexical verb, but each clause need not have
a nite verb.
.
The functions of clauses
As mentioned, embedded clauses function inside another clause as subject, direct or
phrasal object, subject predicate, or adverbial. For instance, in (1) above, the embedded
clause functions as direct object; in (4), it is a subject; in (5), a subject predicate; in
(6), a phrasal object; and in (7), an adverbial. e embedded clauses are indicated by
means of brackets here:
(4) [at she le] was nice. (embedded subject)
(5) e problem is [that she reads junk]. (embedded subject predicate)
(6) I gured out [that it didn’t work]. (embedded phrasal object)
(7) He read books [because it was required]. (embedded adverbial)
Embedded clauses do not function as indirect objects or as objects of prepositional
objects. ey do not function as object predicates either. Inside an NP or AdjP, clauses
function as modiers (e.g. relative clauses) or complements (e.g. noun complements).
Examples of relative and complement clauses will be given in Chapter 10.
Coordinated clauses have no function in another clause. ey are on an equal
footing, most argue, with each other. In (3), this means that you could even make them
into two independent clauses, as in (8), and although that sounds very choppy’, it is
still better than (2):
(8) e food arrived. ey ate.
We’ll now turn to the tree structures.
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
. The structure of the embedded clause: The Complementizer Phrase (CP)
As mentioned, embedded sentences have complementizers that connect the embedded
clause to another clause. ese complementizers are sisters to S and a sentence with
a complementizer is a Complementizer Phrase, abbreviated as CP. e CP, as in (9),
expresses that there is a sentence S that can be independent, i.e. occur on its own but,
when it functions in another sentence, it is glued to that sentence by the C. e C
determines the nature of the phrase above the C and S, namely the CP:
(9)
CP
CS
that
Unlike the S, the CP cannot appear on its own, since a sentence such as (10) is a fragment:
(10) at he went to the store.
Using CP, C, and S, a sentence such as (1), has a structure as in (11). I have slightly
simplied (1) by taking the auxiliaries out:
(11)
NP
S
VP
I
VCP
noticed
CS
that
NP VP
Zelda
VNP
likes Zoltan
Using a CP makes it possible to include the complementizer in the sentence and link
the embedded S to the main S. In (11), the embedded CP is the sister to noticed, which
means that it functions as the direct object to noticed.
ere are also embedded clauses that express questions. In these, the C position
can be occupied by if or by whether, as in (12). e CP here, as in (11), functions as a
direct object:
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(12)
S
NP VP
I
V
CP
asked
CS
whether
NP VP
he
her
VGPNP
had seen
pf V
Trees for a CP as subject and a subject predicate clause are given in (13) and (14)
respectively:
(13)
S
VP
V AdjP
was Adj
nice
CP
CS
at
NP VP
she V
le
(14)
S
NP VP
DN
V
CP
e problem is
C
S
that
NP VP
she
VNP
reads N
junk
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
Other examples of complementizers are because, before, aer, unless, and since. ese
particular complementizers are oen used to introduce adverbial clauses. An example
of an embedded adverbial is given in (15):
(15)
S
NP VP
He
V CP
VNPCS
readN because
books NP VP
it VGP
pass V
was required
e position of clauses functioning as adverbials, like that of non-clausal adverbials,
is very exible. For instance, in a sentence such as (15), the because-clause can also
precede he read books, as in (16):
(16) Because it was required, he read books.
We will assume that the tree structure for this is as in a sentence with an S-adverbial,
discussed in Chapter 5, namely, as in (17). However, other trees are possible:
(17)
S
CP
CS
Because
NP VP
it VGP
pass V
was required
S
NP VP
he
VNP
readN
books
Sentences containing embedded subjects, such as (4) above, are oen changed into
extraposed sentences, such as (18). e reason for the extraposition of the subject clause
is that speakers do not like to have embedded sentences in the beginning or middle of
the main clause. e dummy subject it takes the place of the extraposed clause:
(18) It was nice [that she le].
I'll refrain from drawing a tree here, but if you want to draw one, attach the extraposed
CP as if it were an S-adverbial.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Aer seeing a C and CP, some of you might have wondered if there is a C as well.
ere is, and we will briey mention this in Chapter 11. Until then, we will draw the CP
with just a C and S. Now, well continue with the structure of the coordinate clause.
.
Coordinate sentences: The Coordinator Phrase (CP)?
As in the case of coordinate phrases (discussed in Chapter 3), there is a debate over
how best to represent coordinate sentences, such as (3) above. I think (3) is similar to
(19) in that there is a connection between the two clauses. In (20), on the other hand,
the two clauses have no causal relationship (at least not one obvious to me):
(19) [She arrived] and [he le].
(20) [Phoenix is a city in Arizona] and [the moon is made of cheese].
We could argue that sentences that are more closely connected have a coordinator and
that really means and then. en, the second clause is subordinate to the rst and the
structure of (19) would be similar to the adverbial clause in (15) above. I have repre-
sented that as (21), where and he le would function as an adverbial to the main clause:
(21)
S
NP VP
She
V
V
arrived
CP
CS
and
NP VP
he V
le
In (20), neither clause is subordinate to the other. is could be represented as (22):
(22)
S
C
and
S
NP VP
DNVGPPP
the moon
is made PNP
of N
cheese
S
NP VP
Phoenix
VNP
is
DN
a
NPP
city
PNP
in Arizona
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
I leave it to the reader to decide whether (21) or (22) is more appropriate for (19) and
(20) (see Chapter 3, Section 4, for more arguments).
Coordinated structures, especially the ones with a closer connection between the
clauses, oen leave out the second subject if it is identical to the rst, as in (23). is is
analyzed as ellipsis, i.e. deletion, of the second subject. In a tree, you could leave this as
a blank space. It is possible to repeat the subject, as in (24), or use a pronoun but this
is only done if you want to emphasize the subject:
(23) [Streams of people arrived] and [ate arctic anchovies].
(24) [Streams of people arrived] and [streams of people ate arctic anchovies].
As an alternative to analyzing (23) as a case of ellipsis, you could argue that it involves
the coordination of two VPs, as in (25). Again, I leave that to you to decide:
(25)
S
VP
VP CVP
arrived and
VNP
ate
NP
NPP
Streams
PNP
of N
people
AdjPN
Adj anchovies
arctic
In this section, I have used the coordinator and since that is the most frequent. In some
kinds of writing, it is the third most frequent word (aer the articles a and the, and
usually in competition with the prepositions to and of). Other, less frequent coordina-
tors are (n)or, (n)either, so and but.
.
Terminological labyrinth and conclusion
In this concluding section, I will list some synonyms or near synonyms for terms
related to clauses that are used in the grammatical tradition. Remember that, in this
book, the terms clause and sentence are used interchangeably. Grammar has existed
for thousands of years and hence there are many terms that have come to be used. I
am sorry about that. We need a United Nations decision on this but, for the moment,
perhaps Table 7.1 helps.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Table 7.1. Terms for clause
sentence = main clause = matrix clause = independent clause = superordinate clause = S
clause = embedded clause/sentence = dependent/subordinate clause = CP (Complementizer Phrase)
complementizer = subordinating conjunction = subordinator = C
clause = coordinated clause = coordinated sentence = CP (Coordinator Phrase)
coordinator = coordinating conjunction = C
Note that a main clause always has to be nite but that an embedded clause can be
nite or non-nite.
In conclusion, this chapter discusses sentences that contain more than one lexical
verb. ese are of two types, embedded and coordinated. Embedded clauses are part
of another clause and typically function as subject, direct object, or adverbial in that
clause. Examples are given of all the functions that clauses have as well as of their trees.
ere can be more than one embedded clause in a main clause, and sometimes clauses
are extraposed. We represent embedded clauses as CPs (Complementizer Phrases),
and these consist of a complementizer C and an S.
Coordinated sentences are like independent sentences but are combined with a
coordinator, such as and. Two possible structures are suggested, one when the two
clauses are dependent on each other and another when thats not the case. e reader
is invited to choose for him- or herself depending on the kind of sentence. ere is also
a brief discussion of subject ellipsis which may occur when the subject in coordinate
sentences is identical. Here too, there is an alternative analysis.
Key terms are clause and sentence; main clause/sentence; embedded and coordi-
nate, CP, S, and C; complementizer; coordinator; ellipsis; and extraposition.
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
Exercises
A. Find the lexical verbs in the sentences below (adapted from the London Times,
http://www.timesonline.co.uk/tol/news/uk/article509889.ece). After that, put brackets
around the clause that goes with each of these verbs.
(26) Mr. Bell, the Chief Inspector of Schools in England, nds that classroom discipline
is worse since the current government took oce several years ago.
(27) The proportion of secondary schools with good pupil behavior has fallen from
three quarters to two thirds, while 9 per cent have serious discipline problems.
(28) The Chief Inspector worries that so many of the 60 per cent of youngsters from
non-professional backgrounds lack the ambition or qualications to go to university
a quarter of a century after he became the rst in his family to do so.
(29) Access to good schools is the key. Mr. Bell highlights unacceptable levels of
variability in the performance of state schools.
(30) He focuses particularly on the 10 per cent that make little or no improvement
between inspection visits.
(31) Because children in these schools are eectively being written o, it is little
wonder that many suer the greatest problems with discipline.
B.
Underline the lexical verbs and put brackets around the clauses that go with them in the
following text, adapted from http://www.eva.mpg.de/psycho/dogs/dogs_research.html.
One aim of the Department of Comparative and Developmental Psychology is the
investigation of the evolution of dierent cognitive processes. The comparative approach
includes the study of a variety of animal species. Although most of our work is done
with the great apes, we also investigate other species such as goats, seals, and dogs. For
a number of reasons, the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) is a very interesting model for
investigating dierent questions regarding the evolution of cognitive abilities. The fact
that dogs have been living with humans for at least 15.000 years may have led to the
selection of cognitive abilities by humans or even the co-evolution of dogs cognitive
abilities with those of humans. We know from dierent studies that dogs are sensitive
to the attentional state of humans. We also know that dogs understand communicative
cues. Those abilities have not been found in nonhuman primates and wolves.
C.
Draw trees for:
(32) Zelda noticed that candies disappear.
(33) They suggested that the sketch was done by daughters of the architect.
(34) They fussed that the unpleasant computer was down again.
(35) They puried books because they didn't like them.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(36) I heard that a manuscript has been stolen.
(37) Amir didn't know if Zoya was unhappy.
(38) He left the party because she arrived.
D. And for:
(39) Fortunately, Zelda discovered that Zoltan missed her.
(40) Because the snow was bad, the trac on that street became impossible.
(41) Zoltan mentioned that Bela had gone to the library without his rain jacket.
(42) That two paintings were stolen from the Munch Museum is so sad.
(43) I wondered whether that would happen.
E. List the functions of the embedded clauses in (32) to (43).
F. Draw trees for (44) and (45). The latter is a non-nite and this construction will be
discussed in the next chapter:
(44) I wonder what he saw.
(45) He told us where to go.
G. There are a few special types of sentences that we haven't had a chance to talk about
above, namely (46) and (47):
(46) If he was rich, (then) he would own an island.
(47) He did that task as well as he could.
We won't draw trees for these, but think about the structures. They are very dierent from
each other.
Class discussion
H. Sentences such as I mentioned that Sue won the Nobel Prize yesterday are ambiguous.
How are they (draw trees) and how would you change them if you wanted to avoid
ambiguity?
I.
Find the lexical verbs in:
We the People of the United States, in Order to form a more perfect Union, establish
Justice, insure domestic Tranquility, provide for the common defence, promote the
general Welfare, and secure the Blessings of Liberty to ourselves and our Posterity, do
ordain and establish this Constitution for the United States of America. (The Preamble
to the Constitution of the United States)
J.
How might you bracket the long sentence in Figure 7.1?
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
Keys to the exercises
A. The lexical verbs are underlined and the clauses surrounded by brackets:
(26) [Mr. Bell, the Chief Inspector of Schools in England, nds [that classroom discipline
is worse] [since the current government took oce several years ago]].
(27) [The proportion of secondary schools with good pupil behavior has fallen from
three quarters to two thirds, [while 9 per cent have serious discipline problems]].
(I think in this sentence, we could argue that while is a coordinator).
(28) [The Chief Inspector worries [that so many of the 60 per cent of youngsters from
non-professional backgrounds lack the ambition or qualications [to go to university
a quarter of a century [after he became the rst in his family to do so]]]]. (The
clause around go should get clear in the next chapter; don’t worry about that now).
(29) [Access to good schools is the key]. [Mr. Bell highlights unacceptable levels of
variability in the performance of state schools].
(30) [He focuses particularly on the 10 per cent [that make little or no improvement
between inspection visits]].
(31) [[Because children in these schools are eectively being written o], it is little
wonder [that many suer the greatest problems with discipline]].
B. [One aim of the Department of Comparative and Developmental Psychology is the inves-
tigation of the evolution of dierent cognitive processes]. [The comparative approach
includes the study of a variety of animal species]. [[Although most of our work is done
with the great apes], we also investigate other species such as goats, seals, and dogs].
[For a number of reasons, the domestic dog (Canis familiaris) is a very interesting model
for [investigating dierent questions regarding the evolution of cognitive abilities]]. [The
fact [that dogs have been living with humans for at least 15.000 years] may have led to
the selection of cognitive abilities by humans or even the co-evolution of dogs cognitive
abilities with those of humans]. [We know from dierent studies [that dogs are sensitive
to the attentional state of humans]]. [We also know [that dogs understand communicative
cues]]. [Those abilities have not been found in nonhuman primates and wolves].
C. (32)
S
NP VP
Zelda
VCP
noticed
CS
that
NP VP
NV
candies disappear
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(33)
S
NP VP
They
VCP
suggested
CS
that
NP
DN
the sketch
VP
VGPPP
pf passV PNP
hadbeen doneby
NPP
daughters
of the architect
(34)
NP
S
VP
T
hey
V
CP
fussed
CS
that
NP VP
DN V
AdvP
the
Adv
AdjPNV AdjP again
Adj computer was Adj
unpleasant down
(35)
S
NP VP
They
V CP
VNP
C
S
purified N
because
books
NP VP
they
VGPNP
them
do-neg V
didn'tlike
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
Sentence (36) involves an embedded object and is similar in structure to (32), (33), and (34).
Sentence (37) is similar too, except that if is in the C. Sentence (38) has the same structure as (35).
D. (39)
S
AdvP S
Adv
Fortunately NP VP
Zelda
VCP
discovered
CS
that
NP VP
Zoltan
VNP
missed her
(40)
CP
CS
Because
NP VP
DNV AdjP
thesnow was
Adj
bad
S
S
VP
V AdjP
became Adj
impossible
NP
D
N
the
NPP
traffic
PNP
on
that street
Sentence (41) is similar to the above embedded objects, and so is (43), except that whether is in C.
The structure of (42) is as below, with the embedded clause as subject:
(42)
S
CP
CS
T
hat
(I made the Munch Museum
into a compound)
NP
DN
two paintings
VP
VGP PP
pass VP NP
were stolen from
DN
the Munch Museum
VP
V AdjP
is
Adv Adj
so sad
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
E. In (32), (33), (34), (36), (37), (39), (41), and (43) it is a Direct Object; in (35), (38), and (40) an
Adverbial; and in (42), it is a Subject.
F. In (44), the object of the embedded clause what is used as complementizer. We can
represent that by saying that what moved to the C position.
(44)
S
NP VP
I
VCP
wonder
CS
what
NP VP
he
saw
V (what)
The same is true in (45): the adverb where is used as complementizer:
(45)
S
NP VP
He
VNPCP
told us
CS
where
NP VP
Ø
VGP
(where)
to go
G. In (46), the two clauses are coordinated since one is not subordinate to the other. In (47),
as well as he could is an adverbial, and one could argue that the head is the adverb well, so
that it is an AdvP in form. The head well is modied by a clause as he could that is dependent
on the adverb as, a discontinuous modier. This is quite a complex construction.
Special topic: Preposition or complementizer: The preposition like
Especially since the 1980s, like has expanded its uses tremendously. It is sometimes claimed
that it is the most frequent word in the speech of certain groups of speakers (see cartoon
below). Prescriptive grammarians are not too pleased with this development, but tend to focus
on the use of like as a complementizer. This prescriptive rule goes as follows:
(48) like is a preposition and not a complementizer.
Chapter 7. Finite clauses 
That means that like can introduce an NP but not a clause. Instead of like, as is used to
introduce a sentence. Fowler (1926 [1950]: 325ff.) is not too clear in the following excerpt
but is not happy with the use of like except as preposition. He writes:
It will be best to dispose rst of what is, if it is a misuse at all, the most agrant &
easily recognizable misuse of like. A sentence from Darwin quoted in the OED [Oxford
English Dictionary] contains it in a short & unmistakable form: Unfortunately few have
observed like you have done. Every illiterate person uses this construction daily; it is
the established way of putting the thing among all who have not been taught to
avoid it … in good writing this particular like is very rare.
Swan (1980: 73) is more low-key and says that “[i]n informal American English, like is very often
used as a conjunction instead of as”.
According to prescriptive authorities, we should allow like as a preposition as in (49), but
not as a complementizer as in (50) to (54):
(49) Certainly, he is not like Mr. Knightley.
(Jane Austen, Emma, Vol 1, chap 4)
(50) When we open up our hearts and our minds to those who may not think
precisely like we do, or believe precisely what we believe, that’s when we discover
at least the possibility of common ground. (President Obama, 17 May 2009,
Commencement Speech Notre Dame University)
(51) Shop like you mean it. (advertisement)
(52) I felt like I could tell you anything. Now I don’t feel like I can anymore. (quoted in
Tannen’s That's not what I meant).
(53) Winston tastes good like a cigarette should. What do you want: Good grammar or
good taste? (an ad in the 1960s that caused much controversy)
(54) She forgot all about the library like she told her old man now. (Beach Boys Song)
Except as complementizer and preposition, like is often used to mark direct speech, as in (55),
focus, as in (56), or to soften a request or demand, as in (57). These uses are not accepted in
formal speech either, even though some are old, as (58) and (59) show, quoted in the OED:
(55) So the other girl goes like: ‘Getting an autograph is like, be brave and ask for it’.
So I got it. I just went up to him and he like. ‘O.K ...
(56) I couldn’t get to class because, well, like I had this accident on the freeway.
(57) Stephanie, you, like, still owe me that $10.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(58) ȝon man is lyke out of his mynd.
(Dunbar Poems, xix, 19)
(59) all looking on, and like atonisht staring
(Spenser, Fairie Queen, iv, x, 56)
Figure 7.2. Quotative ‘like’
(Reprinted with the permission of Universal Press Syndicate. All rights reserved.)
Chapter 8
Non-nite clauses
1. Non-nite clauses
2. The functions of non-nites
3. The structure: CP
4. Coordinating non-nites
5. Conclusion
Chapter 7 deals with nite embedded and coordinated clauses, i.e. those sentences or
clauses that contain nite verbs. e present chapter deals with non-nite sentences
(or clauses), i.e. those that contain only non-nite verbs. Non-nite sentences can only
function as parts of another sentence; they are not considered well-formed sentences on
their own in formal writing but are seen as sentence fragments. Since they are not com-
plete sentences, they cannot be coordinated unless that coordinated structure is itself
embedded. Remember from the last chapter that one lexical verb means one clause, two
such verbs two clauses and so on. is holds for non-nite lexical verbs too!
In Section 1, I list the three kinds of non-nite clauses and review the general
characteristics of non-nites. en, in Section 2, I will briey illustrate the functions
that non-nite clauses have. Tree structures are provided in Section 3 and using a CP
with empty positions is justied. In Section 4, we’ll consider coordinated non-nites.
.
Non-nite clauses
ere are three kinds of non-nite clauses, namely those whose verb groups contain
innitives, or present participles, or past participles. We’ll rst consider innitives.
In (1), there are two lexical verbs, expected and go. is means there are two
clauses, which I have put brackets around. e non-nite clause her to go is the object
of expected. is non-nite clause can of course be rephrased by means of a nite
clause, as in (2):
(1) [I expected [her to go]].
(2) [I expected [that she would go]].
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
e innitive implies something uncertain or something that will happen in the future
perhaps. e corresponding main clause therefore has a modal would, expressing a
similar uncertainty.
ere are two types of innitives: one with to, as in (1), and a bare one, without to,
as in (3). e bare innitive lacks the uncertainty:
(3) She made [him leave].
e bare innitive in (3) occurs only as the object aer verbs such as make, see, hear,
and feel. e to-innitive is much more frequent. It occurs as object to many verbs, as
subject, subject predicate, and adverbial, as we’ll see.
e innitival clause with to frequently has a for as complementizer, as in (4), or
an in order, as in (5), that connects the innitival clause to the main clause:
(4) I expected [for him to be scared … ]
(from the catsite.com)
(5) [In order to understand the legislative process], it is necessary rst of all to know
something about the nature of the lawmaking body itself. (from an Alabama Senate
document)
As well see when we draw the Verb Group for the non-nite clause, to will be put
inside this Verb Group since it is similar to a modal. If you want to name it, call it an
innitive marker. I abbreviate it as ‘inf.
Apart from innitives, there are two other kinds of non-nite Verb Groups, usu-
ally referred to as participles. ey involve the present participle ending in -ing, as in
(6), and the past participle ending in -ed or -en, as in (7). Again, I have bracketed the
clauses these verbs go with:
(6) [Wa lk i ng down Rural Road], he was bothered by the trac lights.
(7) [Kidnapped last night], he is in Central Asia right now.
e form of kidnapped is a regular past participle because it ends in -ed. However, past
participles, like simple past tenses, can have irregular endings. e participle clause in
(6) can have while as a complementizer.
As mentioned in Chapter 6, non-nites fail to express tense. us, in (8), the non-
nite to walk in the subordinate clause is neither past nor present nor future. Instead,
the nite verb is/was/will be in the main clause determines the tense. e same is true
for the non-nite in (9):
(8) [To walk in the Superstition Mountains] is/was/will be nice.
(9) [Wa lk i ng in the Superstition Mountains] is/was/will be nice.
In addition, the verb in non-nite clauses displays no person or number marking, as is
shown by the ungrammaticality of walks in (10):
(10) *[For him to walks in the Superstitions] is nice.
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
A third characteristic of non-nites is that the subject is not nominative. us, (11)
and (12) are grammatical with the subject of the innitive as him, i.e. accusative. Sen-
tence (13) with a nominative he as subject of the non-nite is not:
(11) I want [him to go].
(12) I heard [him playing a song].
(13) *I want he to go.
If the subject of the non-nite clause is not a pronoun, the accusative or objective case
on this subject is of course not visible, as (14) shows:
(14) She couldn’t bear to see [Edward suering].
e non-nite verb suering in (14) can also be a verbal noun, as in (15a), and then
the subject has genitive case, namely his or Edward’s, rather than the accusative him or
Edward. In (14), suering is a verb but in (15a) it is a noun because it is preceded by a
possessive. Note that you could replace the possessive by an article, as in (15b):
(15) a. She couldn’t bear to see [his/Edwards suering].
b. She couldn’t bear to see [the suering of him/Edward].
Prescriptive grammarians object to (12) and (14), and prefer (15), known as gerund.
We will come back to gerunds as a special topic. I like to think of present participles as
a hybrid category, in between nouns and verbs, and prefer to avoid the term gerund.
As we saw in the previous chapter, nite embedded clauses, as in (16a), can become
independent from the main clause by leaving the complementizer that out, as in (16b).
A non-nite clause, as in (17a) can be the object inside another clause but, on its own,
as in (17b), it is not a complete sentence:
(16) a. I know [that he le].
b. He le.
(17) a. I want [him to go].
b. *Him to go.
We’ll now turn to the functions of the non-nite clauses.
.
The functions of non-nites
e functions of non-nite clauses are similar to those of nite ones. ey function
at sentence level as subject in (18), direct object in (19), adverbial in (20), and subject
predicate in (21):
(18) [Eating pancakes] is a pleasant thing.
(19) I love [eating pancakes].
(20) ey went there [to eat fry bread and chocolate].
(21) e problem is [to decide on what to eat].
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
e present participle clause, as in (18) and (19), and the to-innitive clause, as in (20)
and (21), are the most versatile in function. is is indicated as ‘broadin Table 8.1.
Past participle clauses, as in (7), are more restricted in that they usually function as
adverbials and bare innitives are mostly objects of certain verbs. is is indicated in
the table as narrow’. I think the functions of non-nites are not dicult but, just as a
review, see if you can identify the functions of the clauses in (1) to (9).
5
Table 8.1 lists the dierent kinds of nite and non-nite clauses, with examples,
their complementizers, and in how many functions they are used.
Table 8.1. Embedded clauses
Example C Function
Finite (2) that, because, etc broad
Non-Finite innitive (1) sometimes: for, as in (4); broad
in order, as in (5)
bare-innitive (3) no narrow
present participle (6) while, aer, before broad
past participle (7) when, where narrow
In Chapter 10, nite and non-nite clauses will be shown to function inside
phrases as well.
.
The structure: CP
I represent a non-nite clause by means of a CP, as in (22), the structure for (17a). is
captures that the non-nite clause is fairly similar in structure to the nite clause but,
as we’ll see, it is a little more reduced and therefore can have more empty positions:
(22)
S
NP VP
I
VCP
want
C
S
Ø
NP VP
himVGP
inf V
to go
5.  In (1) to (4), these clauses function as objects, in (5) and (7) as adverbials, and in (6), (8) and
(9) as subjects.
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
In (22), I have put in an empty complementizer. is C position can be lled by for in
a number of cases, e.g. in (23). I like to use the C position, even if it is empty, because
it shows that the non-nite clause is embedded:
(23) I want [for you to do your homework].
Non-nite clauses need not include a subject. e subject may be understood, as in
(24). Since the subject is understood, I will add a subject position, with an empty sub-
ject, as in (25), as well as an empty C since there is no complementizer:
(24) To hike around Weavers Needle is pleasant.
(25)
S
VP
V AdjP
is Adj
pleasant
CP
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
PP
PNP
around
DN
Weaver’s Needle
VGP
inf V
To hike
We could add the subject and complementizer in (25) and the reason I prefer (25) is
that it is pleasant for someone to walk around Weaver’s Needle; the tree expresses that
there is a subject even if this subject is le out.
e innitive marker to adds some uncertainty, as we have seen comparing (1)
and (3). I think it is somewhat similar to a modal and have therefore placed it in the
Verb Group. Be careful in recognizing this to: it goes before a verb. A preposition goes
before a noun or pronoun, as in to us.
Sentences such as (6) and (7) above can also be represented using a CP, as (26)
and (27) show:
(26)
S
S
NP VP
he
VGP
passV
was bothered
PP
PNP
by N
trac lights
CP
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
VPP
Walking
PNP
down Rural
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(27)
S
S
NP VP
he
AdvP
Adv Adv
rightno
w
CP
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
VNP
Kidnapped
DN
last night
V
VPP
is
PNP
in Central Asia
As in the case of innitives, there are people who prefer a structure with fewer empty
positions. I like seeing which clause is embedded and which clause has a potential
subject. is comes in handy with dangling modiers, for instance, as well see in the
special topic to this chapter.
In the previous chapter, I mentioned extraposed nite clauses. Extraposition is
possible with non-nites as well. For instance, (24) might be rendered as (28):
(28) It is nice [to hike around Weavers Needle].
In (28), the innitival clause that functions as the subject of the entire sentence has
been moved from the beginning to the end of the sentence.
Table 8.2 gives examples of some non-nite Verb Groups that have a full CP, an
empty C, an empty subject, and an empty C as well as empty subject.
Table 8.2. e non-nite CP
To-infinitive Present participle
Full CP
I want [for her to do well].
Ø C
I want [Ø her to do well]. I saw [Ø him crossing the street].
C Ø NP *I want [for Ø to do well].
6
[While Ø doing that], she fell.
Ø C Ø NP
I want [Ø Ø to do well]. I like [Ø Ø doing well].
. Coordinating non-nites
Non-nite clauses can be coordinated, as in (29). Note that the coordinated non-nite
clauses gossiping about Zelda and chewing gum function as subject to the verb is. (Speakers
dier as to whether the verb is is or are):
(29) [[Gossiping about Zelda] and [chewing gum]] is hard to do at the same time.
6. Note that most varieties of English do not accept for to here but that certain varieties do.
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
Other examples of coordinated non-nites are given in (30) to (32):
(30) She could not think of [[Emma losing a single pleasure], or [suering an hour’s
ennui]]. (adapted from Jane Austens Emma)
(31) But Emma, in her own mind, determined that he did not know what he was talking
about, and that he shewed a very amiable inclination [[to settle early in life], and
[to marry]].
(Emma, Vol 2, Chap 6)
(32) e point is [[to watch the whales], and [to participate in fun]] …
e coordinated non-nites function as the object to think in (30), as the complement
to inclination in (31) (this will get clearer in Chapter 10); and as the subject predicate in
(32). So, coordinated non-nite clauses always function as a unit inside another clause.
I will suggest (33) as a tree for coordinated non-nites functioning as a subject. I
leave it to your imagination what the verbs are:
(33)
S
CP VP
and
VGP AdjP
Adj
wouldbe
hard
CP
CS
for
NP VP
him
to …
CP
CS
For
NP VP
her
to …
Note that I am using coathangers’ in (32). at means that I am not indicating that the
VPs can be divided into a V (or VGP), an NP. and a PP. It shows that in this particular
tree, we are not really interested in the structure of the VP. However, I have tried to
avoid using coathangers in this book.
.
Conclusion
In this chapter, non-nite clauses are discussed. eir structure and function is quite
similar to that of nite clauses. ey are CPs and function inside another clause as
subjects, objects, adverbials, and subject predicates.
ey dier from nite clauses in that the complementizer oen does not appear
and the subject can be absent. ey also cannot stand on their own. If they are coordi-
nated, they have to function together as an embedded clause.
Key terms are non-finite Verb Group, infinitive, bare infinitive, present participle,
past participle, gerund, CP, empty C, and empty subject.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Exercises
A. Please nd the non-nite clauses that function as direct objects and adverbials in the
sentences below, adapted from an article in the East Valley Tribune (4 April 2005, by
Jason Emerson). You might try to locate the lexical verbs rst.
(34) Arizona State University wants to make wholesale changes to the undergraduate
curriculum.
(35) The eort is still in the early stages but could result in a fundamental shift in the
knowledge and skills gained from a university education.
(36) The discussion intends to transform ASU into what President Crow calls the
“New American University.
(37) Changing a universitys curriculum is often controversial. It can erupt into
full-blown controversies, as Stanford University discovered in the 1980s when
faculty members voted to replace a number of books that had been considered
part of the Western Canon.
(38) The task force is focusing on what the ideal ASU student should know upon
graduation.
(39) The task force hasn’t talked yet about specic changes. But some early ideas
include adding interdisciplinary classes and continuing them over four years until
graduation. Now, general studies are clustered in the rst two years.
(40) Carlson said the task force continues to debate how to make undergraduate
education more meaningful.
B.
Are there any coordinated sentences? Is there ellipsis?
C. Draw trees for the following sentences:
(41) Drawing trees is easy.
(42) Emma wanted to do that.
(43) For Ed to be resigning from that job is stupid.
(44) Anselm made Vicky read the paper.
(45) I saw turkeys crossing the street.
(46) Santa set the alarm to be on time.
D. In the text below, nd the non-nite clauses.
When movie producers for Star Trek III needed someone to produce alien-sounding
dialogue for the Klingons, they turned to Marc Okrand, a linguist who has done
scholarly work on Native American languages. Okrand, however, didn’t limit himself to
creating lines of dialogue. He also developed a language, complete with phonological,
morphological, and syntactic rules, in addition to vocabulary. His hard work paid o
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
not only by giving him subsequent lm work, but also by making him a sort of celebrity
among Star Trek fans across the world, who are studying Klingon. These fans are also
teaching their children to speak it, and are translating major works into Klingon. Any
short sentence will show you the complexities of Klingon. All nouns may be followed by
one or more suxes divided into ve types. If there are two or more suxes, the suxes
must occur in a specic order.
E.
Construct a sentence with two embedded sentences, one of which must be a non-nite
clause functioning as direct object.
F.
Construct a sentence with three non-nite clauses.
Class discussion
G. The following sentences are ambiguous. Why?
(47) Flying planes can be dangerous. (from Chapter 1)
(48) Visiting aliens should be amusing on a Monday morning.
H. Bracket the clauses in the sentence in Figure 8.1.
Figure 8.1. Embedded sentences
(Pardon my planet (I need help) © 2008 Vic Lee. King Features Syndicate)
I.
Read Keats poem To Autumn and circle/list the nite verbs in therst two stanzas. Discuss
the dierence between the two paragraphs in class.
To Autumn
Season of mists and mellow fruitfulness,
Close bosom friend of the maturing sun,
Conspiring with him how to load and bless
With fruit the vines that round the thatch-eaves run;
To bend with apples the mossed cottage-trees,
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
And ll all fruit with ripeness to the core;
To swell the gourd, and plump the hazel shells
With a sweet kernel; to set budding more,
And still more, later owers for the bees,
Until they think warm days will never cease,
For summer has oer-brimmed their clammy cells.
Who hath not seen thee oft amid they store?
Sometimes whoever seeks abroad may nd
Thee sitting careless on a granary oor,
Thy hair soft-lifted by the winnowing wind;
Or on a half-reaped furrow sound asleep,
Drowsed with the fume of poppies, while thy hook
Spares the next swath and all its twined owers:
And sometimes like a gleaner thou dost keep
Steady thy laden head across a brook;
Or by a cider-press, with patient look,
Thou watchest the last oozings hours by hours.
Where are the songs of spring? Ay, where are they?
Think not of them, thou hast thy music too, –
While barred clouds bloom the soft-dying day,
And touch the stubble-plains with rosy hue;
Then in a wailful choir the small gnats mourn
Among the river sallows, borne aloft
Or sinking as the light wind lives or dies;
And full-grown lambs loud bleat from hilly bourn;
Hedge-crickets sing; and now with treble soft
The red-breast whistles from a garden-croft;
And gathering swallows twitter in the skies.
J.
Make up some sentences with an innitival and participial object, as in (49) and (50).
Try to think about the dierence between the use of innitive and present participle.
You might look back to Table 8.2 as well:
(49) I want to visit Iceland, Israel, India, and Indonesia.
(50) I like traveling.
This is quite complex and has to do with to. I think verbs with the present participle as
complement are more auxiliary-like and the two clauses are not as independent as when
an innitive is involved. What do you think?
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
Keys to the exercises
A. The lexical verbs are underlined and the non-nite clauses functioning as objects and
adverbials are indicated by brackets:
(34) Arizona State University wants [to make wholesale changes to the undergraduate
curriculum]. (=Object, DO)
(35) The eort is still in the early stages but could result in a fundamental shift in the
knowledge and skills gained from a university education. (coordinated clause and
reduced relative)
(36) The discussion intends [to transform ASU into what President Crow calls the
“New American University]. (=Object, DO)
(37) [Changing a university’s curriculum] is often controversial. It can erupt into
full-blown controversies, as Stanford University discovered in the 1980s when
faculty members voted [to replace a number of books that had been considered
part of the Western Canon]. (=Adverbial)
(38) The task force is focusing on [what the ideal ASU student should know upon
graduation]. (=Object)
(39) The task force hasn’t talked yet about specic changes. But some early ideas
include [[adding interdisciplinary classes] and [continuing them over four years
until graduation]]. (=Object that consists of coordinated non-nite clauses). Now,
general studies are clustered in the rst two years.
(40) Carlson said [the task force continues [to debate [how to make undergraduate
education more meaningful]]]. (=Object three times, DO)
B. Coordinated clauses occur in (35), since but can be seen as a coordinator. There is ellipsis
of the subject before could. In (39), two present participle clauses are coordinated and
form a complement.
C. (41)
S
VP
V AdjP
is Adj
easy
CP
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
VNP
Drawingtrees
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(42)
S
NP VP
Emma
VCP
wanted
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
VGPNP
that
infV
to do
(43)
S
VP
V AdjP
is
Adj
stupid
CP
CS
For
NP VP
Ed
VGPPP
inf progr VP NP
to be resigning from
DN
that job
(44)
S
NP VP
Anselm
VCP
made
CS
Ø
NP VP
Vicky
VNP
read
DN
the paper
Sentence (45) has the same structure as (44).
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
(46)
NP VP
Santa
S
V
VNP
set
DN
the alarm
CP
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
VGPPP
infVPNP
to be on time
D. Non-nite clauses are in brackets: When movie producers for Star Trek III needed [someone
to produce alien-sounding dialogue for the Klingons], they turned to Marc Okrand, a
linguist who has done scholarly work on Native American languages. Okrand, however,
didn’t limit himself to [creating lines of dialogue]. He also developed a language, complete
with phonological, morphological, and syntactic rules, in addition to vocabulary. His hard
work paid o not only by [giving him subsequent lm work], but also by [making him a
sort of celebrity among Star Trek fans across the world, who are studying Klingon]. These
fans are also teaching [their children to speak it], and are translating major works into
Klingon. Any short sentence will show you the complexities of Klingon. All nouns may be
followed by one or more suxes [divided into ve types]. If there are two or more suxes,
the suxes must occur in a specic order.
E. Kim and Paul hoped [to see unicorns in the parking lot] [because they had studied
their habits].
F. They intend [to nd out if [looking out the window more often] makes [them work better]].
Special topic: Dangling participles and gerunds
There are two topics discussed here that involve the present participle. Both have been much
criticized by prescriptivists.
As we have seen in Chapter 3, PPs can go with a noun (e.g. woman in the woman with
glasses) or with a verb (e.g. saw in saw her with glasses). Sometimes, it is hard to tell. In this
special topic, we’ll look at non-nite adverbial clauses that are misplaced and often result in a
funny reading. There are prescriptive rules about how to place the modier. In (51), there is one
relevant for a non-nite clause:
(51) Avoid Dangling Participles
The subject of a clause with a participle in it (i.e. without a subject of its own) must
be the same as the subject of the main clause.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Swan (1980: 455) provides the following rule: “It is usually considered a mistake to make
sentences like these in which the subjects are dierent: Looking out of the window of our hotel,
there were lots of mountains … However, there are some very common expressions which
break this rule. Generally speaking, … Judging from his expression, … Considering, …. Fowler
(1926 [1950]: 675) says that “it is to be remembered that there is a continual change going on
by which certain participles or adjectives acquire the character of prepositions or adverbs,
no longer needing the prop of a noun to cling to. Hence, neither Swan nor Fowler are very
critical of the use.
‘Incorrect uses are given in (52) to (56). Some of these are funny because we automatically
think of the participles as having the same subject as the main clause:
(52) Running down the street, the house was on re.
(53) Referring to your letter of 5 September, you do not state …
(54) Although spoken in Shakespeare’s First Folio, we do not speak that way today.
(55) Lying in a heap on the oor, she found the clothes.
(56) Being a student, the challenges are many.
Sometimes, the left out subjects seem to be able to refer to the subject of the main clause, or
to the closest NP, or to neither. The rst meaning we come up with in (57) is the one where the
waiter is drenched in syrup:
(57) The waiter brought the waes to the table drenched in maple syrup.
Misplaced participles are not new. An example from Shakespeare appears in (58). I have put
brackets around the participle:
(58) It’s giuen out, that [sleeping in mine Orchard], A Serpent stung me: so the whole
eare of Denmarke, Is by a forged processe of my death Rankly abus’d: But know
thou Noble youth, The Serpent that did sting thy Fathers life, Now weares his
Crowne. (Hamlet I, v, 35)
The gerund was mentioned in section 1. Many grammarians avoid the term gerund’;
I mention it in this book since it seems to be a pervasive term among my audience. It is a
present participle that looks either as a noun or as a verb, as in (59) and (60) respectively:
(59) I like [his doing that].
(60) I like [him doing that].
Fowler calls the present participle used as a verb the ‘fused participle (1926 [1950]: 206), probably
because it gets the same case as the noun preceding it. He calls the one used as a noun the
gerund. In the Modern English Usage, Fowler doesn’t quite dene the fused participle, except by
providing examples, as in (61):
(61) [Women having the vote] reduces mens political power.
Chapter 8. Non-nite clauses 
About its users, Fowler says “[i]t need hardly be said that writers with any sense of style do not,
even if they allow themselves the fused participle, make so bad a use of the bad thing as is
shown above to be possible” (1926 [1950]: 207).
In conclusion, the dangling participle, as in (52) to (58), can result in amusing ambiguity;
the present participle used as a verb, as in (60), has been denounced for centuries (without
much success).
Review of Chapters 7 and 8
Chapters 7 and 8 cover embedded and coordinated sentences. If a sentence contains
more than one lexical verb, it contains multiple sentences or clauses. An embedded
clause functions (as subject, object, adverbial, or subject predicate) inside another clause.
is is true for nite and non-nite embedded clauses. e nite embedded clauses
discussed in chapter 7 all contain a nite verb and a complementizer that is optional in
some cases. e non-nite clauses come in a number of shapes, as innitives, present
participles, and past participles. ey are ‘smaller’ in that the complementizer and sub-
ject need not be present.
A coordinated clause can be split into two independent clauses but only if its
clauses are nite. Coordinate nite sentences can leave the second subject out aer
a coordinator, and that is called ellipsis. Some coordinated sentences are borderline
embedded sentences since the second clause can be seen as an adverbial.
e structure of the embedded sentence is a CP which accommodates the comple-
mentizer C and the S. A typical embedded clause is given in (1), with the CP embedded
in the main S as sister to the V:
(1)
S
NP VP
Sam
VCP
said
CS
that
NP VP
Diane
VNP
was
DN
a poetess
In a non-finite clause, some of the positions in (1) would be empty; see (26) of
Chapter 8.
e structure of coordinate sentences is also a CP with a coordinator C and S.
Subjects and complementizers can be absent in the non-initial coordinated clause. We
discussed two possible structures.
Review of Chapters 7 and 8 
Exercises
A. In the following sentences, please identify by means of brackets and labels the nite
and non-nite clauses that function as subjects, direct objects, subject predicates,
and adverbials:
(2) Some linguistic historians prefer to believe that languages live and die by social
evolution.
(3) They saw him cross the street without looking.
(4) It is gratifying to see that idea becoming more accepted.
(5) Since the advent of printing, the standard language may have developed that way
because of increased standardization.
(6) The president that founded this organization was arrested twice before he was
replaced.
B.
Which are the lexical verbs in (2) to (6) and which are the nite Verb Groups (i.e. a VGP
containing a nite verb)?
C.
Draw a tree for (7):
(7) Poirot thought that he had sucient evidence to solve the mystery.
Keys to the exercises
A. (2) Some linguistic historians prefer [OBJECT: to believe [OBJECT:
that languages live and die by social evolution]]. Note that I consider
‘live and die as coordinated verbs not as separate clauses.
(3) They saw [OBJECT: him cross the street [ADVERBIAL: without looking]].
(4) It is gratifying [(extraposed) SUBJECT: to see [OBJECT: that idea becoming more
accepted]].
(5) Since the advent of printing, the standard language may have developed that way
because of increased standardization. NOTHING
(6) The president that founded this organization was arrested twice [ADVERBIAL:
before he was replaced]. Note that this sentence also contains a relative clause.
Relative clauses modify a N and do not function independently. For more on this,
see Chapter 10.
B. In (2), prefer, believe, live, die are lexical. In (3), saw, cross, and looking are; in (4) is, see,
becoming; in (5), developed; in (6), founded, arrested, replaced are.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Finite VGPs in (2) are prefer, live and die; in (3), saw; in (4), is; in (5), may have developed;
and in (6), founded, was arrested, was replaced.
C. (7)
S
NP
VP
Poirot
V
CP
thought
C S
that
NP VP
he
V
VNP
had
AdjPN
Adj evidence
sufficient
CP
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
VGP NP
infVDN
to solve themystery
Sample quiz/exam, covering Chapters 7 and 8
A. What is prescriptively wrong with sentence (1)? Provide the name of this phenomenon
and explain why it is wrong.
(1) Although spoken by Shakespeare, we don’t speak that way today.
B. Identify the (main and subordinate) clauses by means of brackets in the short text below.
Indicate which clauses are nite:
The future of 100,000 refugees was dealt another blow this week after the Bhutanese
government rejected a UN formula. Bhutan and Nepal started negotiations to solve
the problem of the people in refugee camps in 1992. Since then 9 high-level meetings
have taken place without resulting in a solution, however.
C.
Draw trees for (2), (3) and (4):
(2) Those Martians decided that they would take along some chickens on their trip.
(3) They wanted to see him before leaving Malacandra.
(4) For us to accompany penguins on that trip is a wonderful opportunity.
Review of Chapters 7 and 8 
Keys to the Quiz/Exam
A. The subject of the main clause (we) and that of the embedded clause (a hidden subject that
is probably language/English) are not the same. Thats why the prescriptive problem is
that of a dangling participle.
B. The clauses are bracketed and the nite clauses have FIN marking that:
FIN[The future of 100,000 refugees was dealt another blow this week FIN[after the
Bhutanese government rejected a UN formula]]. FIN[Bhutan and Nepal started
negotiations [to solve the problem of the people in refugee camps in 1992]]. FIN[Since
then 9 high-level meetings have taken place [without resulting in a solution, however]].
(2)
S
NP VP
DNVCP
Those Martians decided
CS
that
they VP
(I have the phrasal verb as compound)
PP
on their trip
V
VGPNP
modal VD N
would take along some chickens
(3)
S
NP VP
They
CP
C S
before
NP VP
Ø
VNP
leavingMalacandra
V
V CP
wanted
CS
Ø
NP VP
Ø
NP
him
V
infV
to see
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(4)
S
VP
VNP
is
D
N
a
AP N
A opportunity
wonderful
CP
CS
For
NP VP
us
V
PP
VGPNP on that trip
N
infVpenguins
to accompany
Chapter 9
The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP,
and NP
1. The structure of the PP, AdjP, and AdvP
and the functions inside
2. The structure of the NP and the functions
inside
3. Arguments for distinguishing complements
from modifiers (Optional)
4. Conclusion
Up to now, we have mainly seen phrases function at sentence level (as subjects, direct
objects, subject predicates, adverbials, etc.). In this chapter, examples are given where
phrases function inside other phrases, as modiers and complements to the heads of
these phrases. Grammatical categories, such as the determiner, also function inside
phrases, whereas auxiliaries function in the Verb Group (see Chapter 6) and comple-
mentizers link one sentence to another.
Some of the structure of the NP, AdjP, AdvP, and PP has already been provided in
Chapter 3. ere, we noted that PPs could be modiers to a noun (or adverbials in a
sentence). In this chapter, we discuss the modier function in more detail and add the
complement. e four possible functions inside a phrase are determiner, head, modi-
er, and complement. ere can be more than one modier in a phrase, but not more
than one complement or determiner.
In Section 1, I review the structure for the simple phrase, the PP, AdjP, and
the AdvP, and examine the components of each of these phrases. In Section 2, we
discuss the NP. It can have a modifier as well as a complement and a determiner.
The distinction between modifier and complement is elaborated on Section 3.
This distinction can be skipped depending on how much detail you want (or have
time) to explore.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
. The structure of the PP, AdjP, and AdvP and the functions inside
e structure of the Prepositional Phrase is relatively straightforward, with a P head
and an NP complement, as in (1a). e PP can of course be longer, as in on the roof of
the very fancy gingerbread house, with a tree as in (1b):
(1)
a. PP b. PP
PNPPNP
on on
DN DN
the roof the
NPP
roof
PNP
of
DN
the
AdjP N
gingerbread house
7
Adv Adj
very fancy
e preposition is the head of the PP and the NP that follows always functions as
complement. ere are a limited number of modiers to PPs, e.g. right and straight, as
in right to school. We wont draw a tree including those here.
Instances of Adjective Phrases are very fancy in (1b), blatantly illegal in (2), perfectly
safe, nice, interesting, and too good:
(2) at was [blatantly illegal].
ese phrases are called AdjPs because their heads are adjectives, i.e. illegal in (2). A
structure for an AdjP would be as in (3), where illegal is the head and the adverb bla-
tantly modies it:
(3)
AdjP
AdvP Adj
Adv illegal
blatantly
e adverb blatantly expresses the manner of the illegality. I have made it into an AdvP
because you can expand it into very blatantly, but most modiers to adjectives are
degree adverbs, so just Adv.
us, (3) contains an Adj head and an AdvP modier. In very rare cases, there can
be a complement to the adjective as well (not to the adverb though). For instance, in
7.  I treat gingerbread house as a compound noun.
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
(4a), of his catch does not describe the manner or the place of being proud but what
someone is proud of, i.e. of his catch is the complement of proud (inside the VP wed
call it a direct object). e same is true of about that waste in (4b):
(4) a. He was [blatantly proud of his catch].
b. ere is something that is [very illegal about that waste].
A tree for the AdjP in (4b) is given in (5). I have indicated the dierent functions of the
elements of the phrase. As in the case of VPs where objects are sisters to V, the complement
about that waste is sister to the Adjective. In (5), I have put in the (intermediate) label Adj
(pronouncedAdjective-bar’). In Chapters 3 and 5, we mentioned intermediate nodes in
connection with the NP and the VP. As mentioned, as much structure as in (5) is
unusual for an AdjP:
(5)
AdjP
Adv Adj
very
Modier Adj PP
illegal
Head
PNP
about
DN
Complement
that waste
Some other examples of adjectives that have complements are able, afraid, aware, conscious,
fond, glad, happy, mad, proud, reasonable, and successful.
Very frequently, adjectives are ‘stacked’, as in (6), which is a bit exaggerated:
(6) e beautiful, large, fast, young, spotted leopard jumped out of nowhere.
Adjectives occur in a particular order that native speakers dont even pay attention to,
but that is very complex to work out. Try ordering the adjectives in (6) in a dierent
way and see how that works out. If you are curious about the order and the tree for (6),
look at question D and its answer.
AdvPs have a simple structure. e ones listed in (7) came up aer a search for
happily in the British National Corpus (all from written non-ction):
(7) a. One day he will happily walk along a busy road.
b. I’d happily buy her this.
c. It is a contradiction which thousands happily go along with because they are
keen to advance up the social ladder.
d. He was a gentle man, happily dominated by his competent wife.
e. I turned to nd the young Mr. Cardinal beaming happily at me.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
So, these have just a head, as in (8a), or as I oen put them, as in (8b):
(8)
a. AdvP b. AdvP
Adv
Adv happil
y
happily
e AdvPs can be expanded by a modier that precedes the head, e.g. by the degree
adverb very in very happily in (9), also from the BNC non-ction collection, with the
AdvP drawn in (10):
(9) e majority of popular community sh will survive very happily on this diet.
(10)
AdvP
Adv Adv
very happil
y
e adverbs that modify adverbs are few in number. Some examples are very, so, too,
extremely, really, and quite. ey are all degree adverbs and cannot be expanded. ats
why they do not head their own AdvP, but are represented as just an Adv in (10). A
summary table is provided.
Table 9.1. Components of the PP, AdjP, and AdvP
PP P head and NP complement (on the roof)
AdjP Adv(P) modier and Adj head
and occasional PP complement (very proud of his mother)
AdvP Degree Adv modier and Adv head (very happily)
When drawing a tree, you need not put the functions in, just use D, Adj, N, PP, etc.
.
The structure of the NP and the functions inside
Typical instances of NPs are provided in (11a) and (11b):
(11)
a. NP b. NP
DN DN
that the
AdjP NNPP
Adj
manatee manatee
blue PNP
from Florida
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
In (11a), the AdjP blue modies the head in that it describes a quality or characteristic of
the manatee. We can add many such modifers, e.g. where the manatee comes from, if
it is fast or slow, and whether we think it is nice or not. From Florida in (11b) modies
the head as well since it tells you where the manatee is from. So modiers can precede
or follow the head: AdjPs precede and PPs follow. Hence, they are sometimes called
pre-modiers and post-modiers respectively. Determiners function as pointers: that
points to a particular manatee and the makes it a specic manatee. In (12a) and (12b),
I repeat these structures with the functions added, but we dont usually clutter up the
tree that way since the functions are predictable from the tree.
(12)
a. NP b. NP
DN D
N
that Determiner the
AdjP
N
N
PP
Adj manatee
manatee
blue Head PNP
Determiner from Florida
Modier
An NP in English can also contain what is called a complement to the noun. Unlike objects
in the VP, complements to N and Adj are optional and thats what makes it hard to distin-
guish between modiers and complements. See how helpful the following discussion and
that in Section 3 are and decide if the distinction is important or interesting for you.
8
Examples of NPs with complements to the head nouns are given in (13) to (16),
with the complement in brackets:
(13) e teacher [of Martian]
(14) eir discussion [about genetics]
(15) e student [of elephants]
(16) Your reply [to my letter]
e nouns that can have complements are verb-like. One way to check if a PP is a com-
plement is to make the noun into a verb. If you change the nouns into verbs in (13) to
(16), the complements change into direct objects in (17) to (19) and into prepositional
object in (20). e type of object they become depends on the verb:
(17) You teach [Martian].
(18) ey discussed [genetics].
(19) She studied [elephants].
(20) You replied [to my letter].
8.  From my own experience, this is difficult material and, since it is less crucial to the understanding
of the NP (and AdjP), I sometimes skip complements to nouns.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
e NPs in (13) to (16) change into full sentences in (17) to (20) as well. A table with
typical PP modiers and complements to nouns is given as Table 9.2.
Table 9.2. Examples of nouns with modiers and with complements
Modiers Complements
the manatee [from Florida] the teacher [of English]
the student [with red hair] the student [of physics]
a boy with [with green hair] an appeal [to reason]
a book [on the table] the investigation [of corruption]
a glass [on the table] the allegations [of murder]
green tea [from Korea] recruitment [of new sta]
a computer [with sound] his attack [on that celebrity]
Modiers are quite free, e.g. with red/green hair and on the table can occur with many
nouns. Complements are more restricted and only go with certain nouns, e.g. student,
teacher, discussion, disgust and investigation. Adding of physics to teacher and student is
ne but adding it to boy results in a very strange phrase!
As in the case of objects inside the VP (Chapter 4), complements to the N can be
represented in the tree as sisters to the head, in this case N, as in (21) and (22):
(21)
NP
DN
e
NPP
teacher
PNP
of Martian
(22)
NP
DN
eir
NPP
discussion
PNP
about N
genetics
So far, we have seen that the elements of an NP in English function as determiner,
head, modier, and complement. is is summarized in Table 9.3. e name (i.e. label
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
or realization) of each of these functions is listed underneath the function. Note that
there can be many modiers but only one determiner and one complement.
Table 9.3. Functions inside the NP (the ‘^’ indicates that there can be more than one)
determiner modier^ head complement modier^
D AdjP^ N PP PP^
the nice student of chemistry from Macedonia
several interesting discussions about politics at night
a delicious pie from my friend
those noisy teachers of linguistics from outer space
In tree form, the expanded NP in the rst example of Table 9.3 looks like (23):
(23)
NP
DN
e
AdjP N
Adj
nice N PP
NPPP NP
student from Macedonia
PNP
of chemistry
As indicated by the ^^ in Table 9.3, modiers can be repeated on both sides of the
noun. Multiple modiers oen result in ambiguities, as in (24) and (25):
(24) On a menu: ‘Vegetarian Chicken Soup. (Was the chicken vegetarian?)
(25) An o/Old English French teacher. (When spoken, the punctuation doesn’t appear!)
In this section, we have spent most time on the head, the modier, and the comple-
ment. e determiner is relatively easy. Check Chapter 2 for a list of determiners. If the
D is there, it appears right underneath the NP, as in (26):
(26)
NP
DN
Determiner my/the
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
e determiner is special in that it is both an umbrella category name, which includes
articles, quantiers, demonstratives, possessives, etc. (see Chapter 2), as well as a func-
tion name.
As you may remember from Chapter 2, the pre-determiner may be added as a
function inside the NP. In (27), three quantiers that function in this way are given. In
a tree, they would precede the D, but I won’t go into this here:
(27) All the nice books; half the people; both my pictures.
e last element we are adding to the NP is the focusser or emphasizer, but these are
relatively rare. Some of the ones that occur in English are just, only, especially, and
even, as in (28). Most of these are tricky in that they can be used in other ways too, e.g.
even and just are also adjectives, and most are adverbs as well:
(28) then it will perhaps gravel [even a philosopher] to comprehend it.
(George Berkeley, Treatise 97)
. Arguments for distinguishing complements from modifiers (Optional)
As was mentioned in the previous section, inside the NP, some elements are more
closely related to the head N than others. We can refer to these as complements and
modiers respectively. ey can be compared to the objects (even though the latter
are more obligatory) and the adverbials in the VP. Above, I have suggested that, if you
can change the noun into a verb (discussion into discuss), the PP complement will
change into an NP object (or PP object depending on the verb). To me, that is the most
crucial argument. In this section, I provide several additional arguments (summarized
in Table 9.4) for distinguishing complements from modiers and provide trees that
show the distinction.
.
Complement and modifier follow the head N
e NP in (29) has a modier from England that tells you where the teacher is from
and a complement of physics that tells you what the teacher teaches:
(29) A teacher of physics from England.
In the tree structure, we represent the dierence between the complement and the
modier by having complements be sisters to N and modiers sisters to N. For
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
instance, in (30), of physics is sister to N and is therefore the complement, whereas
from England is a sister to the N and is therefore the modier:
(30)
NP
DN
a
N PP
NPPPNP Modier
teacher
from England
PNP
of N
physics Complement
ere can only be one complement and the order between complement and modier
cannot be reversed as the ungrammaticality of (31) shows:
(31) *A teacher [from England] [of physics].
e impossibility of complement modier reversal is the rst argument that you can
use to distinguish between complements and modiers.
Apart from word order (complement is closest to the head), there is a second
way to distinguish complements from modiers and it involves determining what pro-
noun one can use to pronominalize certain parts of the NP. In (28), teacher of physics and
teacher of physics from England are Ns. e N can be replaced by one, but the N (and NP)
cannot be replaced by one. In (32), one replaces teacher of physics, i.e. an N, and the
sentence is grammatical; in (33), one replaces teacher, i.e. an N, and this results in an
ungrammatical sentence:
(32) I know the [teacher of physics] from England and the one from France.
(33) *I know the [teacher] of physics from England and the one of chemistry.
ese sentences provide evidence for the special status of the intermediate category N
in that it can be replaced by one, as in (32), unlike the N in (33).
.
Complement and modifier precede the head N
Complements and modiers can also precede the N, as in (34). e modier English
again says something about the teacher, i.e. where he or she is from, and the complement
physics claries what the teacher teaches:
(34) at English physics teacher.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
ere can be many modiers, as (35) shows, but there can only be one complement
connected to the noun, of course, as (36) shows:
(35) at [nice] [intelligent] teacher [with purple hair].
(36) *at [physics] teacher [of chemistry].
Again, the complement is closer to the head than the modier, as (34) shows. e
complement is sister to the N whereas the modier is sister to the N:
(37)
NP
DN
that
AdjP
N
Adj
Modier English NP N
N teacher
Complement physics
e same arguments to distinguish complements and modiers hold as in the case of
(29). First, their order cannot be reversed, as the ungrammaticality of (38) shows, and
there can only be one complement but many modiers, as in (39):
(38) *at physics English teacher.
(39) at nice, patient, English [chemistry] teacher.
Secondly, replacement by one of the N physics teacher in (34) is possible. See (40). e
N teacher cannot be replaced. See (41):
(40) at English one.
(41) at English physics one.
e N English physics teacher can also be replaced of course.
So far, I have only drawn trees with the modiers and complements either preced-
ing or following the noun. Tree (42) has modiers on either side as well as a comple-
ment preceding the head:
(42)
NP
DN
the
AdjP N
Adj
adventurousN PP
NP NP NP
Nteacher with N
physics
ideas
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
e complement physics in (42) is sister to the N teacher and the two modiers are
sisters to N.
A question that oen comes up in class is what the category of some of these
pre-nominal modiers and complements is. When complements precede the head,
it is unclear what the category is, an N or an Adj. For instance, English in (37) is a
clear adjective when it tells you where someone comes from. However, it looks like
a noun when the teacher teaches the English language, as in the ungrammatical
(38). I will treat it as a noun when it is a complement and an adjective when it is a
modier.
In this section, I have shown that there is evidence that complements and modi-
ers are distinguished in an NP: their order, coordination, and pronominalization by
one dier. I will nish by summarizing the dierences that are the easiest to use, and
among these it is (a) and (b) that may be clearest. See also Table 9.2.
Table 9.4. Modiers and complements to N: a summary
Modiers Complements
a
All Ns may have modiers only certain Ns have complements:
those Ns that are verb-like
b gives general background information gives information pertinent to the N
c position is relatively free occurs either right before or right aer the N
d more than one are possible in one NP only one per NP
. Conclusion
In this chapter, we have discussed the dierent functions of elements inside the AdjP,
AdvP, PP, and NP. e functions include head, determiner, modier, and complement.
Not all phrases include all these functions, only the NP does. e other phrases are
less complex. e PP has a head and a complement, the AdjP a head, a modier, and a
complement, and the AdvP just a modier. e most important part is to recognize a
phrase and its head and to be able to draw a tree.
e functions of modier and complement are similar to the functions of
adverbial and object in the VP, discussed in Chapters 4 and 5, with the exception
of the names given and their optionality. is is not something you need to think
about further if it makes the matter more complex. e adverbial of the VP is called
modier when it occurs in the AdjP, AdvP and NP, and the dierent kinds of objects
in the VP are not dierentiated but just called complements in the AdjP, PP, and NP.
e NP may also contain a determiner where the VP has a subject. e complements
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
in the NP and AdjP are usually optional, whereas objects and predicates in the VP
are obligatory.
Key terms in Sections 1 and 2 are determiner, modier, head, and complement;
in Section 3, they are complement as opposed to modier; word order; pronominal-
ization; and coordination.
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
Exercises
A. In the sentences below, adapted from The Death of Ivan Ilych by Leo Tolstoy, find the PPs
that function as modifiers inside phrases, i.e. as modifiers to nouns or adjectives:
(43) During an interval in the Melvinski trial, the members and public prosecutor met
in Ivan Egorovich Shebek’s private room, where the conversation turned on the
celebrated Krasovski case.
(44) On receiving the news of Ivan Ilychs death, the first thought of each of the
gentlemen in that private room was of the changes and promotions it might
occasion among themselves or their acquaintances.
(45) Leaning against the wall in the hall downstairs near the cloak-stand was a
coffin-lid covered with cloth of gold, ornamented with gold cord and tassels.
B. Provide a tree structure for the following NPs (use NP, AdjP, D, etc.). Also list the functions
of the different elements.
(46) one of their irrational responses
(47) the attack on the conclusions of that report
(48) a hilarious look at two geniuses
(49) four fluffy feathers on a Fiffer-feffer-feff (from Dr. Seuss’s ABC)
C.
Provide a tree structure for the following sentences:
(50) This wonderful fridge is available in Montana.
(51) A very curious, red book with ink stains was found.
(52) He hides behind the pile of books on his desk.
(53) The lovely pig from Wyoming told the bureaucrat in Washington the story of his life.
Optional (Section 1)
D. In (6) above, repeated here as (54), we saw five adjectives in a row. Try to see how the
classes listed in (55) are ordered in relation to each other:
(54) The beautiful, large, fast, young, spotted leopard jumped out of nowhere.
(55) opinion size appearance speed age shape color origin material
pretty, ugly large soft, sweet fast old round pink Israeli golden
Now try to draw a tree for (54).
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Optional (Section 3)
E. Try to draw trees for (56) and (57) expressing the difference between complements and
modifiers. Which PPs and NPs are complements? Provide reasons for your answer:
(56) Canadian students of English
(57) a French Old English student.
Class discussion
F. My own favorite ambiguous NP is given in (58):
(58) The chocolate toy factory.
I can think of three interpretations and a few trees. Two of my favorite trees are as
follows. Discuss the difference!
(59)
a. NP b. NP
DN DN
The The
NP NAdjPN
factory Adj
AdjP N chocolateNPN
AdjN Nfactory
chocolate toy toy
Remember that sisters to N categories are complements, so that the NP complements to
the N factory would be the products of the factory, and that sisters to N nodes describe
physical properties of the factory.
G.
The first sentence of Exercise A is actually as in (60). Do you think there is ambiguity?
Is the trial modified by the PP that follows, or is that PP an independent adverbial?
(60) During an interval in the Melvinski trial in the large building of the Law Courts the
members and public prosecutor met in Ivan Egorovich Shebek’s private room,
where the conversation turned on the celebrated Krasovski case.
H.
Compare the NP in (61a) with the S in (61b). What are the similarities/differences?
(61) a. Stella’s destruction of that awful set of dishes.
b. Stella destroyed that awful set of dishes.
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
Keys to the exercises
A. The below PPs all modify an N:
(43) During an interval [in the Melvinski trial], the members and public prosecutor met
in Ivan Egorovich Shebek’s private room, where the conversation turned on the
celebrated Krasovski case.
(44) On receiving the news [of Ivan Ilych’s death], the first thought [of each [of the
gentlemen [in that private room]]] was of the changes and promotions it might
occasion among themselves or their acquaintances.
(45) Leaning against the wall [in the hall downstairs] (could also be Adverbial) near the
cloak-stand was a coffin-lid covered with cloth [of gold], ornamented with gold
cord and tassels.
B. The structure for (46) is as follows, with one as the head and of their irrational
responses as the modifier. In this phrase, the determiner one is functioning as noun
head (see Chapter 2 for other determiners that do this). A test for picking the head is
making the phrase into a subject and then checking the agreement on the verb (One
of their responses was to ... and not One of their responses were…):
(46)
NP
NPP (and if you want to show that irrational is a modifier,
one i.e. sister to an N:
PNP…N
of
DN
AdjP N
their
Adj N
AdjP N irrational responses)
Adj responses
irrational
In (47), the is the determiner, attack the head, and the rest is the complement (because attack is
a verb-like noun):
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(47)
NP
DN
the
NPP
attack
P NP
on
DN
the
NPP
conclusions
PNP
of
DN
that report
In (48), a is the determiner, hilarious the modifier, look the head, and the PP the complement:
(48)
NP
DN
a
AdjP N
Adj
Nhilarious PP
look
PNP
at
D geniuses
two
In (49), four is the determiner, fluffy is the modifier, feathers the head, and the PP is the (post-)
modifier. I could have shown that the PP is the modifier by making it a sister to an N but havent:
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
(49)
NP
DN
four
AdjP N
Adj
Nfluffy
AdjP
Adj
N
-fe
Fiffer-feffer
PP
feathers
PNP
on
DN
a
C. (50)
S
NP VP
DN V PP
This
AdjP NV AdjP P NP
Adj fridge is Adjin Montana
wonderful available
(51)
Sa.
NP VP
VGP
DN
A
AdjP
Adv
very curious
Adj
N
pass
was found
V
AdjP
Adj
N
Nred
book
PP
N
with
NP
AdjP
Adj stains
N
ink
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
There are other correct trees for (51). You could put red and book more closely together than
book and the PP, as I’ve done above. Also, if you wanted to express that the PP in (51) is a
modifier not a complement, you could make it sister to N rather than N, as in:
(51)
b. N
N N
N
book with ink stains
A structure for (52) is as follows. There are again other possible trees for (52), e.g. one could
indicate that the PPs are modifiers by making them sisters to N. Notice in (52) that on his desk
is modifying the pile of books. If it were an independent adverbial, it would mean that he hides
on his desk behind a pile of books:
(52)
S
NP VP
He
VPP
hides
P NP
behind
DN
the
NNP
pile
PNP
of
NPP
books
NNP
on
DN
his desk
A tree as in (53) is a challenge if you don’t have enough space. Doing it by hand (in draft form)
will save you a lot of time over drawing it using a computer. Note that I could have made the
PPs from Wyoming and from Washington sisters to an N, showing their modifier status:
Chapter 9. The structure of the PP, AdjP, AdvP, and NP 
(53)
NP
S
DN
T
he
AdjP N
Adj
lovely PPP
pig
PNP
Wyomingfrom
NPV
told
VP
DN
the the
PPP
bcrat
PNP
Washingtonfrom
NP
DN
PPP
story
of his life
D. The adjectives are usually seen as ordered in the way that I have listed the types
in (55). In (54), tall is an adjective of dimension, thin of physical characteristic, as is
strong, young is age-related, and clever expresses a value. The tree for the NP in (54) is
as below (and of course the last AdjP could be sister to an N too):
(54)
NP
DN
The
AdjP N
Adj
beautiful
AdjP N
Adj
large AdjP N
Adj
fast AdjP N
Adj
young
AdjP N
leopard
Adj
spotted
E. The structure for (56) is as in (38) above, with Canadian as modifier and of English as comple-
ment. One of the reasons is that you can say The Canadian one, but not the one of English:
(56)
NP
AdjP N
Adj
Canadian NPP
students
P
of English
NP
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
In (57), since Old is capitalized, it goes with English, and I have made it into a D, but modifier
would be ok too. The modifier is French and the complement is Old English:
(57)
NP
DN
a
AdjP N
Adj
French NP
student
N
Old
D
English
N
Special topic: Pronoun resolution
In this special topic, we’ll look at some problems that speakers and writers encounter when
they are deciding on which pronoun to use. The choice of the pronoun, of course, depends on
the antecedent, the phrase that the pronoun refers to. Some are easy, as in (62), but some are
ambiguous, e.g. the he in (63) could in principle go back to Obama or to Putin:
(62) I noticed a woman in that store. She was wearing a greenish purple jacket.
(63) Obama met Putin at the Kremlin. He wanted to discuss Afghanistan.
Writers are usually advised, as in (64), not to be ambiguous and sentence (63) would be
clearer using the former or the latter rather than he:
(64) Avoid ambiguity: There should not be two parties justifying even a momentary
doubt about which the pronoun represents. (Fowler 1926 [1950]: 464)
The use of the plural they and their to refer to a singular antecedent is still frowned upon. The
prescriptive rule could be formulated as (65) and an example appears in (66):
(65) Avoid ‘singular they’: do not use they, them, or their to refer to a singular antecedent.
(66) The student should be aware not to leave their computer unattended in the library.
‘Singular their’ in (66) is of course used because English lacks a gender-neutral third person
pronoun.
The OED is not prescriptive on this topic and lists the following function of they. It also
cites examples of this use from the 16th century:
(67) They: “Often used in reference to a singular noun made universal by every, any, no,
etc., or applicable to one of either sex (= ‘he or she’).
There are many other rules to help writers decide on the use of pronouns. In Chapter 10,
we’ll discuss, as special topic, the prescriptive rules for which relative pronoun to use for which
antecedent.
Chapter 10
Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs
1. Relative clauses (RC)
2. Inside the NP: Relative and complement
clauses
3. NPs as compared to AdjPs, AdvPs, and PPs
4. More on RCs
5. The structure of modiers
and complements (optional)
6. Conclusion
In Chapters 7 and 8, the functions of nite and non-nite clauses are discussed at
sentence level (e.g. as subjects or objects). e present chapter shows that clauses can
also function inside the phrase as modiers or complements. Traditionally, modier
clauses are called relative clauses and we'll continue that practice. Relative clauses
come in many kinds, as well see.
In Section 1, I provide a brief introduction to the shape and function of relative
clauses. In Section 2, examples are given of relative clauses and complement clauses
inside the NP. e non-nite reduced relative is also discussed. ese two rst sections
are the most important. In Section 3, we look at the internal structure of phrases other
than the NP, and in Section 4, we explore some dierent types of relative clauses. In
Section 5, we examine the tree structure.
.
Relative clauses (RC)
roughout the book, we have seen that PPs can function as adverbials and as modi-
ers. A PP used as modier has a function very similar to that of a relative clause, as
(1) and (2) show:
(1) e student [from Zombie Island] has yellow hair. (modication by PP)
(2) e student [who is from Zombie Island] has yellow hair. (modication by RC)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
A relative clause (RC) typically starts with a who, which, or that relative pronoun and
provides further information about a noun.
Let’s look at the short text in (3), from the Times of London, and see where the RCs are:
(3) Changes have to be made,” said a 34-year-old political activist [who asked to remain
anonymous]. Her rst target would be headscarves, [which are mandatory in Iran].
“e least of the freedoms [we need] is the ability to choose what to wear. For women
this is really an issue. Whenever you go out, you have to be vigilant because the moral
police may not think it is appropriate and they may even take you to jail. A womans
integrity is judged by the colour of your dress – well, isn’t that stupid?”
As you can see, I have put brackets around the three relative clauses. e rst modies
the noun an activist, the second headscarves, and the third freedoms. e third one has
the relative pronoun that or which le out, but you could always add it. When you look
at the entire text, there are lots of other clauses that are not relative, e.g. whenever you
go out is a nite clause functioning as adverbial and it is appropriate is a nite clause
functioning as direct object. So, be careful when you see a clause!
.
Inside the NP: Relative and complement clauses
In this section, we will divide the nite relative clauses into restrictive and non-restrictive.
We also add the complement clause and the non-nite reduced relative.
.
Relatives
Clauses that modify nouns, such as the one in (4), are referred to as relative clauses
because the noun they modify (stories in this case) plays a role (has a function) in the
RC. e RC is related to the noun by means of which:
(4) e stories [which he repeats oen] are boring.
e element that connects the noun and the clause, i.e. which in (4), is called a relative
pronoun. In (4), the relative pronoun functions as the direct object of repeat. Relative pro-
nouns can also function inside the relative clauses as subjects, as in (2), or have other
functions. e rst two relative pronouns in (3) are subjects. e third one lacks a rela-
tive pronoun but, if we added which, it would function as object.
RCs are usually divided into restrictive as in (4) and non-restrictive, as in (5) and (6):
(5) Hillary Clinton, who just returned from a trip to Cuba, intends to write a book.
(6) Queen Elizabeth the rst, who was born in 1533, was the last sovereign of the house
of Tudor.
e reason we discuss the dierence between restrictive and non-restrictive clauses is
that the use of one over the other has grammatical (and possibly other) consequences.
ree dierences between restrictive and non-restrictive relative clauses are listed
in Table 10.1. First, restrictive RCs can have a that, as in (7), or a who/which, as in (4).
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
In the non-restrictive RC (5) and (6), on the other hand, only a wh-pronoun occurs
and that is not possible, as (8) shows:
(7) e stories that he told us oen are boring. (restrictive)
(8) *Hillary Clinton, that just returned from a trip to (non-restrictive)
Cuba, intends to write a book.
e second dierence is that restrictive RCs provide essential information, unlike
non-restrictive ones. For instance, in (4), the stories is so general that the RC restricts
and species the stories that are meant. In the case of (5), everyone living in the US
at the beginning of the 21st century is expected to know who Hillary Clinton is and
therefore the NP Hillary Clinton does not need to be restricted. e RC just provides
background information that is not essential in knowing which noun is meant. ats
why it is called non-restrictive.
ird, since the information in non-restrictive RCs is background information, the
non-restrictive RC in (5) can be surrounded by commas or parentheses, and is therefore
sometimes referred to as a parenthetical, whereas the restrictive RC in (7) cannot be.
Table 10.1. Restrictive and Non-Restrictive RC
Restrictive Non-Restrictive
wh-pronoun or that only wh-pronouns
highly relevant information additional information
commas cannot surround it commas may surround it
As an illustration of the dierence, (9) and (10) are given. e restrictive RC in (9)
contrasts interestingly with a non-restrictive in (10). In (9), only a small set of climbers
reached the top, but in (10), all the climbers did:
(9) e hikers who reached the top were very tired. (restrictive)
(10) e hikers, who reached the top, were very tired. (non-restrictive)
(anks to Johanna Wood for the examples)
Another set that I sometimes use is (11) and (12). If you were a journalist, youd be ok
using the restrictive (11) but might get into trouble using the non-restrictive (12):
(11) Bankers that are crooks should be red. (restrictive)
(12) Bankers, who are crooks, work overtime nowadays. (non-restrictive)
Of course, you could substitute bankers with politicians, investors, house keepers, pro-
fessors, or doctors as well.
.
Complement clauses
ere is a type of clause that looks deceptively like the RC, namely the complement
clauses in (13) and (14):
(13) Reports [that he reached Mars] are exaggerated.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(14) e fact [that he reached Mars] went unnoticed.
e nite clause following the noun in (13) and (14) is a complement (and not a RC)
for several reasons. e rst is that noun (reports and the fact) can be le out, as in (15)
and (16):
(15) [at he reached Mars] is exaggerated.
(16) [at he reached Mars] went unnoticed.
e nouns can be deleted because the complement spells out what reports and the
fact are.
e second reason this clause is not a relative clause is that the head N reports
plays no role inside the clause. If we changed (13) to (17a), we would force the comple-
ment to be a relative clause. Now, reports (through that) is the object of reached but the
result is very strange (indicated by the question mark) since report is not an object you
would expect with reached. If we change the verb to read, as in (17b), we do get a RC
because one can read reports:
(17) a. ?Reports [that he reached] are exaggerated.
b. Reports [that he reads] are (always) exaggerated.
e third reason that the clause in (13) and (14) is a complement and not a RC is that
the complementizer has to be that. is that is not a relative pronoun. When we change
that in (13) and (14) to which, the result is very strange, indicated by an ungrammatical
mark in (18) and (19):
(18) *Reports which he reached Mars are exaggerated.
(19) *e fact which he reached Mars went unnoticed.
Table 10.2 summarizes the dierences between relative and complement clauses. I have
added a fourth one, namely that the type of noun complemented by a clause is quite
restricted, e.g. story, fact, dream, idea and concept.
Table 10.2. Relative Clauses and Complement Clauses
Relative Clause (RC) Complement Clause
relative pronoun has a function in the RC that has no role in the clause, as in (17)
relative marker: which, who, that, etc only that as marker, as in (18) and (19)
noun preceding RC cannot be deleted noun can be deleted (see (15) and (16))
any noun can have a RC follow noun is fact, story, idea, …
. Reduced relative clauses
Non-nite clauses, as in (20) to (22), just like their nite counterparts above, can be
modiers to N:
(20) e stories [to tell him] are the following.
(21) at story [written by him] is awful.
(22) e author [writing those marvellous books] lives in Antarctica.
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
ese non-nite clauses are called reduced relative clauses since one can paraphrase
them with full relative clauses. For instance, (20) to (22) are similar to the relative clause
structures in (23) to (25):
(23) e stories [which you need to tell him] are the following.
(24) at story [which was written by him] is awful.
(25) e author [who is writing those marvellous books] lives in Antarctica.
In cases where there is a PP present, as in (24), we can oen further reduce it, as in
(26), in which case the modier is a PP, not a (reduced) RC:
(26) at story [by him] is awful.
We don’t generally distinguish between restrictive and non-restrictive clauses in (20)
to (22). Unlike nite clauses, non-nite clauses cannot be complements to nouns.
Dont memorize this kind of information; just be able to analyze the structure of
phrases.
us, the functions of nite clauses inside the NP are complement and modi-
er. Modiers are referred to as relative clauses (RCs) and can be restrictive or non-
restrictive. Non-nite clauses only function as modiers and are referred to as reduced
relatives. Some examples of reduced RCs appear in Table 10.3.
Table 10.3. Examples of Reduced RC
Innitival e plumber [to nd the leak in the White House]
Present Participle e ship [exploring Antarctica]
Past Participle e book [written in an Edinburgh café]
A dictionary [plagiarized in 1803]
In this section, we have looked at clauses that go with a noun. e next section will
look at categories other than a noun that can be modied or complemented.
.
NPs as compared to AdjPs, AdvPs, and PPs
As weve seen, inside an NP, clauses can function as relatives (i.e. modiers) or comple-
ments, as (27) shows:
(27) [e man [who crossed Antarctica]] was happy.
Let’s look at the AdjP, AdvP, and PP.
Finite and non-nite clauses, as in (28) to (31), can be complements to AdjPs:
(28) ey were [happy [that he enjoyed his sugar-coated zucchini]].
(29) Are you [condent [that your full Social Security benets will be paid to you]]?
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(30) He was [unsure [what to do with the elephant in the room]].
(31) She was [proud [to have grown the largest blue eggplant]].
Since adjectives can be compared, we also have comparative clauses, as in (32) and
(33). ese clauses function as modiers since they indicate the degree of happiness
and niceness. With adjectives, dont worry too much about the dierence between
complements and modiers:
(32) Most people are as [happy [as they want to be]].
(33) She was [nicer [than I had thought]].
As shown in the previous chapter, AdvPs do not have complement or modier PPs.
ey have no clausal complements or modiers either. ey only can have degree
modication by another adverb.
Prepositions have complement clauses such as in (34) but do not generally admit
object clauses with a that complementizer, as (35) shows. Instead, a non-nite clause,
as in (36) or (37), appears:
(34) I relied [on [what he wrote about clauses]].
(35) *I insisted [on [that he/Stan should pay the bill]].
(36) I insisted [on [him/Stan paying the bill]].
(37) I insisted [on [his/Stan's paying (of) the bill]].
Some prepositions, such as before or aer in (38), do introduce a clause but, as I men-
tioned in Chapter 7, they are then complementizers rather than prepositions:
(38) He le [aer she arrived].
. More on RCs
In this section, well briey discuss a few other facts about English RCs. First, we’ll look
at relativized adverbials. en, we’ll look at prepositional and possessive relatives.
In Sections 1 and 2, most examples have the relative occupying a subject position
or an object position. ere is another frequently relativized function, namely that of
adverbial, such as in (39) to (41):
(39) e time [when you decide to discuss that] is important.
(40) e place [where you plan to live] is important.
(41) e reason [why/that I avoided that party] is clear.
e dierence between subject, object, and adverbial relative is in the relative pronoun
used. For subjects, who is used for humans, as in (2), (5), (6), (9), and (10) above; which
for non-humans, as in (4) above; and that for both if it is restrictive, as in (7). For (direct,
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
indirect, and prepositional) objects, whom is used for humans in very formal English,
who in less formal English; which for non-humans, and that in restrictive relatives for
both subject and object. Relativized adverbials, as in (39) to (41), use the relative pro-
noun most appropriate to the adverbial, when for a time adverbial, where for place, etc,
but can sometimes also use that.
RCs that relativize PPs occur and English has many options. ey can ‘strand’ the
preposition, as in (42), or ‘pied pipe’ it (as in the Rat Catcher of Hamelin), as in (43):
(42) e translation [which I insisted on] was unavailable.
(43) e translation [on which I insisted] was unavailable.
e relative pronoun can be le out, as in (44); and that can be used, but only when the
preposition stays in place, as in (45):
(44) e translation [I insisted on] went missing.
(45) e translation [that I insisted on] went missing.
When who is used, there are some other possibilities, depending on whether the objec-
tive marked whom is used; that is still a possibility too:
(46) e man [about whom she heard that rumor] is in prison.
(47) e woman [who(m) I heard this rumor about] is pleasant.
(48) e woman [that I heard this rumor about] is pleasant.
Possessives can be relativized too, as in (49). ey have an alternative as in (50), but
the use of whose is not restricted to human antecedents, as (51) shows. And theres an
alternative in (52):
(49) You start with S, [whose daughters are always NP and VP].
(50) You start with S, [the daughters of which are always NP and VP].
(51) e book, [whose author is well-known], was on NPR this morning.
(52) e book, [the author of which is well-known], was on NPR this morning.
. The structure of modiers and complements (optional)
Relative clauses and complement clauses have a structure very similar to those clauses
discussed in Chapters 7 and 8, with a CP, a C, and an S. e CP will be the same for all;
the crucial dierence between the dierent kinds of RCs and complement clauses is
how close the CP is to the noun, i.e. what they are sister to. In this section, I provide the
trees for some of these, namely for restrictive and non-restrictive RCs, complement
clauses, reduced relative clauses, and complements to adjectives.
Structures of NPs with restrictive and non-restrictive RCs are given in (53) and
(54) respectively. Structurally, the restrictive RC is closer to the head noun than the
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
non-restrictive. In (53) and (54), this is indicated by being sister to the N and to the
NP respectively:
(53)
NP
DN
e
N CP
N
woman CS
that
NP VP
I
V (woman
)
met
(54)
NP
NP CP
Zelda
CS
who
NP VP
I
V AdvP
Adv
V (Zelda
) well
know
To indicate the function of the modied noun inside the RC, a copy in parentheses is
introduced. It has a line through it to show that it is not pronounced there. In (53), for
instance, the woman is met, i.e. the woman functions as the direct object in the RC.
Similarly, in (54), Zelda is the object in the RC and a copy indicates that. I am focus-
sing on trees in which the copy is an object since they are the least complex. In the
(advanced) exercises, there will be other trees to draw.
Structurally, the restrictive RC is said to be closer to the N head but not as close as
the complement clause shown below. e non-restrictive RC is oen said to be sister
to the NP, i.e. outside the NP. In Chapter 3 (Section 3), a number of structures were
discussed that have a similar structure (with one NP branching to another), namely,
coordinated NPs and appositive NPs. Many grammarians have noticed the similarities
between non-restrictive RCs and appositive NPs, hence the alternative name of appos-
itive RC. us, as mentioned, the appositive NP We, the people of the United States, ...
could be rewritten as We, who are the people of the United States, …
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
Now, let’s turn to the clause with the closest connection to the noun, the comple-
ment clause as in (55). Note that the CP is sister to the N reports:
(55)
S
VP
V AdjP
are
Adj
exaggerated
NP
NCP
reports
CS
that
NP
VP
he
VNP
reached Mars
e reduced RC in (56) is similar to (53) but with more empty positions:
(56)
NP
DN
e
N CP
N
book CS
Ø
(book
)VP
VPP
written
by them
PNP
Finally, some trees for complements to adjectives are provided in (57) and (58), one
nite and the other non-nite:
(57)
AdjP
AdjCP
happy
CS
that
NP VP
he
V AdvP
went
Adv
away
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(58)
AdjP
AdjCP
unsure
CS
what
ØVP
VGP(what
)
inf V
to do
To summarize the dierences in the positions of the clauses in relation to their heads,
I provide Table 10.4.
Table 10.4. e sisters of CP
Complement Clause CP is sister to N Adjective Clause CP is sister to Adj
Restrictive RC
CP is sister to N
Reduced RC
CP is sister to N
Non-restrictive RC CP is sister to NP
. Conclusion
In Chapters 7 and 8, functions of nite and non-nite clauses were discussed at sen-
tence level, namely subject, direct object, adverbial, and subject predicate. In the cur-
rent chapter, we have discussed nite and non-nite clauses at phrase-level, namely
functioning inside an NP or AdjP. ese phrase-internal clauses have two functions:
modier and complement.
Sections 1 and 2 are the most important of the chapter. It is rst explained that nite
clauses that function as modiers are called relative clauses and can be divided into
restrictive and non-restrictive relatives. Clauses that function as complements to a lim-
ited set of nouns are also discussed as are non-nite clauses functioning as modiers,
namely those called reduced relative clauses. Section 3 examines clausal modiers and
complements to the P and Adj heads. Section 4 adds complexity to the RC by provid-
ing three additional varieties and Section 5 provides tree structures for the clauses
discussed in Sections 1, 2, and 3.
e key terms are relative and complement clauses; restrictive and non-restrictive
relative clauses; and reduced relatives.
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
Exercises
A. Label the clauses in (59) to (64) as complements or relatives and as nite or non-nite:
(59) The javelina [that I saw next door] was unafraid of coyotes.
(60) The report [that javelinas are dangerous] is exaggerated.
(61) Gerald, [who lives next door], will be leaving soon.
(62) The yellow fog [that rubs its back upon the window-panes]
(from T.S.Eliot’s Love Song for J.A.P)
(63) The president [that founded this organization] was arrested twice before he was
replaced.
(64) I am the man [to x this].
(65) The story [that Kissinger went to Moscow for Obama] seems true.
B. In the text below, from Harry Potter, there are three RCs. Find them and label them as
restrictive, non-restrictive, or reduced. If there are relative pronouns, what is their function
inside the RC?
Harrys feet touched road. He saw the achingly familiar Hogsmeade High Street: dark
shop fronts, and the outline of black mountains beyond the village, and the curve in
the road ahead that led o towards Hogwarts, and light spilling from the windows
of the Three Broomsticks, and with a lurch of the heart he remembered, with pierc-
ing accuracy, how he had landed here nearly a year before, supporting a desperately
weak Dumbledore; all this in a second, upon landing – and then, even as he relaxed
his grip upon Rons and Hermione’s arms, it happened. The air was rent by a scream
that sounded like Voldemorts when he had realized the cup had been stolen: It tore
at every nerve in Harrys body, and he knew immediately that their appearance had
caused it. (J.K Rowling, Harry Potter, volume 7: 554)
C.
Change one of the nite clauses in (63) into a non-nite one.
D. In the (challenging) text below, identify the relative clauses by putting brackets around
them:
To educate as the practice of freedom is a way of teaching that anyone can learn. That
learning process comes easiest to those of us who teach who also believe that there
is an aspect of our vocation that is sacred; who believe that our work is not merely to
share information but to share in the intellectual and spiritual growth of our students.
To teach in a manner that respects and cares for the souls of our students is essential
if we are to provide the necessary conditions where learning can most deeply and
intimately begin. (from bell hooks Teaching to Transgress, 1994: 13)
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Optional (Section 5)
E. Draw trees for (59) to (65).
Class discussion
F. What is the basic structure of (65)? (Don't draw a tree!) Which are the relative clauses?
(66) Shakespeare, Loves Labour's Lost, I, 2, 157
Armado: I doe aect the very ground (which is base) where her shooe (which is baser)
guided by her foote (which is basest) doth tread.
G.
Can clauses (relative or complement clauses) ever precede the head? If yes, give
examples. If no, give ungrammatical examples.
Keys to the exercises
A. In (59), that I saw next door is a (restrictive) RC which is nite; in (60), that javelinas are
dangerous is a nite noun complement; in (61), who lives next door is a (non-restrictive) RC
which is nite; in (62), that rubs its back upon the window-panes is a (restrictive) RC, also nite;
in (63), that founded this organization is a (restrictive) RC which is nite, and in (64), there is a
reduced (non-nite) RC. Sentence (65) contains a nite complement to the noun story.
B. The rst RC is restrictive (and nite), the second reduced (and non-nite), and the third
restrictive (and nite). The two relative markers function as subjects.
Harry’s feet touched road. He saw the achingly familiar Hogsmeade High Street: dark
shop fronts, and the outline of black mountains beyond the village, and the curve in the
road ahead [that led o towards Hogwarts], and light [spilling from the windows of the
Three Broomsticks], and with a lurch of the heart he remembered, with piercing accuracy,
how he had landed here nearly a year before, supporting a desperately weak Dumble-
dore; all this in a second, upon landing – and then, even as he relaxed his grip upon Rons
and Hermione’s arms, it happened. The air was rent by a scream [that sounded like Volde-
morts when he had realized the cup had been stolen]: It tore at every nerve in Harrys body,
and he knew immediately that their appearance had caused it.
C. The president that founded this organization was arrested twice before being replaced.
D. To educate as the practice of freedom is a way of teaching [that anyone can learn]. That
learning process comes easiest to those of us [who teach [who also believe that there is an
aspect of our vocation [that is sacred]]]; who believe that our work is not merely to share
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
information but to share in the intellectual and spiritual growth of our students. To teach
in a manner [that respects and cares for the souls of our students] is essential if we are to
provide the necessary conditions [where learning can most deeply and intimately begin].
It is not clear how the clause starting with who believe … is connected to the rest of the
sentence and that’s why I haven’t marked it.
E. (59)
(You could make ‘next’
an Adj)
S
DN
The
N CP
N
javelina CS
that
NP VP
I
V NP
V(javelina
)D N
saw next door
NP
V AdjP
was
Adj PP
unafraid
PNP
of N
coyotes
VP
In (60), you could also argue that exaggerated is the past participle form of the verb and part
of the VGP. Then, is will be an auxiliary, not a copula as in the tree below:
(60)
S
VP
V AdjP
is Adj
exaggerated
NP
DN
The
NCP
report
CS
that
NP VP
javelinas
V AdjP
are Adj
dangerous
In (61), I have indicated that soon and next door are adverbials by making them sisters
to V':
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(61)
S
VP
V AdvP
VGP Adv
soon
modal progr V
will be leaving
NP
NP CP
Gerald
CS
who
who VP
V NP
V
lives DN
next door
(62)
NP
DN
The
AdjP N
Adj
yellow N CP
N
fog CS
that
(fog
)VP
V
VNP
rubs
DN
its back
PP
PNP
upon
DN
the window-panes
(63)
S
NP
DN
The
N CP
N
president CS
that
(president
)
VP
VNP
founded
DN
this organization
VP
pass V
wasarrested
CP
CS
before
NP VP
he VGP
pass V
was replaced
V
V AdvP
VGP Adv
twice
Chapter 10. Clauses as parts of NPs and AdjPs 
In (63), I have expressed that twice and before he was replaced are adverbials by making them
sisters to Vʹs. (Note that VGP is used when auxiliaries are present, but that V suces when there
is just the lexical verb).
(64)
S
NP VP
I
V NP
am
DN
the
N
CP
N
man
CS
Ø
NP VP
(man
)
VGPNP
this
infV
to fix
(65)
S
VP
V AdjP
seems Adj
true
(and to Moscow could be sister to V
to show it is an adverbial)
NP
DN
The
NCP
story
CS
that
NP VP
Kissinger
V PP
VPP
for Obama
went
to Moscow
Special topic: Relative choice and preposition stranding
In spoken language, our most favorite relative marker is that. In writing, we predominantly use
a wh-pronoun. This shows that there are some strong prescriptive rules at work where relatives
are concerned. Fowler has denite ideas on all of these issues. In the older edition, he says:
“Relative pronouns are as troublesome to the inexpert but conscientious writer as they are
useful to everyone, which is saying much (1926 [1950]: 709). Other style books have similar
ideas. Three of these rules can be phrased as follows:
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(67) The case of the relative: “The case of the pronouns who and whom depends on
their function within their own clause. When a pronoun serves as the subject, use
who or whoever; when it functions as an object, use whom or whomever (Kirszner &
Mandell 1992: 376–7).
(68) The antecedent:who refers to people or to animals that have names. Which
and that usually refer to objects, events, or animals and sometimes to groups of
people” (Kirszner & Mandell 1992: 381).
(69) Do not strand prepositions.
The choice between nominative or accusative case has been talked about in the special topic
to Chapter 4, as well as briey in the chapter above. The only position where speakers still use
whom is directly following a preposition, as in (70):
(70) This is a man about whom I know very little.
The debate about the use of who, which, or that is a very lively one. Many argue that that can only
be used in restrictive relatives when the antecedent is non-human. Sentence (71) violates both:
(71) ‘was her Brother, that in pure kindnesse to his Horse, buttered his Hay.
(Shakespeare, King Lear II, 4, 128)
Fowler is careful about criticizing the use of that and thinks it will change (1926[1950]: 716)
at present there is much more reluctance to apply that to a person than to a thing. Politeness
plays a great part.
The dislike of stranding prepositions allegedly started with John Dryden. Many good’
writers employ constructions with stranded prepositions, and would rewrite (70) as (72) with a
stranded about. Sir Winston Churchill is said to have ridiculed the construction by uttering (73):
(72) This is a man (who) I know little about.
(73) This is something up with which I will not put.
Stranding prepositions does not just occur with relatives, but in questions as well, as in (74):
(74) who did I want to talk to?
Chapter 11
Special sentences
1. Questions/interrogatives: The CP
2. Exclamations
3. Topicalization, passive, cleft, and pseudo-cleft
4. Conclusion
In this chapter, I discuss sentences in which elements have moved around for a par-
ticular reason, e.g. to enable the speaker to ask a question, to make an exclamation,
or to emphasize something. e latter occurs through topicalization, passive, cle,
and pseudo-cle. Question sentences are referred to as interrogatives, whereas most
of the sentences we have seen up to now assert something and are called indicatives
or declaratives.
In Section 1, we look at questions, both yes/no and wh-questions and suggest a
tree for them using a CP. In Section 2, we briey cover exclamatives and how they
dier from questions. Section 3 goes into topicalization, passives, and cleing. Some
of the material in this chapter has been covered, e.g. questions and passives, but in
slightly dierent contexts.
1.
Questions/interrogatives: The CP
Questions can be main clauses (Will she leave?) or embedded clauses (I wonder if she’ll
leave). ey can also be classied according to whether the entire sentence is ques-
tioned, in which case a Ye s or No answer is expected, or whether another element is
questioned using a wh-word (also called an interrogative pronoun) such as who, what,
why, etc., in which case a full answer is expected.
In yes/no questions, the only appropriate answer is Yes or No (or Perhaps/maybe).
To make a question, e.g. of (1), the auxiliary has is fronted, as in (2):
(1) She has gone.
(2) Has she gone?
206 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
If there is no auxiliary present, a dummy do is used, as in (3), as discussed in Chapter 6:
(3) Did you see Santa?
A structure for yes/no questions is given in (4), where the auxiliary moves to C (indi-
cated by its copy le in the VGP that is not pronounced):
9
(4)
CP
CS
Can
NP VP
sheVGP
modal V
(can
)go
A main clause wh-question starts with a wh-word (who, what, why, when, where, and
how) or wh-phrase (which book, etc). e auxiliary follows. ere are empty positions
in the sentence, indicated by copies (crossed out because we do not say them out loud).
Examples are given in (5) to (7):
(5) Who will you (will)
see (who) in the Highlands?
(6) How heavy is that box of chocolates (is) (how heavy)?
(7) How much wood would a wood chuck (would) chuck (how much wood), if a wood
chuck could chuck wood?
Evidence that the question word was at some point in the position of the copy is that,
with special intonation, movement is not necessary. us, (8) is possible with empha-
sis on what. In this case the auxiliary stays in place too, as (8b) shows:
(8) a. You saw WHAT?
b. You would do WHAT?
Questions such as (8) are called ‘echo-questions
As to the structure, I will suggest (9) for wh-questions. e wh-word moves to the
position immediately underneath the CP and the auxiliary moves to the C position. e
original position of the wh-word and the auxiliary is indicated by means of a copy:
9.  Some people indicate that the sentence is a question by having a Q in C in (4) and in (9).
Chapter 11. Special sentences 207
(9)
CP
WhoC
CS
will
NP VP
you
V
VGP (who
)
modal V
(will
) see
PP
PNP
in
DN
the Highlands
Using C, C, and CP makes sense for these sentences and also makes the CP similar to
the VP, the NP, and the other categories that have a phrase, a head, and an intermedi-
ate category.
2.
Exclamations
Sentences such as (10) can be analyzed using a CP as well, namely as structures where
the phrase what a nasty person is in the same position as the wh-word in (9). Notice
that in sentences, such as (10) and (11), the auxiliary does not move, unlike in the
questions in (5) to (7):
(10) What a nasty person he is (what a nasty person)!
(11) “What an excellent father you have, girls,” said she, when the door was shut.
(Jane Austen, Pride & Prejudice, Chapter 2)
is was not always the case in English, as (12) shows. In (12), have has moved to
before the subject I, unlike is in (10) and have in (11):
(12) O what a Scene of foolry haue I seene. Of sighes, of grones, of sorrow, and of teene:
(Shakespeare, Loves Labors Lost, IV, iii, 163)
Modern English exclamatives dier from questions in not fronting the auxiliary, but
they do involve movement of a wh-phrase to a position in the CP.
208 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
3. Topicalization, passive, cleft, and pseudo-cleft
Even though the structures of topicalizations, cles, and passives look very dierent,
they have in common that the order of words is rearranged to emphasize a part of the
sentence. Examples such as (13) are similar to exclamations but occur without the
question word. I have marked where tomatoes comes from by means of an arrow. In
(14), the topic is preceded by as for, and repeated by a pronoun:
(13)
Tomatoes, I really don’t like __ in my cereal.
(14) and as for herself, she was too much provoked . . .
(Jane Austen, Emma, Vol 1, Chap 15)
Some topicalizations serve to front old information, which is convenient to listeners.
In the same way, passives and cles can shi phrases to put old information at the
beginning of a sentence and new information towards the end. In a passive, as seen in
earlier chapters, the subject she in (15) is the object her of the corresponding active in
(16). is shis the attention:
(15)
(16)
She was persuaded to go by Columbo. (passive)
Columbo persuaded her to go. (active)
Examples of a cle and pseudo-cle are given in (17a) and (18a). I have provided a
declarative version of each in (17b) and (18b). A cle starts with it is or it was and a
pseudo-cle starts with a wh-word:
(17) a. It was on the wedding-day of this beloved friend that Emma rst sat in mourn-
ful thought of any continuance. (Jane Austen, Emma, vol 1, Chap 1)
b. Emma rst sat in mournful thought of any continuance on the wedding-day of
this beloved friend.
(18) a. What he threw away was the winning lottery ticket.
b. He threw away the winning lottery ticket.
e cle picks out one phrase from the sentence to focus on, e.g. on the wedding-day
of this beloved friend in (17a). By doing so, it separates the phrase from the rest of the
sentence which becomes subordinate to the rst part. e pseudo-cle focuses on a
phrase by doubling it through a wh-word and then having the focussed phrase come
aer the copula.
Chapter 11. Special sentences 209
e structure of cles is that of a restrictive relative clause, as in (19). e structure
of pseudo-cles is controversial and will not be given:
(19)
S
NP VP
It
VPP
was
P NP
on
DN
the
N CP
NPPC S
wedding day that
Focus of Cle
of her …
NP VP
Emma
AdjP V
Adj
rst
V
PP
VPP
(on …
)
sat
in … thought
Note that I have represented an NP and PP by means of a coathangersince the internal
structure is not relevant here; and I havent lled in the words to some of the phrases.
is is for reasons of space.
4.
Conclusion
In this chapter, I briey describe a number of special constructions, where movement
seems to be taking place to achieve a special eect, namely questions, exclamations,
topicalization, passive, and cleing. Tree structures using a CP are provided for the
yes/no and wh-questions and the cle.
Key terms are questions (wh and yes/no); exclamations; topicalization, cle,
pseudo-cle, and passive.
210 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Exercises
A. Identify the special constructions in:
(20) It is his character that I despise.
(21) She was recognized going into the store.
(22) Higgins I hate.
(23) Who did Anne say that she saw?
B. Draw trees for:
(24) Will she go then?
(25) What will they think?
C. Explain the ambiguity in the following headline:
(26) Stolen Painting Found by Tree.
Keys to the exercises
A. Sentence (20) is a cleft; (21) a passive; (22) a topicalization, and (23) a wh-question.
B.
(24)
CP
CS
Will
NP VP
she
VGP
AdvP
Adv
modal Vthen
(will
)go
(25)
CP
What C
CS
will
NP VP
they
VGP (what)
modalV
(will
)think
C. The sentence can be a passive in which case the meaning is strange/funny since typically
trees are inanimate objects and don’t nd things. The intended meaning is not a passive,
Chapter 11. Special sentences 211
but one where by tree is a place adverbial, i.e. the painting was found by someone at the
site of a tree; the nder has been left out or is unknown.
Special topic: Comma punctuation
Commas are used in writing to indicate a slight pause in speech. Pauses help disambiguate
structural ambiguities, i.e. are syntactic in nature. The discussion below is not meant to be
exhaustive but merely discusses commas in connection to some of the constructions dealt with
in this book. The main use of commas is to indicate that some information is not crucial. Since
objects and complements are more important than modiers and adverbials, we don’t use
commas for the former but we may do so for modiers and adverbials (some people argue that
one must use them there).
Some specic rules are (26), (27), and (28):
(26) Commas are not used inside the core sentence to separate the subject, verb, and
object.
(27) Commas may be used for non-restrictive relative clauses and for adverbial
clauses but not for restrictive clauses. Sentence adverbials are always surrounded
by commas.
(28) Commas are not used between independent clauses. If they are, the construction
is called the comma splice.
As to (26), subjects, as in (29), cannot be separated from their verbs, even if the subject is a
clause, as in (30), and neither can commas appear before objects, as in (31), or before subject
and object predicates:
(29) *He, left.
(30) *That he didn’t want to meet the Dalai Lama, is true.
(31) *I noticed, that she …
I have marked these as ungrammatical but that is perhaps too radical since technically they
aren’t.
Commas are used for non-restrictive relative clauses, as in (32), sentence initial adverbials,
as in (33), but are not used for restrictive relative and complement clauses, as in (34):
(32) Pure Empiricism, which he was disposed not to accept, leads to scepticism.
(33) Fortunately, she was on an urban safari tour.
(34) *The story, that he met the Dalai Lama, is true.
The comma splice is well-known from composition classes. An example appears in (35):
212 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(35) Scientists think they have detected life on the Moon, visions of people living in
lunar colonies that stop o to refuel on the way to Mars can be envisaged.
Where would you put a period in (35) to x this?
I’ll now give some examples where pauses in speech and commas in writing do make a
dierence. The well-known (36) is ambiguous; it is sexist either way but illustrative. When (36) is
pronounced without pauses or written without commas, it is unclear:
(36) Woman without her man is a savage.
The two possible interpretations are either (37) or (38):
(37) Woman, without her, man is a savage.
(38) Woman, without her man, is a savage.
The tree structure of (38) is given in (39), with woman the subject and is a savage as the
predicate. The structure of (37) is more complex since woman is topicalized and, as shown in
(40), man is the subject and is a savage is the predicate:
(39)
S
VP
VNP
is
DN
a savage
NP
NPP
woman
PNP
without
DN
her man
(40)
S
NP S
N
(as for) womanS
NP VP
N
man VNP
is
D N
a savage
PP
PNP
without her
Chapter 11. Special sentences 213
A construction where there is no agreement about when to use commas in coordinating three
or more elements. Some argue that all commas should be present in (41), e.g. Fowler and
Oxford University Press; others argue the last can be left out. Allegedly, it once became the
matter of a law suit, when something like (42) appeared in a will:
(41) The books, magazines, and records in this store are on sale.
(42) Equal parts of the estate will go to Mary, Jane, Edward and Michael.
Apparently, Mary and Jane assumed they would each get a third and Edward and Michael
each a sixth, whereas Edward and Michael assumed each would get a quarter. I now put all
commas in!
Review of Chapters 9 to 11
In Chapters 9 and 10, the inner structure of the phrase is examined. PPs and AdvP
are the simplest: PPs have a head and a complement and AdvPs have a modier and
a head. AdjPs and NPs are more complex. e AdjP can have an Adj head, an Adv(P)
modier, and a complement; an NP can have a determiner, a head, several modiers
(both preceding and following the head) and one complement (either preceding or
following the head). DON’T memorize this; just be able to analyze a given sentence. A
relatively complex NP is given in (1):
(1)
NP
DN
the
AdjP N
Adj
typical
NPP
teacher
PNP
from
AdjPN
10
Adj
space
outer
In Chapter 10, we give names to the dierent kinds of clauses. ese include restrictive
and non-restrictive relative clauses, complement clauses, reduced relatives, and com-
plements to adjectives. Trees for these are provided in the last section of that chapter.
Chapter 11 gives examples of some special eect sentences such as topicalizations,
passives, questions, and cles. Just be prepared to recognize these. A tree for some of
these is also given in that chapter, using a CP, with a C and a C.
Rather than providing separate exercises, I include three homework assignments,
a short one that covers the special topics, a second one that I use regularly and that stu-
dents nd very helpful, and a third, very comprehensive and dicult one, that covers
Chapters 7 to 11 and that I oen use as a special credit assignment. No keys are given
because they are meant to be real homework or take home assignments. ere are also
three samples of nal exams covering the entire book. A key is provided on the web-
site. (I have tried to balance convenience and too much convenience by doing this).
10.  If you want to show that the PP in (1) is a modifier, make it sister to an N.
Review of Chapters 9 to 11 
Homework 1, on Chapter 1 and special topics
The book has discussed prescriptive grammar in Chapter 1 and has covered 11 special topics.
Some of these topics include well-known prescriptive rules, e.g. the one on the split innitive
(Chapter 1), the multiple negative (Chapter 3), the dangling participle (Chapter 8), and
preposition stranding (Chapter 10). Some topics help explain a grammatical category, for
instance, the adjective and adverb (Chapter 2) or the complementizer (Chapter 7) and how these
categories change over time. Case and agreement phenomena (Chapter 4) are very relevant to
the grammar of English and the changes it is undergoing while other phenomena are stylistic
choices, such as the use of the passive (Chapter 5). Thus, in an academic paper, one must use the
correct agreement on the verb but, depending on the eld, the use of the passive is ne.
Choose one of the special topics and show by means of examples from your own writing
whether or not you follow the prescriptive rule. Then, choose an audience (perhaps rst year
students taking a composition class) and write a short (200 words) explanation on how, and
possibly how not, to use this construction.
Homework 2, covering Chapters 2–11
Take a page of your own writing (an academic paper might be the easiest) and analyze it in
terms of the grammatical structures it uses. For instance, we have seen that Hemingway ((76) in
Chapter 5) uses relatively short sentences with copulas and that Tolstoy ((43) to (45) in Chapter
9) uses lots of PP modiers. Use similar observations about your own writing.
To analyze your own writing, consider the ten points below. You could mark the page of
your writing in a dierent color or use multiple copies of your text. Then write a paragraph
summarizing what you see. If you think it is easier to compare your writing to someone else’s,
do that, still using these points.
A. Modication of nouns. Do you use a lot of adjectives and PPs as modiers?
B. Types of verbs used? Do you use a mixture of intransitives, transitives, copulas, and other
types?
C. Do you use coordination? If so, which coordinators do you use and is your coordination
of phrases or of clauses?
D. Adverbials. Are there many adverbials and, if so, which kinds (clausal or PP)?
E. Auxiliaries.
F. Passive.
G. Embedded clauses. What functions do they have?
H. Finite as opposed to non-nite clauses.
I. Dummy elements.
J. Split innitives, dangling modiers, or anything else banned by prescriptive rules?
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(For fun, you could use http://textalyser.net. This site will give you the frequency of words in a
text you want).
Homework 3, or take-home exam, covering Chapters 7–11
In the text below, taken from Thomas Kuhn:
A. Locate the relative clauses and indicate whether they are restrictive or non-restrictive.
B. Find all the nite verbs and indicate whether or not they are lexical.
C. Analyze the last sentence of the rst paragraph in terms of basic sentence structure.
Try to draw a tree.
D. Draw a tree for the NP its practitioners’ insistence . . . being considered (second paragraph)
E. How might one analyse a sentence with if as in (last but one sentence in the second
paragraph).
Anyone who studies the history of scientic development repeatedly encounters a question, one
version of which would be, Are the sciences one or many?” Ordinarily that question is evoked by
concrete problems of narrative organization, and these become especially acute when the
historian of science is asked to survey his subject in lectures or in a book of signicant scope.
Should he take up the sciences one by one, beginning, for example, with mathematics, proceeding
to astronomy, then to physics, to chemistry, to anatomy, physiology, botany, and so on? Or
should he reject the notion that his object is a composite account of individual elds and take
it instead to be knowledge of nature tout court? In that case, he is bound, insofar as possible, to
consider all scientic subject matters together, to examine what men knew about nature at each
period of time, and to trace the manner in which changes in method, in philosophical climate, or
in society at large have aected the body of scientic knowledge conceived as one.
Given a more nuanced description, both approaches can be recognized as long-traditional
and generally noncommunicating historiographic modes. [note deleted] The rst, which treats
science as at most a loose-linked congeries of separate sciences, is also characterized by its
practitioners’ insistence on examining closely the technical content, both experimental and
theoretical, of past versions of the particular specialty being considered. That is a considerable
merit, for the sciences are technical, and a history which neglects their content often deals with
another enterprise entirely, sometimes fabricating it for the purpose. On the other hand, historians
who have aimed to write the history of a technical specialty have ordinarily taken the bounds of
their topic to be those prescribed by recent textbooks in the corresponding eld. If, for example,
their subject is electricity, then their denition of an electrical eect often closely resembles the
one provided by modern physics. With it in hand, they may search ancient, medieval, and early
modern sources for appropriate references, and an impressive record of gradually accumulating
knowledge of nature sometimes results. (from Kuhn “Mathematical versus Experimental
Traditions in the Development of Physical Science”)
Review of Chapters 9 to 11 
Examples of nal exams
Example 1
This exam is based on a text adapted from The Games Helmet (the London Sunday Times,
25 Nov 2007).
A. Label the categories (N, V, D, etc.) in the sentence below:
(1) The fact that an advanced system has been developed
for computer games should come as no surprise.
B. Label the six lexical verbs (Intransitive, etc) that are underlined:
Think carefully before you answer: is a device that is capable of reading peoples minds
fact or fantasy? We knew youd say that. But scientists at an American laboratory have
been brainstorming the same question for more than ve years and have thought up
a mind-blowingly dierent answer. They call it Epoc, but when it is launched early next
year in Britain and the US we will probably give it the name of “mind-reading helmet”,
capable, supposedly, of knowing what users are thinking.
is: reading:
thought: call:
give: knowing:
C. Identify the functions of the eight underlined and numbered phrases (Su, Dir Object, etc.
or Modier):
The device is being hailed as a revolutionary breakthrough in the way that humans
will be able to interact (1) with computers. Its implications are (2) massive, opening
the possibility that one day (3) people will be able to control everything from light
switches to the cursor on their computer screen simply by thinking about it. (4)
However, for now, the technology will be used as the ultimate gimmick: to play
computer games simply by thinking your screen character (5) into action. Emotiv
Systems, the San Francisco company that has developed the technology, says Project
Epoc could mean the end of joysticks and keyboard bashing. Instead games players
will be able to visualise a move in their head and that move will be replicated (6)
on the screen in front of them. So, for example, Harry Potter could be ordered to
cast exotic spells, or a jedi might exert the force to ing (7) his enemies around – all
through the willpower (8) of the gamer, with no buttons pressed.
D.
AND name these phrases (e.g. NP, PP):
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
Function (e.g. Adverbial) Name (e.g. NP, PP)
1………………………………………………………………………
2………………………………………………………………………
3………………………………………………………………………
4………………………………………………………………………
5………………………………………………………………………
6………………………………………………………………………
7………………………………………………………………………
8………………………………………………………………………
E. Draw a tree for:
(2) Scientists at an American laboratory brainstormed the question for ve years
F. Circle the lexical verbs and underline the auxiliaries in the text. Which are nite?
So how does the helmet work? In simple terms it relies on the fact that every time a
human thinks about something, electrical impulses are triggered in the brain. This has
been known for years in the medical world and is the basis of an electroencephalogram
(EEG) – the technique that measures the electrical activity of the brain by recording from
electrodes placed on the scalp. Emotiv claims to have rened the technique to isolate
and identify the electrical patterns that are given o when humans think about a given
course of action, such as moving their arm to the left or right or depressing their right
thumb or index nger. The Epoc helmet recognizes these electrical patterns and
translates them into “real” movements on the screen.
G.
Identify the function and type of the clauses in brackets, e.g. modier/reduced RC,
Subject/non-nite, etc. in:
[To look at], the helmet resembles nothing so much as a novelty head massage gadget
with several spidery arms [curving around the head and meeting at the top]. The arms
are tted with a total of 16 sensors [that are positioned so that they are in contact with
the relevant part of the head and pick up electric signals in the brain]. The systems
software analyzes these signals and then wirelessly relays what it detects to a receiver
plugged into the USB port of the game console or PC. Emotiv says that it has mapped
12 specic actions [that the helmet will recognize].
H.
Draw a tree for:
(3) Emotiv says that it has mapped specic actions to use in its program.
I. Circle the phrasal verbs, if any, and underline the passives, if any, in:
If true, the implications are huge. Gaming is believed to merely be the way to popularize
the technology rather than being an end in itself: the tip of the virtual iceberg. In the long
Review of Chapters 9 to 11 
run the headset could be used widely, from the use of brain scanners as lie detectors
to see whether suspects can make out a crime scene, to enabling consumers to turn
machines on or o or change television channels without a remote control.
Example 2
Please read the following text. Most questions are based on it. It is adapted from The
New York Times, 4 December 1996, but even as late as 2009, the debate on ice/water on the
moon continued.
The Moon May have Water
Scientists think they have detected water on the Moon. Suddenly, visions of people living
in lunar colonies that stop o to refuel on the way to Mars are less far-fetched. After two years
of careful analysis, scientists said yesterday that radar signals from an American spacecraft
indicated the moon was not bone-dry. The spacecraft’s radar signatures suggested the presence
of water ice in the permanently cold shadows of a deep basin near the lunar south pole.
The survey revealed a vast landscape in which ice crystals are mixed with dirt. It seems a
kind of permafrost that is presumably the residue of moisture from comets striking the Moon
over the last three billion years.
Even though scientists are not positive, they see signals consistent with ice. Dr. Paul
Spudis, one of the scientists reporting on the discovery, acknowledged that the discovery
needed to be conrmed by an independent investigation. That might come a year from now
because then another spacecraft will orbit the Moon with instruments of even greater precision
for determining the presence of lunar water.
This discovery gives astronauts hope for longer stays in space. Told of the new discovery,
Dr. Story Musgrave was very enthusiastic. He said that this implied there might be water and
water is extraordinarily important to establishing a permanent base on the Moon. Other scientists
reacted to the report with a mixture of caution and enthusiasm. They noted that the radar results
were particularly dicult to interpret.
A.
Identify all the categories in (1), e.g. D, N, etc.:
(1) a kind of permafrost that is presumably the residue of moisture from comets
striking the Moon over the last three billion years.
B.
List all PPs used as adverbials in the rst paragraph (or underline them clearly in the text).
C. Indicate function and name (or realization) of the phrases at sentence/clause level in the
sentences/clauses below, e.g. the world is round: SU: NP/Pred: VP/SubjPr: AdjP. DO NOT
ANALYSE THESE UNITS ANY FURTHER.
(2) Suddenly, visions of people living in lunar colonies that stop o to refuel on the
way to Mars are less far-fetched.
 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
(3) The survey revealed a vast landscape in which ice crystals are mixed with dirt.
(4) another spacecraft will orbit the Moon with instruments of even greater
precision for determining the presence of lunar water.
(5) This discovery gives astronauts hope for longer stays in space.
D. Locate all non-nite clauses in the third paragraph. List them here or underline them
clearly in the text.
E.
What is the function and name of the following phrases in the structures in which they
occur (e.g. Su/NP):
(6) positive (l. 10)
(7) consistent with ice (l. 10)
(8) Told of the new discovery (l. 15)
(9) that the radar results were particularly dicult to interpret (ll 18–9)
F. List the modiers in the fourth paragraph. Also indicate what their name is (e.g. PP, CP, etc.)
G. List all auxiliaries. Indicate what kind they are (perfect . . .)
H. Draw trees for (10) and (12):
(10) After two years of careful analysis, scientists said yesterday that radar signals from
an American spacecraft indicated the moon was not bone-dry.
(11) Paul Spudis acknowledged that the discovery needed to be conrmed by an
independent investigation.
Example 3
Please read the following text, A Life of Fiction, adapted from Jane Smiley (New York Times
Magazine, 3/12/00)
When Charles Dickens was traveling home from France in June 1865, the train he was
riding in went o the tracks while crossing a bridge over a river. Seven rst-class carriages
dropped into the river. The eighth, which was the one Dickens was travelling in, dangled o the
bridge. Dickens calmed his companions and clambered out. He was indefatigable and helped to
free his friends in the carriage and many others.
When all that could be done for the victims had been done, Dickens, who was 53 years old
and not in very good health, climbed into the carriage again and retrieved from the pocket of his
coat the installment of ‘Our Mutual friend’ he had just nished.
The author, who hadn’t shrunk from describing the lurid and the terrible before, made
no eort to describe what he had seen. “I don’t know what to call the accident” he wrote to a
friend. He also refused to give testimony to the subsequent inquest. Why did Dickens hide his
heroism? It so happens that Dickens’ traveling companions were his mistress Ellen Ternan and
Review of Chapters 9 to 11 
her mother. What is really interesting is that a man whose volume of writings approach logorrhea
could dissemble his most intimate concerns and feelings so consistently and for so long.
A.
List all adverbials in the second paragraph.
B. Indicate function and name of the phrases/clauses at sentence level, e.g. Su/NP;
Adverbial/PP in the sentences below. Do not go further than the rst layer:
(1) I don’t know what to call the accident
(2) When all that could be done for the victims had been done, Dickens, who was
53 years old and not in very good health, climbed into the carriage again
(3) … helped to free his friends in the carriage and many others
C. What is the function and name of the following:
(4) his mistress … mother (ll. 12–3)
(5) testimony (l. 11)
(6) o the bridge (l. 3–4)
(7) a man … logorrhea (l. 13)
D. List all auxiliaries in the second paragraph. Indicate what kind they are.
E. List all nite verbs in the third paragraph.
F. Indicate the relative clauses in the rst and second paragraphs. Are they restrictive,
non-restrictive, or reduced?
G.
Draw trees for:
(8) When Charles Dickens was traveling home from France in June 1865, the train he
was riding in went o the tracks while crossing a bridge over a river.
(9) Why did Dickens hide his heroism?
Glossary
At the end of each chapter, there is a list of key terms. These are the most relevant and
should be understood. The glossary tries to be somewhat comprehensive, and lists key
terms, abbreviations, non-key terms, and some common terminology not used in this book,
e.g. attributive adjective, but perhaps used elsewhere. Don’t attempt to memorize the glos-
sary! There is also an index to the book so, if the definitions in the glossary do not suffice,
check the index and read the relevant pages.
accusative case The case of the object or
prepositional object, only visible on pronouns
in English, e.g. me, in He saw me, also called the
objective case.
active A sentence in which the doer of
the action is the subject, as in She guided the
elephant.
Adj = adjective.
Adj
Adjective-bar, intermediate category,
see Chapter 9, Section 1.
adjective A word which often describes
qualities, e.g. proud, intelligent, or physical
characteristics, e.g. short, strong.
adjective complement Complement to an
adjective, e.g. of him in proud [of him]; see
Chapter 9, Section 1.
AdjP = Adjective Phrase: group of words
centered around an adjective, e.g. very nice.
adjunct Term not used in this book; alterna-
tive for adverbial’.
Adv = adverb, i.e. the category
Adv-ial = adverbial, i.e. the function
adverb E.g. proudly; it is similar to an adjec-
tive but it modies a verb, adjective, or other
adverb, whereas an adjective modies a noun.
adverbial A function at sentence level
providing the background on where, when,
how, and why the event described in the VP
takes place.
AdvP = Adverb Phrase: group of words cen-
tered around an adverb, e.g. very nicely.
ax Cannot stand on its own, e.g. an ending
such as -ing; see Table 6.2.
ax-hop Process where an ax belonging
to an auxiliary ‘hops’ and attaches to the verb
immediately to the right of the auxiliary; see
Chapter 6 and Table 6.2.
agreement E.g. -s in she walks, ending on
the verb that ‘agrees’ with the subject.
ambiguity/ambiguous Word (lexical ambi-
guity) or sentence (structural ambiguity)
with more than one meaning; see Chapters
1 and 3.
antecedent What a pronoun refers to, e.g.
the noun that a relative pronoun such as who
refers to in the man who(m) I saw; see Chapters
9 and 10. Antecedent is used more generally
though for any pronoun that refers to a noun.
antonym A word with the opposite mean-
ing, e.g. hot/cold, good/bad
; Chapter 2.
appositive NP The second NP in Teguci-
galpa, the capital of Honduras; see Chapter 3. It
rephrases the rst and provides extra informa-
tion; similar to a non-restrictive relative clause.
appositive relative clause Another word for
non-restrictive relative clause; see Chapter 10
and below.
article A, an, the in English; see Chapter 2,
Section 2.1.
aspect When the type or duration of the
action is emphasized, as in he is reading, rather
than when the action took place, Chapter 6.
attributive adjective Term not used in this
book; an adjective that modies a Noun inside
an NP.
AUX = auxiliary, see below.
Glossary 223
auxiliary A ‘verb’ that cannot stand on its
own, but that ‘helps’ (combines with) another
verb, e.g. have in They have seen a riot; see
Chapters 2 and 6 and Table 6.1.
bare innitive Innitive without a to; e.g.
leave in I saw her leave, see Chapter 8.
branch A line that marks the relationship
between two nodes in tree; it shows how a
phrase is divided up; see Chapter 3.
C = complementizer, see below.
C
= C-bar, intermediate category, only used
in Chapter 11.
case In English, case is only visible on pro-
nouns. Thus, she in She saw me has nomina-
tive case, i.e. is used in subject position, and
me has accusative or objective case, i.e. is
used in object position.
clause Unit containing a lexical verb, see also
main clause, subordinate clause.
cleft A construction of the form It is Catwea-
zle who caused the problems; see Chapter 11.
coathanger Not dividing a phrase into sepa-
rate branches (to save space); used rarely in
this book.
comma splice A comma between two inde-
pendent clauses; see extra topic Chapter 11.
comparative Forms such as greater that
compare one situation or entity with another.
complement There are complements to V, N,
Adj, and P. Complements to VPs are divided into
direct and indirect object, subject predicate,
object predicate, prepositional and phrasal
object. Nouns, adjectives, and prepositions can
also have complements.
complementizer E.g. that, if, and whether;
connects two clauses, one subordinate to the
other; see Chapters 2 and 7.
complex transitive A verb with a direct object
and an object predicate; see Chapter 4.
compound When two words are put
together to form one word, e.g. blackboard
and sleepwalk.
conjunction Not generally used in this book,
except in quotes and to indicate an alterna-
tive phrasing. It is a general term to describe a
word that joins two or more words or phrases
or sentences together. There are subordinat-
ing (that) and coordinating (and) conjunctions;
see Chapter 2.
consonant Sound such as b, p, f, v, t, k, made
by somehow modifying the airstream; see
Chapter 1, 1.1. for use in a rule.
constituent Not used in this book; a group
of words that form a unit, typically a phrase.
contraction A word that is shortened, e.g.
he’s for he has; see special topic Chapter 6.
coordination Connecting two phrases or
clauses that are equal to each other by means
of e.g. and; see Chapter 3.
coordinating
conjunction Not used in this
book; same as coordinator, see there.
coordinator Connects two phrases or
clauses that are equal to each other, e.g. and/
or, also called coordinating conjunction; see
Chapter 2.
copula A verb with a subject predicate, typi-
cally to be or to become, see Chapter 4.
corpus A set of texts collected to provide a
representative view of the language of a par-
ticular time, social group, or genre.
CP Complementizer Phrase, see Chapter 7
and 8.
D = determiner.
(D)Adv Degree adverb, see below.
dangling modifier An adverbial clause
whose subject is not the same as the sub-
ject of the main sentence, see special topic
Chapter 8.
daughter For example, P is a daughter of a
PP, i.e. lower in the tree but connected to the
‘mother’ by a branch; see Chapter 3.
declarative A neutral sentence that is a
statement, not a question or command.
denite article The article the.
degree adverb Adverbs that indicate degree,
e.g. very, too, so, more, most, quite, rather; see
Chapter 2.
descriptivism Describing what language
users really say, as opposed to what they
should’ say; see Chapter 1.
224 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
determiner Word that points or species, e.g.
the; see Chapter 2, Section 2.1.
direct object Object of a verb such as eat,
see, and enjoy. For instance, him in They saw
him.
ditransitive Verb that has both a direct and
indirect object, e.g. tell, give; see Chapter 4.
dummy A word used to fulll a grammatical
requirement, see dummy do and dummy sub-
ject below.
dummy do
If no auxiliary is present in a sen-
tence, do is used with questions and negatives;
see Chapter 6.
dummy subject If a subject is not present, it
or there are used, also see pleonastic subject;
see special topic to Chapter 5.
E = Event time, used in Chapter 6.
echo question Question formed by special
intonation, as in You saw WHAT
? See Chapter 11.
ed-participle See past participle.
elided/elision Word or phrase left out to
avoid repetition after a coordinator, e.g. in He
wrote a poem and painted a pictures, the sub-
ject of painted has been left out.
ellipsis Word or phrase left out to avoid
repetition.
embedded
sentence/clause A clause or sen-
tence inside another phrase or sentence/clause;
see Chapter 7.
emphasizer Words such as even and just that
are used to emphasize a phrase, also called
focusser; see Chapter 9.
exclamation or
exclamative Sentences such
as What a fool he is!! See Chapter 11.
extraposed/
extraposition When an embed-
ded clause (usually in subject position) is
placed at the end of the sentence, e.g. It was
nice [that he left]. A dummy subject it is put in
the original position.
nite clause A clause with a nite verb
(see below) and a nominative subject; see
Chapter 7.
nite verb A verb expressing agreement
and tense (past or present), e.g. has in He has
left; see Chapters 6 and 7.
at structure A tree that does not express
hierarchies because many braches descend
from one node; see Chapter 3, Section 1.
focusser Words such as even and just that
are used to emphasize a phrase, also called
emphasizer; see Chapter 9.
formal language Language used in formal
situations such as ceremonies, formal lec-
tures, or meeting a government official; see
Chapter 1.
fragment An incomplete sentence, i.e. one
missing a nite verb.
functional category Not used in this book,
alternative to grammatical category.
functions Phrases (and clauses) have func-
tions, such as subject and direct object, at the
level of the sentence. There are also functions
inside the phrase, namely as determiner, modi-
er, and complement. See Chapters 4 and 5 for
functions at sentence level and Chapters 9 and
10 for functions at phrase level.
future (tense) E.g. will see in They will see the
new bridge on their trip.
future perfect E.g. will have done in They will
have done that by 5; see Chapter 6, Figure 6.1.
genitive case The case that a possessive has,
e.g. Catweazle’s in Catweazle’s book; see special
topic Chapter 4.
gerund A verbal noun that ends in -ing,
briey discussed in Chapter 8 and special topics
Chapter 8.
gradable Not used in this book. Adjective
that can be modied in terms of degree, e.g.
very happy, happier.
grammar The rules to form and understand
language. In this book, we focus on how to
analyze sentences, rather than full texts, words,
or sounds. We also focus on descriptive, rather
than prescriptive rules.
grammatical A sentence (or word) that native
speakers consider acceptable.
grammatical category Word with little
lexical meaning, e.g. Determiner, Quantier,
Auxiliary, Coordinator and Complementizer;
see Chapter 2.
Glossary 225
head The most important part of a unit/
phrase, e.g. the N seadog is the head of the NP
the blue seadog.
hypercorrection When speakers are so con-
scious that a prescriptive rule exists that they
make a mistake. The use of between you and I is
a good example.
inf = innitival marker to; see Chapter 8.
ing
-participle See present participle.
imperative A command such as Go away,
shut up! See Chapter 6, Section 3.
indenite article The articles a and an.
indicative A normal’ sentence, i.e. not ask-
ing a question, indicating a wish or command.
Declarative is also used.
indirect object Object that can be preceded
by to or for, e.g. Doris in Clovis gave Doris a
ower; see Chapter 4.
innitive Form such as to go, to be, to ana-
lyze; it is one of the non-nite constructions.
See Chapter 8.
informal language Language used in infor-
mal situations such as casual conversation. In/
formality depends on the situation, the partici-
pants, the topic. See Chapter 1.
innate faculty Enables us to acquire lan-
guage; see Chapter 1.
interrogative
pronoun Pronouns that
start a wh-question such as who left; see
Chapter 11. In form, they are similar to rela-
tive pronouns.
interrogative sentence A question such as
who will go there; see Chapter 11.
intransitive A verb without an object, e.g.
laugh, swim; see Chapter 4.
irregular verbs The past tense and past par-
ticiple of these verbs are not formed by add-
ing -ed to the present, as in the case of regular
verbs. Some examples of irregular verbs are:
go, went, gone; see, saw, seen; write, wrote, writ-
ten. See Chapter 6.
lexical category Word with lexical meaning,
such as a Noun, Verb, Adjective, Adverb, and
Preposition; see Chapter 2. It has a synonym
and antonym.
lexical verb Verb that can stand on its own,
e.g. see, walk; see Chapters 2 and 6.
light verb Verbs such as make, do, take
with a very general meaning that combine
with nouns, such as take a walk. They can be
replaced by verbs, e.g. walk; see Chapter 4.
linguistic knowledge Knowledge about lin-
guistic notions and rules that we have in our
heads, e.g. consonants and vowels, structure,
question formation; see Chapter 1.
linguistics The study of language.
main clause Independent clause, i.e. a sen-
tence that can stand on its own, minimally
containing a subject and a predicate and not
embedded within another clause.
modal Auxiliary such as must, will, would,
can, could that expresses necessity, uncer-
tainty, possibility; see Chapter 6.
modier An element whose function is to
provide more information on another element,
e.g. purple in purple sage; see Chapter 9.
modify Describe the quality of something.
monotransitive See transitive.
morphology Rules for how to build words,
e.g. formal + ize; see Chapter 1.
mother In a tree, the node above another
node, e.g. PP is the mother of P.
multiple negation When two or more negative
words (not, nobody) occur in the same clause, e.g.
I didn’t eat nothing; see special topic Chapter 3.
N = noun.
N
= N-bar, intermediate category; see
Chapters 3 and 9.
negation/negative E.g. not or n’t, or a negative
word such as nothing.
node a point in the tree, e.g. NP is a node,
see Chapter 3.
nominative case The case of the subject,
only visible on pronouns, e.g. she in She left
early; see special topic Chapter 4.
non-nite clause A clause lacking a nite
verb; see Chapter 8.
non-nite verb A verb that lacks tense and
a nominative subject, e.g. to be in to be or not to
be is the question; see Chapters 6 and 8.
226 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
non-linguistic (or social) knowledge Knowl-
edge of social rules; see Chapter 1, Section 3.
non-restrictive RC A clause that provides
background information to the noun it modies;
is often set apart from the rest of the sentence
through commas or comma intonation; see
Chapter 10.
noun A word such as table, freedom, book,
love; see Chapter 2.
noun complement Of chemistry in teacher of
chemistry; see Chapter 9.
NP = Noun Phrase, group of words cen-
tered around a noun, e.g. the red balloon; see
Chapter 3.
numeral A word such as one or two. They
can be seen as Adj or D; see Chapter 2 and
Table 2.4.
objective or accusative case In English, case
is only visible on pronouns, e.g him, in Hermi-
one saw him. Objects typically get this case,
hence the name objective. See special topic,
Chapter 4.
object predicate Realized as an AdjP, NP, or
PP, making a claim about the object, e.g. nice
in I consider her nice. It occurs together with a
complex transitive verb such as consider, elect;
see Chapter 4.
OED Oxford English Dictionary, see references.
P = preposition.
participle Either accompanied by an aux-
iliary, see Chapter 6, or on its own heading a
non-nite clause, see Chapter 8.
particle Similar in form to prepositions and
adverbs, but a particle is only used together
with a verb; see Chapter 5.
passive auxiliary A form of to be used
together with a past participle. For instance,
was in She was arrested; see Chapter 6.
passive construction A construction where
an undergoer of the action is functioning
as a subject, e.g. she in She was met at the
airport.
past (tense) An event took place in the past,
i.e. the speech time and event time do not
overlap.
past participle Typically follows auxiliary to
have to form a perfect, or to be to form a pas-
sive. It can function on it own in a non-nite
clause. The participle ends in -ed/-en (walked,
written, chosen) or may be irregular, such as
gone, swum, begun, learnt.
past perfect E.g. had done in She had done
that by ve; see Chapter 6, Figure 6.1.
perfect auxiliary To have when used
together with a past participle. For instance,
have in I have done that already.
phonology The structure of the sound sys-
tem; see Chapter 1.
phrasal verb A verb that is always com-
bined with a preposition-like element but
which has a special meaning. For instance,
look up does not mean see upwards, but go
to the library and check on something’, see
Chapter 5.
phrase A group of related words, centered
around a head; see Chapter 3.
pied piping Taking the preposition along in
a relative clause or a question, as in the man [to
whom] I talked; see Chapter 10.
pleonastic subject See dummy subject, see
also Chapter 4 and special topic to Chapter 5.
possessive E.g. his or Catweazles in his book
or Catweazles book; see Chapter 2. It is of the
D category.
postmodier Modier that follows the head,
e.g. from Venice in a stone from Venice.
PP = Prepositional Phrase: group of words
belonging to the preposition, e.g. in the garden.
pre-D = Pre-determiner, quantiers such as
all, both, half can occur before the determiner,
e.g. in all that trouble; see Chapter 2.
predicate Says something about the sub-
ject, realized as a VP, e.g. saw him in Hermione
saw him.
predicative adjective Term not used in
this book; an adjective that heads an AP with
the function of subject predicate or object
predicate.
premodier Modier that precedes the
head, e.g. blue in a blue hat.
Glossary 227
preposing Moving a word or phrase to a
position towards the beginning of a sentence.
preposition A word indicating location (in
place and time), such as at, in, and on, direction,
such as to, into, and towards, relationship,
such as with, between, among, and of; see
Chapter 2.
preposition stranding Leaving the preposi-
tion behind in a relative clause or a question,
as in the man who I talked to; see Chapter 10.
prepositional verb A verb that has a PP
as a complement, e.g. rely on, refer to; see
Chapter 5.
prescriptive rules A rule typically learned in
school, see Chapter 1, e.g. don’t split an inni-
tive or don’t use multiple negation.
present (tense) The event occurs at the time
of speech.
present participle Forms that end in -ing, e.g.
walking, used after a progressive auxiliary, as in
he is walking, or on its own in a non-nite clause,
as in walking along the street, I saw a re.
present perfect A Verb Group like have lived
in I have lived here for ever. See Chapter 6 and
Figure 6.1.
progressive (aspect) Indicating that the
action is or was going on; see Chapter 6.
pronominalization Referring to an NP, PP,
VP, AdjP, or AdvP, i.e. a phrase, by means of a
pronoun.
pro-form Word that pronominalizes a PP,
VP, AdjP, or AdvP. Used as a broader term than
pronoun.
pronoun Words such as he, she, it, me that
refer to an NP; pronouns replacing PPs (there),
AdjPs (so), AdvPs (thus), or VPs (do so) are called
either pronoun or pro-form.
pronoun
resolution Determining the proper
antecedent for a pronoun. See special topic to
Chapter 9.
proper noun Not used in this book. A noun
that used for names, e.g. Bertha, Arizona.
pseudo-cleft A construction such as What
he did was stupid, used to emphasize/focus a
part; see Chapter 11, Section 3.
pun The use of a word to suggest two mean-
ings; see Chapter 1.
quantier Words such as all, some, many,
each; they are either like determiners or adjec-
tives, or occur before determiners.
question See yes/no question and wh-
question.
RC = relative clause, see below and Chapter 10.
reflexive pronoun The pronouns myself,
yourself, himself, herself, ourselves and
themselves.
regular verbs Verbs formed by adding -ed to
the present to form the past tense and the past
participle, as in the case of regular verbs such
as walk, walked, walked. See Chapter 6.
relative clause A clause/sentence that typi-
cally modies a noun, e.g. the tree which I see
from the window; see Chapter 10.
relative pronoun pronoun such as who,
whose that introduces relative clauses; see
Chapter 10. The same set is used in ques-
tions and then they are called interrogative
pronouns.
restrictive RC A relative clause with highly
relevant information; see Table 10.1.
S = sentence: a group of words that includes
at least a verb/VP
= Speech on a timeline; see Chapter 6.
S = S-bar, used in the rst edition of this book,
replaced by CP.
S-adverbial An adverbial that modies the
entire sentence or expresses the feelings of
the speaker, e.g. unfortunately, as opposed to
a VP-adverbial. See Chapter 5.
SC = Small Clause, see below.
sentence/clause A unit that contains at
least a verb. The subject may or may not be
expressed; see Chapters 7, 8, and 10.
semantics The linguistic aspects to meaning.
semi-modal Auxiliary such as dare (to), need
(to), used to, ought to, have to. They have prop-
erties of both main verbs and modal verbs; see
Chapter 6.
simple sentence/
clause Sentence or clause
with only one lexical verb; see Chapter 7.
228 An Introduction to the Grammar of English
sister For example, a P and NP are sisters of
each other; each has a branch going up to the
‘mother’; see Chapter 3.
specify Point to something, i.e. a determiner’s
function in a phrase.
split innitive Separating the to from the
verb, e.g. as in to boldly go ...; see special topic
Chapter 1.
small clause A sentence in which the verb
has been left out; see Chapter 4.
stacking When more than one adjective pre-
cedes the noun; see Chapter 9.
strong verbs A term for a verb that has a dif-
ferent vowel for the present, the past, and the
past participle, e.g. swim, swam, swum.
subject In English, the subject agrees with
the verb in person and number, see Chapter 4.
subject predicate Often realized as an AdjP,
making a claim about the subject, e.g. nice in
She is nice. It occurs after a copula verb such as
be, become; see Chapter 4.
subject verb
agreement Finite verbs agree
with subjects in English. Evidence for it is fairly
limited, e.g. the –s on she walks.
subjunctive Expressing a wish or intention
or necessity, e.g. go is a subjunctive verb in it is
important that he go there. In Modern English,
most of these are replaced by modal verbs; see
Chapter 6, Section 2.1.
subordinate clause Dependent clause, or
clause embedded in another by means of a
complementizer such as that, because, if; see
Chapters 7 and 8.
subordinating
conjunction Not used in this
book, same as complementizer, see there.
superlative An adjective such as greatest,
see Chapter 2.
synonym A word with an almost identical
meaning, e.g. often and frequently; see Chapter 2.
syntax Rules for how words are combined
into phrases and sentences, the topic of this
book; see Chapter 1.
tag-question A repetition of the subject and
the auxiliary, as in She has been there before,
hasn’t she? See Chapter 4.
tense Indicating past or present time.
trace Not used in this book. When used,
it indicates that a word or phrase has been
moved. In Chapters 10 and 11, I have used
copies in brackets instead. These copies have
a line through them to show they are not
pronounced.
transitive Verb with one object, e.g. see; see
Chapter 4.
tree A representation of the units/phrases of
a sentence by means of branches and nodes;
see Chapter 3.
two-part coordinator Coordinator with two
parts, e.g. both Mary and John, neither Mary nor
John; see Chapter 2.
Universal Grammar Grammatical properties
shared by all languages; see Chapter 1.
V = verb
V
= V-bar, an intermediate category; see
Chapter 5.
verb A lexical category often expressing a
state, act, event or emotion; see Chapter 2.
VGP = Verb Group; see Chapter 6.
vowel Sounds such as i, e, a, o, and u, made
by not blocking the airstream; see Chapter 1,
for use in a rule.
VP-adverbial An adverbial that modies
the action of the verb, e.g. quickly, slowly, as
opposed to an S-adverbial; see Chapter 5.
weak verbs A term for a verb that has a -ed
past and past participle, e.g. walk.
wh-question A question that starts with
who, what, how, why, when or where; see
Chapter 11.
word order Linear sequencing of words and
phrases.
yes/no question A question for which the
appropriate answer would be ‘yes, ‘no, or
‘maybe/perhaps’; see Chapter 11.
References
British National Corpus, http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk.
Chomsky, Noam 1975. Reections on Language. Fontana.
Corpus of Contemporary American English, http://www.americancorpus.org.
Fowler, H.W. 1926 [1950]. A Dictionary of Modern English Usage. Oxford: Clarendon.
Kirszner, Laurie & Stephen Mandell 1992. e Holt Handbook [third edition]. HBJ.
O'Dwyer, Bernard 2000. Modern English Structures. Broadview Press.
O'Grady, William & Michael Dobrovolsky 1987. Contemporary Linguistic Analysis. Longmans
[1st edition]
Oxford English Dictionary (OED). online edition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Quirk, Randolph & Sidney Greenbaum 1973. A University Grammar of English. London: Longman.
Strunk, William with E.B. White 1959[2000]. e Elements of Style. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.
Swan, Michael 1980. Practical English Usage. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
A
accusative case 8384, 115116,
151, 204
active 69, 94, 102103,
111112, 208
adjective 8, 10, 12, 1418, 2021,
2627, 3233, 3536, 80, 83,
111112, 170176, 179, 187,
194195, 197
AdjP 35, 3840, 47, 49, 53,
65, 7072, 7677, 96, 134,
169176, 179181, 187,
193194, 198, 214
adverb 8, 1012, 1418, 2224,
26, 28, 3234, 3940, 59, 86,
99, 106, 114, 176
adverbial 69, 7273, 77,
8690, 9396, 124, 132,
134, 137138, 140, 150152,
161, 189, 194195, 198,
211, 215
AdvP 35, 3940, 49, 8690, 96,
169172, 193194
ax 1314, 105, 107, 109, 111114,
119, 123
ax-hop 105, 109, 111113
agreement 8, 10, 14, 37, 6667,
8385, 107, 115, 183, 215
ambiguity/ambiguous 25,
89, 15, 35, 4142, 49,
72, 88, 9899, 111, 142,
157, 163, 175, 182, 188,
210, 211213
antecedent 188, 195, 204
antonym 1213, 16
article 12, 13, 1921, 176
aspect 106, 109110,
117118, 121
auxiliary 4, 12, 14, 19, 21, 26,
2930, 35, 37, 66, 69, 103,
105119, 122123, 127128,
132134, 158, 169, 201, 203,
205207, 215
B
bare innitive 150, 152, 155
branch 3539, 4249, 68, 75, 87
C
case 810, 13, 8385, 115, 118119,
151, 162, 204, 215
clause 2122, 39, 44, 56,
65, 68, 70, 76, 89, 96, 118,
132140, 149155, 189, 205,
214216
cle 205, 208210
coathanger 155, 209
comma splice 211
comparative 1718, 194
complement 40, 65, 68, 7374,
7677, 80, 87, 96, 124, 167,
170180, 182188
complement clause 187198,
200, 211, 214
complementizer 8, 12, 19, 2123,
26, 35, 59, 132137, 140,
146148, 150152, 153, 155,
164, 192, 194, 215
complex transitive 7374, 7677,
81, 94, 9697, 118119, 124,
129130
compound 16, 27, 30, 91, 170
consonant 1
contraction 8, 63, 122
coordination 35, 4345, 49, 84,
92, 132134, 138140, 154,
164, 179180, 213, 215
coordinator 12, 19, 2122,
26, 35, 132134, 138140,
154, 215
copula 7277, 8083, 85, 96, 103,
106, 112, 118119, 124125,
129131, 201, 208
corpus 9, 17, 70, 80, 171
CP 35, 132, 135140, 149,
152155, 164, 195198,
203207, 209, 214
D
(D)Adv 40
dangling participle/modier 8,
154, 161163, 167, 215
daughter 43, 46, 65, 74, 79, 87
declarative 205, 208
denite article 19
degree adverb 1517, 2224,
3233, 40, 170, 172
descriptivism 8
determiner 1213, 1921,
2326, 35, 39, 46, 169,
173176, 179180,
183184, 214
direct object 65, 6877, 8384,
86, 89, 9496, 106, 124127,
130, 134135, 140, 151,
156157, 171, 173, 190,
196, 198
ditransitive 70, 7274, 77,
96, 102, 118119, 124125,
129130
dummy do
105, 108, 112113,
206, 215
dummy subject 67, 85, 103104,
137, 215
E
echo question 206
ed
-participle see past participle
elided/elision see ellipsis
ellipsis 106, 139140, 156,
159, 164
embedded 132137, 140, 142,
145146, 149155, 157, 164,
167, 205, 215
emphasizer 176
exclamation/exclamative 205,
207209
extraposed/extraposition 137,
140, 154, 165
F
nite clause 132, 134, 140,
149150, 152, 164, 190193,
197, 198
nite verb 85, 103, 105, 108,
114116, 118119, 124, 134,
149150, 164
at structure 36, 3839, 49, 76,
105106, 114
focuser 176
formal language 69, 33, 195
fragment 114116, 135, 149
Index
Index 231
function 1, 5, 41, 43, 6577,
8396, 124126,
132134, 151152, 155,
169179, 189194, 196, 198
future 108111, 116, 150
future perfect 110, 116
G
genitive case 13, 83, 85, 151
gerund 151, 155, 161163
grammar 16, 8
grammatical category 12, 16,
1923, 2526, 35, 60, 106, 215
H
head 12, 16, 3540, 43, 4547, 49,
67, 8587, 106, 146, 169180,
183184, 192, 195196, 198,
200, 207, 214
hypercorrection 33, 83
I
ing
-participle see present
participle
imperative 116
indenite article 19
indenite pronoun 24
indicative 205
indirect object 65, 6870,
7277, 87, 89, 94, 96,
124125, 134, 195
innitive 8, 1011, 114, 116,
148155, 167, 215
informal language 69, 33, 57,
109, 115, 122, 147
innate faculty 56, 8
interrogative 205
interrogative pronoun 1921, 24
intransitive 7275, 77, 86, 9495,
106, 118119, 124, 215
intransitive phrasal 9293, 126
irregular verb 7374, 109,
116119, 121122, 124, 150
L
lexical category 1219, 23,
2527, 29, 39, 42, 49, 59
lexical verb 21, 105106, 108,
112119, 124, 132134, 140,
149, 164, 203
light verb 65, 76
linguistic knowledge 1, 6, 8, 60
M
main clause 133134, 137138,
140, 150151, 161162,
167, 205
main verb see lexical verb
modal 105, 107109, 112119,
150, 153
modier 89, 95, 134, 146,
169180, 189, 193195, 198,
211, 214215
modify 1418, 20, 3234,
37, 3941, 4849, 71, 73,
8689, 95
monotransitive 7273, 77,
96, 124
morphology 12, 14, 18
mother 43
multiple negation 6, 8, 5658
N
negation 6, 8, 5658, 62
negative 67, 16, 5658, 105106,
112114, 118, 122, 215
node 36, 3839, 44, 49, 76,
171, 182
nominative case 10, 8385,
115116, 118119, 124,
151, 204
non-nite verb 105, 116, 118119,
120, 124, 150151, 154155
non-nite clause 116, 134, 140,
142, 149155, 161, 164,
189190, 192194,
197198, 215
non-linguistic 1, 6, 8, 60
non-restrictive RC 190191,
195196, 198, 211, 214
noun 6, 1226, 32, 3539, 45,
4950, 59, 71, 76, 83, 95, 111,
134, 151, 153, 161, 169170,
173179, 190193, 195198,
200, 215
noun complement 132, 192, 200
NP 21, 3549, 65, 6768, 7071,
7677, 8485, 8687, 8996,
134, 162, 169180, 189198,
207, 209, 214, 216
numeral 1921, 24
O
object predicate 6577, 87, 89,
9496, 124125, 134, 211
P
participle 18, 73, 95, 109,
111112, 114, 116119,
122123, 150, 152, 154155,
161163, 193, 215
particle 86, 9193, 99, 131
past tense 14, 21, 40, 7374,
108109, 116117, 121122,
124, 150
past perfect 109110, 121
passive auxiliary 103, 111113,
114, 118, 123
passive construction 89, 6869,
76, 9396, 102104, 105, 107,
111112, 114, 205, 214215
past participle 109, 111112, 114,
117118, 123, 149150, 152,
155, 193, 201
perfect auxiliary 106, 109, 123
phonology 12, 7
phrasal verb 7273, 75, 86,
9096, 99100, 118119,
124126, 134, 167
phrase 1, 5, 12, 16, 23, 3549, 59,
6566, 7172, 74, 77, 8687,
89, 92, 106, 132, 135140,
169180, 188, 189, 198,
206209, 214
pied piping 195
pleonastic subject 67, 103
possessive 1314, 1921, 24, 51,
83, 151, 176, 194, 195
PP 35, 3843, 4549, 69, 7071,
77, 86, 8890, 9496, 155,
169179, 189, 193195,
214215
pre-D 2021, 176
predicate 6568, 74, 77
preposition 8, 10, 1214, 1819,
2123, 26, 29, 4142, 45, 59,
69, 7273, 84, 86, 9091, 93,
139, 146147, 170, 194195,
203204, 215
preposition stranding
203204, 215
prepositional verb 86, 9091, 93,
96, 118, 124
prescriptive 1, 610, 33, 5659,
8385, 102, 123, 146147, 151,
161, 188, 203, 215
present 1415, 40, 7374, 76, 85,
108110, 115, 117118, 121, 150
232 Index
present participle 111, 114,
118, 123, 149152, 154155,
161163, 193
present perfect 73, 109110,
117118, 121, 124
pro-form 24, 40
progressive 105, 107, 110114,
118119, 121
pronominalization 37, 45, 49,
179180
pronoun 8, 1012, 19, 2324,
26, 29, 31, 37, 3940,
49, 59, 65, 67, 84, 87,
115, 139, 151, 153, 177,
188, 190192, 194195,
203204, 208
pronoun resolution 188
pseudo-cle 205, 208209
pun 3, 8, 18
Q
quantier 1921, 24, 176
question 45, 6667, 69,
105106, 108, 112113, 118,
135, 204209, 214
R
reexive pronoun 2324
regular verb 117, 119
relative clause (RC) 39, 44,
134, 165, 189198,
203204, 209
relative pronoun 188, 190, 192,
194195, 203204
restrictive RC 190191,
193196, 198
S
S 4243, 135136
S-adverbial 88, 90, 96, 137
SC 76, 125
semantics 14, 18
semi-modal 108109
sister 38, 4143, 48, 54, 65,
68, 7476, 87, 90, 135,
164, 171, 174, 176179, 184,
195198, 214
specify 1920, 24, 36, 39, 41
split innitive 8, 1011, 63, 215
small clause 76
stacking 171
strong verb 117
subject 3, 5, 8, 10, 1314, 23, 37,
43, 6577, 8385, 89, 94,
96, 103104, 111, 114116,
118, 124, 132, 134, 136137,
139140, 151155, 161163,
164, 169, 179, 189190,
194195, 198, 204, 207208,
211212
subject predicate 65, 69, 7077,
85, 87, 89, 94, 96, 124125,
134, 136, 150151, 155, 164,
169, 198
subject verb agreement 8, 10,
37, 67, 85, 115
subjunctive 107108
subordinate clause 2122, 134,
140, 150
superlative 1718
synonym 12, 16, 25, 32,
93, 139
syntax 12, 78, 12, 14, 18
T
tag question 6667, 106
tense 14, 21, 40, 7374,
85, 106111, 115119,
121, 150
transitive 70, 7277, 9293, 96,
106, 116, 118119, 124
transitive phrasal 9293
tree 3549, 65, 68, 7477,
8788, 105107, 111, 124,
134, 136137, 139140, 149,
153, 155, 170176, 178179,
195198, 205, 209, 212
two-part coordinator 22
U
Universal Grammar 56, 8
V
verb 45, 1315, 40, 59, 71, 7274,
7677, 8485, 9093, 103,
105119, 122123, 132134,
149155, 215
VGP 105119, 155
vowel 12, 117
VP-adverbial 88, 90
W
weak verb 117
wh
-question 4, 84, 205206,
209, 210
word order 66, 177, 180
Y
Yes/No
question 4, 66,
205206, 209