PERSONALITY PROCESSES AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES
The Do Re Mi’s of Everyday Life: The Structure and Personality
Correlates of Music Preferences
Peter J. Rentfrow and Samuel D. Gosling
University of Texas at Austin
The present research examined individual differences in music preferences. A series of 6 studies
investigated lay beliefs about music, the structure underlying music preferences, and the links between
music preferences and personality. The data indicated that people consider music an important aspect of
their lives and listening to music an activity they engaged in frequently. Using multiple samples,
methods, and geographic regions, analyses of the music preferences of over 3,500 individuals converged
to reveal 4 music-preference dimensions: Reflective and Complex, Intense and Rebellious, Upbeat and
Conventional, and Energetic and Rhythmic. Preferences for these music dimensions were related to a
wide array of personality dimensions (e.g., Openness), self-views (e.g., political orientation), and
cognitive abilities (e.g., verbal IQ).
At this very moment, in homes, offices, cars, restaurants, and
clubs around the world, people are listening to music. Despite its
prevalence in everyday life, however, the sound of music has
remained mute within social and personality psychology. Indeed,
of the nearly 11,000 articles published between 1965 and 2002 in
the leading social and personality journals, music was listed as an
index term (or subject heading) in only seven articles. The eminent
personality psychologist Raymond Cattell even remarked on the
bewildering absence of research on music, “So powerful is the
effect of music . . . that one is surprised to find in the history of
psychology and psychotherapy so little experimental, or even
speculative, reference to the use of music” (Cattell & Saunders,
1954, p. 3).
Although a growing body of research has identified links be-
tween music and social behavior (Hargreaves & North, 1997;
North, Hargreaves, & McKendrick, 1997, 2000), the bulk of stud-
ies have been performed by a relatively small cadre of music
educators and music psychologists. We believe that an activity that
consumes so much time and resources and that is a key component
of so many social situations warrants the attention of mainstream
social and personality psychologists. In the present article we
begin to redress the historical neglect of music by exploring the
landscape of music preferences. The fundamental question guiding
our research program is, Why do people listen to music? Although
the answer to this question is undoubtedly complex and beyond the
scope of a single article, we attempt to shed some light on the issue
by examining music preferences. In this research we take the first
crucial steps to developing a theory of music preferences—a
theory that will ultimately explain when, where, how, and why
people listen to music.
Why Study Music Preferences?
Recently, a number of criticisms have been raised about the lack
of attention to real-world behavior within social and personality
psychology (e.g., Funder, 2001; Hogan, 1998; Mehl & Penne-
baker, 2003; Rozin, 2001). For example, Funder (2001) noted that
although there is a wealth of information regarding the structure of
personality, “the catalog of basic facts concerning the relationships
between personality and behavior remains thin” (p. 212). Accord-
ing to Funder, one way researchers can address this issue is to
extend their research on the structural components of personality
to include behavior that occurs in everyday life. Still others have
criticized the field for focusing on a narrow subset of social
phenomena and ignoring many basic, pervasive social activities.
Rozin (2001) opined, “Psychologists should learn . . . to keep their
eyes on the big social phenomena, and to situate what they study
in the flow of social life” (p. 12). In short, there is a growing
concern that the breadth of topics studied by many research psy-
chologists is too narrow and excludes many important facets of
everyday life that are worthy of scientific attention. Music is one
such facet.
Music is a ubiquitous social phenomenon. It is at the center of
many social activities, such as concerts, where people congregate
Peter J. Rentfrow and Samuel D. Gosling, Department of Psychology,
University of Texas at Austin.
Preparation of this article was supported by National Institute of Mental
Health Grant MH64527-01A1. We are grateful to Matthias Mehl, Sanjay
Srivastava, and Simine Vazire for their helpful comments on this research;
Patrick Randall and Sanjay Srivastava for their statistical advice; Sarah
Glenney for her assistance with Study 1; and Paradise Kaikhany, Mathew
Knapek, Jennifer Malaspina, Yvette Martinez, Scott Meyerott, Ying Jun
Puk, Stacie Scruggs, and Jennifer Weathers for their assistance with the
data collection in Studies 4 and 5.
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Peter J.
Rentfrow, Department of Psychology, University of Texas at Austin,
Austin, Texas 78712. E-mail: [email protected]
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2003, Vol. 84, No. 6, 1236–1256
Copyright 2003 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 0022-3514/03/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.6.1236
1236
to listen to music and talk about it. Even in social gatherings where
music is not the primary focus, it is an essential component
imagine, for instance, a party or wedding without music.
Music can also satisfy a number of needs beyond the social
context. Just as individuals shape their social and physical envi-
ronments to reinforce their dispositions and self-views (Buss,
1987; Gosling, Ko, Mannarelli, & Morris, 2002; Snyder & Ickes,
1985; Swann, 1987; Swann, Rentfrow, & Guinn, 2002), the music
they select can serve a similar function. For instance, an individual
high in Openness to New Experiences may prefer styles of music
that reinforce his or her view of being artistic and sophisticated.
Furthermore, individuals may seek out particular styles of music to
regulate their emotional states; for example, depressed individuals
may choose styles of music that sustain their melancholic mood.
Although the myriad psychological and social processes influenc-
ing peoples music preferences are undoubtedly complex, it is
reasonable to suppose that examining the ties between basic per-
sonality traits and music preferences could shed some light on why
people listen to music.
The present research is designed to extend theory and research
into peoples everyday lives by examining individual differences
in music preferences. By exploring the structure of music prefer-
ences and its links to personality, self-views, and cognitive ability,
we begin to lay the foundations on which a broad theory of music
preferences can be built.
What Do We Already Know About Music Preferences?
Although music has enjoyed considerable attention in cognitive
psychology (e.g., Bharucha & Mencl, 1996; Chaffin & Imreh,
2002; Deutsch, 1999; Drayna, Manichaikul, de Lange, Sneider, &
Spector, 2001; Krumhansl, 1990, 2000, 2002; Radocy & Boyle,
1979; Sloboda, 1985), biological psychology (e.g., Oyama et al.,
1983; Rider, Floyd, & Kirkpatrick, 1985; Standley, 1992; Todd
1999), clinical psychology (Chey & Holzman, 1997; Diamond,
2002; Dorow, 1975; Hilliard, 2001; Wigram, Saperston, & West,
1995), and neuroscience (e.g., Besson, Faita, Peretz, Bonnel, &
Requin, 1998; Blood & Zatorre, 2001; Blood, Zatorre, Bermudez,
& Evans, 1999; Clynes, 1982; Marin & Perry, 1999; Peretz,
Gagnon, & Bouchard, 1998; Peretz & Hebert, 2000; Rauschecker,
2001), very little is known about why people like the music they
do.
Clearly, individuals display stronger preferences for some types
of music than for others. But what determines a persons music
preferences? Are there certain individual differences linking peo-
ple to certain styles of music? The few studies that have examined
music preferences suggest some links to personality (Arnett, 1992;
Cattell & Anderson, 1953b; Cattell & Saunders, 1954; Little &
Zuckerman, 1986; McCown, Keiser, Mulhearn, & Williamson,
1997), physiological arousal (Gowensmith & Bloom, 1997; Mc-
Namara & Ballard, 1999; Oyama et al., 1983; Rider et al., 1985),
and social identity (Crozier, 1998; North & Hargreaves, 1999;
North, Hargreaves, & ONeill, 2000; Tarrant, North, & Har-
greaves, 2000).
Personality
Cattell was among the first to theorize about how music could
contribute to understanding personality. He believed that prefer-
ences for certain types of music reveal important information
about unconscious aspects of personality that is overlooked by
most personality inventories (Cattell & Anderson, 1953a, 1953b;
Cattell & Saunders, 1954; Kemp, 1996). Accordingly, Cattell and
Anderson (1953a) created the I.P.A.T. Music Preference Test, a
personality inventory comprising 120 classical and jazz music
excerpts in which respondents indicate how much they like each
musical item. Using factor analysis, Cattell and Saunders (1954)
identified 12 music-preference factors and interpreted each one as
an unconscious reflection of specific personality characteristics
(e.g., surgency, warmth, conservatism). Whereas Cattell believed
that music preferences provide a window into the unconscious,
most researchers have regarded music preferences as a manifesta-
tion of more explicit personality traits. For example, sensation
seeking appears to be positively related to preferences for rock,
heavy metal, and punk music and negatively related to preferences
for sound tracks and religious music (Little & Zuckerman, 1986).
In addition, Extraversion and Psychoticism have been shown to
predict preferences for music with exaggerated bass, such as rap
and dance music (McCown et al., 1997).
Physiological Arousal
Another line of research revealing links between music prefer-
ences and personality has focused on the physiological correlates
of music preferences. For example, heavy metal fans tend to
experience higher resting arousal than country music fans. Fur-
thermore, listening to heavy metal music has been shown to
increase the arousal level of heavy metal fans beyond that of
country music fans (Gowensmith & Bloom, 1997). Similarly,
preference for highly arousing music (e.g., heavy metal, rock,
alternative, rap, and dance) appears to be positively related to
resting arousal, sensation seeking, and antisocial personality (Mc-
Namara & Ballard, 1999).
Social Identity
Additional evidence linking music preferences and personality
comes from research on social identity. For example, North and
Hargreaves (1999) found that people use music as a badge to
communicate their values, attitudes, and self-views. More specif-
ically, they examined the characteristics of the prototypical rap and
pop music fan. Participants music preferences were related, in
part, to the degree to which their self-views correlated with the
characteristics of the prototypical music fan. This relationship,
however, was moderated by participants self-esteem, such that
individuals with higher self-esteem perceived more similarity be-
tween themselves and the prototype than did individuals with low
self-esteem. Similar findings in different populations, age groups,
and cultures provide additional support for the notion that peoples
self-views and self-esteem influence music preferences (North,
Hargreaves, & ONeill, 2000; Tarrant et al., 2000).
Although the results from these studies provide intriguing
glimpses into relationships between music preferences and person-
ality, taken together they offer an incomplete picture. For instance,
most of the studies examined only a limited selection of music
genres: Cattell and Saunders (1954) examined preferences for
classical and jazz music, Gowensmith and Bloom (1997) examined
preferences for heavy metal and country music, and North and
1237
MUSIC PREFERENCES
Hargreaves (1999) examined preferences for pop and rap music.
Moreover, most of the studies examined only a few personality
dimensions: Little and Zuckerman (1986) examined sensation
seeking, McCown et al. (1997) examined Extraversion and Psy-
choticism, and McNamara and Ballard (1999) examined antisocial
personality. A theory of music preferences needs to be based on a
more comprehensive exploration of the music and personality
domains. Thus, we build on the provocative findings provided by
this important early work with a series of studies using a broad and
systematic selection of music genres and personality dimensions.
Overview of Studies
Given the paucity of research on music preferences, we sought
to explore the structure of music preferences and to examine its
relationship to personality. The questions guiding this research
were as follows: How much importance do people give to music?
What are the basic dimensions of music preferences? How can
they be characterized? How do they relate to existing dimensions
of personality?
In Study 1 we examined lay beliefs about the relevance and
importance of music in peoples everyday lives. Adopting a factor-
analytic approach, in Studies 24 we examined the basic structure
of music preferences. In Study 5 we examined the psychological
attributes of different styles of music. In Study 6, we examined the
relationship between music preferences and personality, self-
views, and cognitive ability.
Study 1: Lay Beliefs About the Importance of Music
It seemed self-evident to us that music is an important part of
individuals lives, but before embarking on this program of re-
search, we wanted to determine whether our beliefs were empiri-
cally grounded. Thus, the purpose of this study was simply to
develop a general understanding of lay beliefs about the role of
music in everyday life. How important is music to people? Is
music more or less important than other leisure activities? Do
individuals believe that their music preferences reveal information
about their personality? What are the contexts in which individuals
typically listen to music? To examine these issues, we adminis-
tered a questionnaire that would provide some preliminary
answers.
Method
Participants. The sample was made up of 74 University of Texas at
Austin undergraduates who volunteered in exchange for partial fulfillment
of an introductory psychology course requirement during the spring se-
mester of 2001. The sample included 30 (40.5%) women and 44 (59.5%)
men, 2 (2.7%) African Americans, 7 (9.5%) Asians, 5 (6.8%) Hispanics, 49
(66.2%) Whites, and 11 (14.8%) individuals of other ethnicities. The
average age of participants was 18.9 years (SD 2.3).
Procedure. On arrival, participants were introduced to a study of
lifestyle and leisure preferences. They were then asked to complete a
packet of questionnaires that were designed to assess their attitudes and
beliefs about various lifestyle and leisure activities. Our first question dealt
with the importance individuals give to various lifestyle and leisure activ-
ities. Participants were presented with a list of eight different activities and
were asked to indicate how personally important each domain was to
them using a scale ranging from 0 (Strongly disagree) to 100 (Strongly
agree; e.g., Music is very important to me). The next question was about
participants beliefs about how much their lifestyle and leisure activities
say about their self-views, using a scale ranging from 0 (Strongly disagree)
to 100 (Strongly agree; e.g., My movie preferences say a lot about who I
am); their personalities, using a scale ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree)
to7(Strongly agree; e.g., My television preferences reveal a great deal
about my personality); and other peoples personalities, using a scale
ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree)to7(Strongly agree; e.g., Peoples
television preferences reveal a great deal about their personality). Finally,
using a scale ranging from 1 (Never)to7(All the time), participants were
asked to indicate the frequency with which they engaged in various
activities while in nine different contexts (alone at home, going to sleep,
hanging out with friends, driving, getting up in the morning, studying,
working, exercising, and getting ready to go out; e.g., How often do you
read books or magazines while at home?).
1
Results and Discussion
How much importance do individuals place on music? As
shown in Figure 1, along with hobbies (M 82.0, SD 19.3),
music (M 78.1, SD 23.6) was considered the most important
of the domains we examined; the difference between music and
hobbies was not significant, t(69) 1.12, ns. Furthermore, music
was considered significantly more important than the next item,
food preferences, t(69) 3.56, p .001. Overall, participants
music preferences were at least as important as or more important
than the other seven domains, supporting our belief that music is
an important part of peoples lives.
How much do people believe music preferences say about
themselves? As shown in Figure 2, along with hobbies (M
76.5, SD 23.4) and bedrooms (M 63.4, SD 31.8), music
preferences (M 69.4, SD 25.7) were believed to reveal a
considerable amount of information about participants personal
qualities; the differences between music and hobbies and music
and bedrooms were not significant (ts 1.91, ps .06). Overall,
participants believed that their music preferences revealed as much
if not more information about themselves than the other domains.
How much do people believe music preferences reveal about
their own and others’ personalities? As shown in Figure 3,
participants considered hobbies to reveal as much about their own
personalities as music (Ms 5.51, 5.26; SDs 1.54, 1.78),
t(71) .91, ns, yet music was believed to reveal significantly more
than the next highest activity, movie preferences (M 4.54,
SD 1.78), t(71) 2.66, p .01.
Furthermore, music preferences (M 5.89, SD 1.61) were
second only to hobbies (M 5.89, SD 1.15) in terms of what
participants believed they revealed about others personalities,
t(71) 2.58, p .025. In addition, music was believed to provide
significantly more information about others than book and maga-
zine preferences (M 4.74, SD 1.75), t(71) 2.54, p .025.
Thus, participants believed that music preferences reveal at least as
1
Participants were not asked how often they engaged in all of the
activities in all the situations because it did not always make sense to do so.
For example, it did not seem appropriate to ask participants how often they
read books while driving because reading is probably an uncommon
activity in this situation.
1238
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
much about their personalities and the personalities of others as the
other lifestyle and leisure domains (with the exception of hobbies).
In which contexts do individuals listen to music? The results
shown in Figure 4 indicate that participants reported listening to
music frequently in every situation listed (M 5.19, SD .93). In
general, music is listened to most often while driving, alone at
home, exercising, and hanging out with friends. In addition, the
results indicated that participants listened to music more often than
any of the other activities (i.e., watching television, reading books,
and watching movies) across all the situations (ts 3.3, ps .001)
except while going to sleep, in which case watching television was
as common as listening to music, t(72) 1.5, ns. These findings
provide further support for the pervasiveness of music in peoples
everyday lives.
Summary
We sought information concerning lay beliefs about music. The
results strongly support the notion that music is important to
people and that individuals believe that the music people listen to
provides information about who they are. Moreover, the fact that
our participants reported listening to music more often than any
other activity across a wide variety of contexts confirms that music
plays an integral role in peoples everyday lives. In general, these
Figure 2. Lay beliefs about the amount of information various preferences and activities reveal about personal
qualities.
Figure 1. Lay beliefs about the importance of various preferences and activities.
1239
MUSIC PREFERENCES
findings reinforce the importance of music as an everyday social
phenomenon and offer further justification for including music on
the research agenda for mainstream social and personality psy-
chology.
Mapping the Terrain of Music Preferences:
A Factor-Analytic Approach
The results of Study 1 indicate that people consider music to be
as important as other lifestyle and leisure activities. Having con-
firmed the importance of music in everyday life, the next step was
to identify the structure of music preferences. Three independent
studies were designed to identify the dimensions of music prefer-
ences and examine their generalizability across samples and meth-
ods. Study 2 was an exploratory analysis of music preferences.
Studies 3 and 4 served as confirmatory studies to test the gener-
alizability of the music-preference dimensions across time, sam-
ples, and methods.
Measuring Music Preferences
What is the most sensible unit of analysis for studying music
preferences? There are a variety of ways in which music prefer-
ences can be assessed. For example, individuals could report their
Figure 3. Lay beliefs about the amount of information various preferences and activities reveal about the
personality of oneself and others.
Figure 4. Self-reported frequency of listening to music in different situations.
1240
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
degree of liking for specific songs (e.g., Born Blind), bands or
artists (e.g., Sonny Boy Williamson), subgenres (e.g., harmonica
blues), genres (e.g., blues), or general music attributes (e.g., re-
laxed). Thus, music preferences could be measured at different
levels of abstraction, ranging from a highly descriptive subordinate
level to a very broad superordinate level (John, Hampson, &
Goldberg, 1991; Murphy, 1982).
What is the optimal level of abstraction with which to categorize
music? The focus of this research is on ordinary, everyday music
preferences, so our goal was to assess music preferences at the
level that naturally arises when people think about and express
their music preferences. When people discuss their music prefer-
ences they tend to do so first at the level of genres and to a lesser
extent subgenres and only later step up to broader terms (e.g., loud)
or down to specific artists (e.g., Van Halen) or songs (e.g., Run-
ning with the Devil; Jellison & Flowers, 1991). Thus, the genre
and subgenre categories were the optimal levels at which to start
our investigations of music preferences.
We used a multistep process to determine which genres and
subgenres to include in our measure of preferences. First, we
created a preliminary pool of music-preferences categories com-
prising music genres and subgenres. Specifically, we used a free-
association type task in which a panel of five judges was asked to
list all the music genres and subgenres that came to mind. Second,
to ensure that a variety of different styles of music were included,
we consulted with online music stores (e.g., towerrecords.com,
barnesandnoble.com) to identify additional genres and subgenres
to supplement the initial pool. This procedure generated a total
of 80 music genres and subgenres that varied in specificity. Next,
we presented these 14 genres and 66 subgenres to a group of 30
participants and asked them to indicate their preference for the
music categories usinga1(Not at all)to7(A great deal) rating
scale. Participants were instructed to skip any category with which
they were not familiar. Our analyses of items left blank showed
that very few participants (7%) were familiar with all of the
specific subgenres (e.g., Baroque, industrial, Western swing), but
nearly all of them (97%) were familiar with the broader music
genres (e.g., classical, heavy metal, country). These findings sug-
gested that the genre level was the appropriate level at which to
begin examining music preferences.
Thus, the final version, called the Short Test Of Music Prefer-
ences (STOMP), is made up of 14 music genres: alternative, blues,
classical, country, electronica/dance, folk, heavy metal, rap/hip-
hop, jazz, pop, religious, rock, soul/funk, and sound tracks. Pref-
erence for each genre is rated on a 7-point Likert-type scale with
endpoints at 1 (Not at all)and7(A great deal).
Study 2: An Exploratory Factor Analysis
of Music Preferences
The primary objective of Study 2 was to identify the basic
dimensions of music preferences. The study was exploratory and,
given the patchy literature on this topic, we had no a priori theories
or expectations about the number of dimensions or the nature of
the underlying structure. Instead, the analyses served as a spring-
board for generating theories and hypotheses regarding the nature
of music preferences. We used exploratory factor analysis to
examine the factor structure of music preferences; then, in a
subsample of participants, we examined whether the music dimen-
sions would generalize across time.
Method
Participants. The sample was made up of 1,704 University of Texas at
Austin undergraduates who volunteered in exchange for partial fulfillment
of an introductory psychology course requirement during the fall semester
of 2001. Of those who indicated, 1,058 (62.6%) were women and 633
(37.4%) were men, 62 (4.1%) were African American, 205 (13.5%) were
Asian, 205 (13.5%) were Hispanic, 988 (65%) were White, and 60 (3.9%)
were of other ethnicities.
Three weeks after the first sample was tested, a subsample of 118 of the
participants was tested again in exchange for partial fulfillment of an
introductory psychology course requirement. Of those who indicated, 94
(82%) were women and 21 (18%) were men, 6 (5.3%) were African
American, 25 (21.9%) were Asian, 11 (9.7%) were Hispanic, 64 (56.1%)
were White, and 8 (7%) were of other ethnicities.
Procedure. Participants completed the STOMP and a battery of per-
sonality measures during a massive testing session (Time 1). Participants
completed the STOMP again 3 weeks later (Time 2).
Results and Discussion
Factor structure. To identify the major dimensions of music
preferences, we performed principal-components analyses on par-
ticipants ratings. Determining the number of factors to retain is
critical in such analyses, because underextraction or overextraction
may distort subsequent findings (Zwick & Velicer, 1986). We
therefore used multiple converging criteria to decide on the ap-
propriate number of factors to retain: scree test (Cattell, 1966), the
Kaiser rule (i.e., eigenvalues of 1 or greater), parallel analyses of
Monte Carlo simulations (Horn, 1965), and the interpretability of
the solutions (see Zwick & Velicer, 1986). Following these crite-
ria, a four-factor solution was retained, which accounted for 59%
of the total variance.
In accord with Pedhauzer and Schmelkin (1991), both orthog-
onal (varimax) and oblique (oblimin) rotations were initially per-
formed. However, the two solutions were virtually identical, and
the mean correlation among the oblique factors was low (r .01),
suggesting that the orthogonal solution offered a good fit for these
data.
As can be seen in the varimax-rotated factor loadings shown in
Table 1, the factor structure was very clear and interpretable, with
few cross-loading genres. Pop music was the only genre with
factor loadings greater than .40 on multiple factors. To determine
the best labels for the dimensions, seven psychologists (including
the two authors) examined the factor structure and consensually
generated labels to capture the main themes underlying the factors.
As in most factor-analytic research, broad labels inevitably capture
some factors better than others and should thus be used only as
guides to the content of each dimension.
The genres loading most strongly on Factor 1 were blues, jazz,
classical, and folk musicgenres that seem to facilitate introspec-
tion and are structurally complexand this factor was named
Reflective and Complex. Factor 2 was defined by rock, alternative,
and heavy metal musicgenres that are full of energy and em-
phasize themes of rebellionand was named Intense and Rebel-
lious. Factor 3 was defined by country, sound track, religious, and
pop musicgenres that emphasize positive emotions and are
structurally simpleand was named Upbeat and Conventional.
1241
MUSIC PREFERENCES
Factor 4 was defined by rap/hip-hop, soul/funk, and electronica/
dance musicgenres that are lively and often emphasize the
rhythmand was named Energetic and Rhythmic.
Generalizability across time. The factor structure is clear, but
is it temporally stable? It is possible that the music individuals
enjoy listening to changes on a day-to-day basis, perhaps depend-
ing on the mood an individual is in. If so, the temporal stability of
music preferences should be quite low. Alternatively, music pref-
erences may be relatively stable, such that preferences for certain
genres do not vary on a day-to-day basis.
To address this issue, we determined the testretest reliability of
the factors using the subsample of participants who completed the
STOMP again (at Time 2, approximately 3 weeks after the initial
testing session). For Times 1 and 2, we created unit-weighted
scales to measure each of the four varimax factors. Next, we
computed the correlation between Times 1 and 2 for each of the
four music dimensions. The results showed that preference for
each of the dimensions remained stable across time, with retest
rs .77, .80, .89, and .82 for the Reflective and Complex, Intense
and Rebellious, Upbeat and Conventional, and Energetic and
Rhythmic dimensions respectively.
The results from this exploratory investigation suggest that there
is a clear underlying structure to music preferences. Four inter-
pretable factors were identified that capture a broad range of music
preferences. The results from the subsample of participants
tested 3 weeks after the first sample indicate that the music-
preference dimensions are reasonably stable. However, the analy-
ses were exploratory in nature, and a more stringent confirmatory
analysis was needed to test the generalizability of the structure
across samples. This was addressed in Study 3.
Study 3: Generalizability Across Samples
The purpose of this study was to test the cross-sample general-
izability of the dimensional structure of the music preferences
identified in Study 2. To address this issue, we used the same
procedure as in Study 2 and administered the STOMP to another
sample of college students.
Method
Participants. This sample was made up of 1,383 University of Texas
at Austin undergraduates who volunteered in exchange for partial fulfill-
ment of an introductory psychology course requirement during the spring
of 2002. Of those who indicated, 726 (59.7%) were women and 490
(40.3%) were men, 30 (2.5%) were African American, 225 (18.5%) were
Asian, 160 (13.2%) were Hispanic, 760 (62.6%) were White, and 39
(3.2%) were of other ethnicities. There was no overlap of participants
between Studies 2 and 3.
Procedure. The procedure used in Study 3 was identical to the one
used in Study 2. To assess music preferences, participants completed the
same version of the STOMP as participants in Study 2.
Results and Discussion
Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA). To examine the general-
izability of the four music-preference dimensions, we performed a
CFA on the music-preference data using LISREL (Jo¨reskog &
So¨rbom, 1989). CFA is a special type of factor analysis in which
hypotheses regarding the number of factors, their interrelations,
and the variables that load onto each factor can be specified and
tested. On the basis of the four orthogonal factors identified in
Study 2, we tested two models to permit a strong test of the
music-preference dimensions: one model in which the factors were
independent and one model in which the factors were allowed to
correlate. In both models, we specified four latent factors repre-
senting the four music dimensions: All the genres that loaded onto
each of the respective factors identified in Study 2 were freely
estimated. In Model 1, the intercorrelations of the latent factors
were set to zero, whereas in Model 2, this constraint was freed.
Evaluation of the fit of each model was based on multiple
Table 1
Factor Loadings of the 14 Music Genres on Four Varimax-Rotated Principal Components in
Study 2
Genre
Music-preference dimension
Reflective
and Complex
Intense
and Rebellious
Upbeat
and Conventional
Energetic
and Rhythmic
Blues .85 .01 .09 .12
Jazz .83 .04 .07 .15
Classical .66 .14 .02 .13
Folk .64 .09 .15 .16
Rock .17 .85 .04 .07
Alternative .02 .80 .13 .04
Heavy metal .07 .75 .11 .04
Country .06 .05 .72 .03
Sound tracks .01 .04 .70 .17
Religious .23 .21 .64 .01
Pop .20 .06 .59 .45
Rap/hip-hop .19 .12 .17 .79
Soul/funk .39 .11 .11 .69
Electronica/dance .02 .15 .01 .60
Note. N 1,704. All factor loadings .40 or larger are in italics; the highest factor loadings for each dimension
are listed in boldface type.
1242
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
criteria (Benet-Martı´nez & John, 1998; Bentler, 1990; Loehlin,
1998).
2
The results indicated that although Model 1, the orthogonal
model, did provide a reasonable fit,
2
(77, N 1,383) 812.3
(GFI .92, AGFI .89, RMSEA .09, SRMR .09), Model 2,
which allowed for correlated factors, fit significantly better,
2
(6) 185.6, p .001;
2
(71, N 1,383) 626.69 (GFI
.94, AGFI .91, RMSEA .07, SRMR .06). As shown in
Figure 5, the intercorrelations among the music-preference dimen-
sions were relatively small, with only one (Upbeat and Conven-
tional with Energetic and Rhythmic) exceeding .20. Furthermore,
the factor loadings for all of the music genres were in the expected
direction. In short, the cross-sample congruence of the music-
preference dimensions identified in Study 2 and the CFA fit from
this study provide compelling evidence for the existence of four
music-preference dimensions.
Limitations. Although the results from the CFA provide sup-
port for the cross-sample generalizability of the music-preference
dimensions, two potential limitations undermine the generalizabil-
ity of the model. First, participants’ music preferences were de-
rived from self-reports. In theory, people know what they like and
what they do not like. However, relying exclusively on self-reports
of music preferences assumes that people are able to accurately
report on their preferences and fails to control for the potential
biases produced by impression-management motivations. An in-
dividual may enjoy listening to country music but might report no
preference for it if listening to country is considered “uncool.”
Second, our participants were attending a public university in
central Texas, a hotbed of country music, which raises concerns
about the generalizability of the results to other geographic re-
gions. It is not clear how Southern culture might influence partic-
ipants’ preferences. Would a similar music factor structure be
obtained among native New Yorkers, or even college students
living in New York City? Thus, it could be premature to conclude
that the music-preference dimensions identified in Studies 2 and 3
generalize across samples. To address these two limitations it is
necessary to examine music preferences using a methodology that
is not dependent on self-reports and does not oversample from a
particular geographic region. Study 4 was designed to address
these limitations.
2
A widely used fit index is the chi-square statistic. For small sample
sizes, a satisfactory fit is obtained when chi-square is approximately equal
to its degrees of freedom. However, the chi-square statistic is very sensitive
to sample size such that, when the sample size is large, slight discrepancies
in fit can lead one to reject an otherwise good-fitting model. Thus, for large
sample sizes researchers are encouraged to use additional indices to eval-
uate the fit of a model (Bentler, 1990; Loehlin, 1998). Widely used
alternatives include the goodness-of-fit index (GFI), the adjusted goodness-
of-fit index (AGFI), the root-mean-square error of approximation (RM-
SEA), and the standardized root-mean-square residual (SRMR). A RM-
SEA less than .10 reflects a good-fitting model and a value less than .05 an
excellent-fitting model (Steiger, 1989). According to Hu and Bentler
(1999), an SRMR less than .08 reflects a good-fitting model (for a detailed
review of the various fit indices, see Loehlin, 1998).
Figure 5. Standardized parameter estimates for Model 2 of the music-preference data in Study 3.
2
(71,
N 1,383) 626.69; goodness-of-fit index .94; adjusted goodness-of-fit index .91; root-mean-square error
of approximation .07; standardized root-mean-square residual .06. e error variance.
1243
MUSIC PREFERENCES
Study 4: Generalizability Across Samples, Methods, and
Geographic Regions
Recently, a number of online Web sites (e.g., audiogalaxy.com,
kazaa.com, morpheus.com, napster.com) have sprung up to allow
individuals to share and download music from the Internet. One
feature offered by some of these Web sites is the ability to view the
music libraries of individuals using the Web site. At the time this
research was conducted, one such music provider (audiogalaxy
.com) had a list of all of the users currently online around the
globe. For the United States, users were organized by state. Each
user was then linked to a separate page that contained a list of all
the songs that the user had downloaded since joining the site. The
lists represent behaviorally revealed preferences of individuals
across the country and are ideally suited to address the limitations
of Studies 2 and 3.
Method and Procedure
We used the features offered by audiogalaxy.com to survey the music
collections of people from around the United States. We downloaded the
music libraries of individuals from each of the 50 states and then catego-
rized the songs in each persons music library into music genres. To ensure
full geographic representation of music preferences within the United
States, 10 users from each state were randomly selected. Thus, the total
sample was composed of 500 individuals. Many of the users had only a few
songs in their music libraries, making it hard to obtain reliable estimates of
their music preferences. Therefore, we implemented a minimum criterion
of at least 20 songs. The number of songs in the remaining music libraries
ranged from 20 to over 500 songs. To ensure equal impact, we randomly
selected 20 songs from each eligible users music library, regardless of how
many songs were in their music library.
The 500 music libraries were divided among a group of seven judges so
that six judges had 70 libraries each and one had 80. Judges were then
trained to code the songs in each users music library into one of the 14
music genres covered in the STOMP: alternative, blues, classical, country,
electronica/dance, folk, heavy metal, rap/hip-hop, jazz, pop, religious,
rock, soul/funk, and sound tracks. If judges were unfamiliar with a song in
a participantsmusic library, they consulted with towerrecords.com or with
another judge to determine the appropriate genre. In instances in which the
appropriate genre of a song could not be determined by these means, the
song was not included in the analyses.
A users preference for a particular genre of music was determined by
the number of songs that appeared in each music genre. Thus, scores for
each of the genres could range from 0 (No preference)to20(Strong
preference). Because there were almost as many music categories as there
were songs for each user, a large number of the music categories contained
zeros. Consequently, the distribution of the music-preference data was
negatively skewed and corrected using Poisson transformations.
The only available information for each user was their username and the
songs in their music library, so we could not determine their gender or age.
However, according to the marketing department for a similar online music
Web site, approximately 60% of online music users are men and 40% are
women, and the average age of users is 25 years (sales department of
kazaa.com, personal communication, July 5, 2002).
3
Results and Discussion
To examine the generalizability of the music-preference dimen-
sions across methods and populations, we performed a CFA on the
audiogalaxy.com data using LISREL (Jo¨reskog & So¨rbom, 1989).
As in Study 3, we tested two models, one in which the factors were
specified as orthogonal and one in which the factors were allowed
to correlate. In both models, we specified four latent factors
representing the four music dimensions: All the genres that loaded
onto each of the respective factors identified in Study 2 were freely
estimated.
The results indicated that although Model 1, the orthogonal
model, provided a reasonable fit,
2
(77, N 500) 176.31
(GFI .95, AGFI .93, RMSEA .05, SRMR .06), Model 2,
which allowed for correlated factors, fit significantly better,
2
(6) 39.27, p .001;
2
(71, N 500) 137.05 (GFI .96,
AGFI .94, RMSEA .04, SRMR .05). As shown in Figure 6,
the intercorrelations among the music-preference dimensions were
generally small, with only one (Reflective and Complex with
Energetic and Rhythmic) exceeding .20. Furthermore, the factor
loadings for the music genres were generally strong, and all were
in the expected direction.
In sum, three separate studies of over 3,500 participants con-
verged on the finding that music preferences can be organized into
four independent dimensions: Reflective and Complex, Intense
and Rebellious, Upbeat and Conventional, and Energetic and
Rhythmic. Although the age range of the audiogalaxy.com sample
was probably not as broad as we had hoped, the convergent
findings provided strong evidence for the generalizability of the
music-preference dimensions. These dimensions generalized
across time, populations, method, and geographic region.
Study 5: Understanding the Music Dimensions
After we had identified some robust music-preference dimen-
sions, the next task was to identify the qualities that define them:
What are the common threads that hold these factors together?
Why do preferences for certain genres of music cluster together?
The attributes of music vary across a wide range of moods, energy
levels, complexities, and lyrical contents. For example, some
genres emphasize negative emotions (e.g., heavy metal), whereas
others emphasize positive emotions (e.g., religious); some genres
are technically complex (e.g., classical), whereas others tend to be
basic (e.g., country); some genres have relatively few songs with
vocals (e.g., jazz), whereas others only have songs with vocals
(e.g., pop). Thus, the objective of Study 5 was to systematically
examine the attributes of the four music-preference dimensions.
Method
Examining the properties of the music dimensions involved three steps.
First, we selected songs that exemplified the music genres defining each of
the music-preference dimensions. Second, we generated a set of specific
music attributes on which the exemplar songs could be judged. Third, a
group of judges independently rated the exemplar songs on each of the
music attributes.
Exemplar song selection. To determine the defining attributes of each
of the music-preference dimensions, we selected a sample of songs that
could serve as stimuli for judges to rate. Specifically, we selected songs to
serve as exemplars for the 14 music genres used in the previous studies:
alternative, blues, classical, country, electronica/dance, folk, heavy metal,
3
Audiogalaxy.com did not respond to our requests for the demographic
information of their subscribers. Therefore, we report here information
furnished by a similar online music provider (kazaa.com), which was
willing to provide us with general demographic information about online
music users.
1244
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
hip-hop/rap, jazz, pop, religious, rock, soul, and funk. Sound track music
was excluded because sound tracks can contain the musical styles of
practically every other music genre; in other words, there were extremely
few specific songs that were more prototypical of sound tracks than of
other genres.
Some songs blend styles from different genres, so it was necessary that
each song exemplify only one genre. To ensure this, we consulted with
various online music providers (e.g., towerrecords.com, audiogalaxy.com)
to identify the exemplar songs of each of the respective genres. Many
online music providers display essentialcompilation albums for a variety
of music genres. The essentialness of each recording is based on either the
number of units sold, customer recommendations, or reviews by music
critics. Using these resources, we created a pool of approximately 25
possible songs for each of the 14 music genres. Selecting only one
exemplary song as an index of a whole genre would not provide a very
reliable estimate of the characteristics of the genre or sufficient content
validity. To improve the content validity of the sets of songs representing
each genre, we selected 10 exemplary songs that represented a broad array
of styles, artists, and time periods within each genre. This process resulted
in a total of 140 songs (see Appendix for a list of the songs).
Music attribute selection. To select the music attributes, we used a
multistep procedure similar to the one described by Aaker, Benet-Martı´nez,
and Garolera (2001) to select commercial brands. Songs are often de-
scribed using terms that are also used to describe people (e.g., complex,
emotional, cheerful, reflective). Therefore, we began our item-selection
procedure with the pool of 300 person descriptors in the Adjective Check
List (ACL; Gough & Heilbrun, 1983). Usinga1(relevant)to3(irrelevant)
rating scale, three expert judges independently rated each of the 300 ACL
adjectives for their relevance in describing various aspects of music. All
attributes that were considered at least somewhat relevant (i.e.,a2onthe
scale) by at least two of the three judges were retained, resulting in an
initial pool of 130 attributes.
To increase the range of music attributes and to test the effectiveness of
the initial ACL-based procedures, a second step used a structured free-
description task in which four independent judges were asked to list all the
music attributes that came to mind while thinking about any and all types
of music. Using this procedure, only seven new attributes were identified
that had not been identified in the previous step; this finding was reassuring
because it underscored the effectiveness of the ACL-based item-generation
procedures.
In the third step, a separate group of seven judges independently eval-
uated the extent to which each attribute could be used to characterize
various aspects of music. Specifically, judges were instructed to first
indicate the extent to which each of the music attributes could be used to
describe various aspects of the music and/or the lyrics using a 5-point scale
(1 Not at all;5 Definitely), then to indicate which aspect of the music
the attribute best described using a three-valued categorization system (i.e.,
L only the lyrics, B both the lyrics and music, M only the music).
For example, if reflective was considered useful in describing a particular
aspect of music it might be given a 4, and if it was thought to be
characteristic of the lyrics only it would get an L.
The number of potential music attributes was very large, so we used a
high-relevance threshold (4.5 or higher) to ensure inclusion of only the
most relevant attributes. This strategy resulted in a list of 20 attributes:
clever, dreamy, relaxed, enthusiastic, simple, pleasant, energetic, loud,
cheerful/happy, uplifting, angry, depressing/sad, emotional, romantic,
rhythmic, frank/direct, boastful, optimistic, reflective, and bitter. Because
the expert judges used person descriptors as a starting point for generating
music attributes, a few important general musical attributes, such as tempo
(e.g., fast, slow) and mode (e.g., acoustic, electric), did not appear on the
Figure 6. Standardized parameter estimates for Model 2 of the music-preference data in Study 4.
2
(71, N
500) 137.05; goodness-of-fit index .96; adjusted goodness-of-fit index .94; root-mean-square error of
approximation .04; standardized root-mean-square residual .05. e error variance.
1245
MUSIC PREFERENCES
list. Thus, we supplemented the 20 attributes with an additional 5 attributes
(fast, slow, acoustic, electric, and voice) for a final list of 25 music
attributes.
Procedure. A group of seven judges, representing a variety of musical
tastes, independently rated the songs on the attributes. The 140 songs were
compiled onto CDs. The songs on the CDs were grouped by genre. To
reduce the impact of order effects, two sets of CDs that differed in song
order were created. Judges were unaware of the purpose of the study and
were simply instructed to listen to each song in its entirety, then to rate each
song on each of the music and lyric attributes using a 7-point scale with
endpoints at 1 (Extremely uncharacteristic)and7(Extremely characteris-
tic). For songs with no lyrics, judges were instructed to leave the lyric
attributes blank; there were 25 songs with no lyrics. Our analyses of
structure in Studies 24 were based on music preferences of ordinary
persons, so for this study, too, we were interested in ordinary persons
impressions of music (rather than the impressions of trained musicians).
Thus, judges were given no specific instructions about what information
they should use to make their judgments.
Results and Discussion
Reliability. To evaluate the reliability of judges attribute rat-
ings of the songs, Cronbachs alphas were computed across songs
for each attribute. In general, reliability was high. As can be seen
in Table 2, inter-rater reliability was highest for the general at-
tributes (M
.90), followed by the music and lyric attributes
(Ms .79, .79). Overall, the coefficients ranged from .43 for
ratings of the rhythm of the lyrics to .93 for the amount of singing.
Interestingly, reliability for some of the more metaphorical at-
tributes such as sad, angry, depressing, bitter, happy, relaxed, and
romantic was at least as high as more observable, literal attributes
such as whether the song was acoustic, electric, fast, or slow.
Distinguishing the music-preference dimensions. What are the
attributes that distinguish the music-preference dimensions? To
examine how the music dimensions differed in terms of musical
attributes, we performed analyses of variance (ANOVAs) on each
of the attributes within the three music-attribute categories (gen-
eral, lyrical, and musical), using music dimension as the indepen-
dent variable.
How did the music dimensions differ in terms of general at-
tributes? As shown in Figure 7, the music dimensions were sig-
nificantly different across the five general attributes; Fs(3, 136)
ranged from 6.68 to 67.13, all ps .001. In general, the Reflective
and Complex music dimension was slower in tempo than the other
dimensions, used mostly acoustical instruments, and had very little
singing. The Intense and Rebellious dimension was faster in
tempo, used mostly electric instruments, and had a moderate
amount of singing. The Upbeat and Conventional dimension was
moderate in tempo, used both acoustic and electric instruments,
and had a moderate amount of singing. The Energetic and Rhyth-
mic dimension was also moderate in tempo, used electric instru-
ments, and had a moderate amount of singing.
How did the music dimensions differ in terms of lyrical at-
tributes? As shown in Figure 8, the music dimensions were sig-
nificantly different across all of the lyric attributes except rhyth-
mic; all statistically significant Fs(3, 128) ranged from 4.10
to 33.02, ps .01. For presentational clarity, the lyric attributes
were divided into four general categories: complexity (e.g., simple,
clever), positive affect (e.g., cheerful/happy, romantic), negative
affect (e.g., depressing/sad, angry), and energy level (e.g., relaxed,
energetic). In general, the lyrics in the Reflective and Complex
dimension were perceived to be complex, to express both positive
and negative emotions, and to have a low level of energy. The
Intense and Rebellious dimension was perceived to be moderately
complex, low in positive affect, but high in negative affect and
energy level. The lyrics in the Upbeat and Conventional dimension
were perceived as simple and direct, low in negative affect, but
high in positive affect and energy level. The lyrics in the Energetic
and Rhythmic dimensions were perceived as being somewhat
complex, unemotional, and moderate in energy level.
How do the music dimensions differ in terms of musical at-
tributes? As shown in Figure 9, the music dimensions were sig-
nificantly different across the 15 music attributes; Fs(3, 136)
ranged from 3.06 to 46.08; ps .05. For presentational clarity, we
again divided the attributes into four general categories: complex-
ity, positive affect, negative affect, and energy level. The musical
attributes of the Reflective and Complex dimension were per-
ceived as complex, high in both positive and negative affect, yet
low in energy level. As with the lyric attributes of the Intense and
Rebellious dimension, the music attributes were perceived as mod-
erately complex, low in positive affect, and high in both negative
affect and energy level. The music attributes of the Upbeat and
Conventional dimension were perceived as simple and direct,
moderately high in positive affect, and low in both negative affect
and energy level. The music attributes of the Energetic and Rhyth-
mic dimension were perceived as moderately complex, unemo-
tional, and moderate in energy level.
Table 2
Interrater Reliability (Coefficient Alpha) of Judges Ratings of
the Music Attributes
Attribute
Music attribute category
General
(M 0.90)
Lyrics
(M 0.79)
Music
(M 0.79)
Fast 0.90
Slow 0.89
Acoustic 0.87
Electric 0.89
Voice 0.93
Frank/direct 0.62
Boastful 0.80
Optimistic 0.83
Reflective 0.62
Bitter 0.87
Clever 0.64 0.66
Dreamy 0.82 0.85
Relaxed 0.85 0.88
Enthusiastic 0.73 0.77
Simple 0.66 0.70
Pleasant 0.70 0.70
Energetic 0.84 0.86
Loud 0.84 0.85
Cheerful/happy 0.86 0.83
Uplifting 0.82 0.70
Angry 0.91 0.90
Depressing/sad 0.87 0.82
Emotional 0.85 0.78
Romantic 0.88 0.86
Rhythmic 0.43 0.45
Note. Means were calculated using Fishers r-to-z transformation. Blank
cell indicates that no data were collected for this attribute in this category.
1246
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
Summary
Overall, the judges ratings of the 140 songs shed light on the
underlying attributes that bind music genres together. In addition,
they suggest that the dimension labels we chose in Study 2 char-
acterize each dimension quite well. Analyses of the music at-
tributes paint rather interesting and unique pictures of each dimen-
sion. Whereas the Reflective and Complex dimension projects a
broad spectrum of both positive and negative emotions that is quite
complex in structure, the Intense and Rebellious dimension dis-
plays moderately complex structure and intense negative emotions.
The Upbeat and Conventional dimension expresses predominantly
positive emotions, is simple in structure, and is moderately ener-
getic, whereas the Energetic and Rhythmic dimension exhibits
comparatively less positive and negative emotion, is moderately
energetic, and tends to place greater emphasis on rhythm.
Study 6: Examining the Relationship Between Music
Preferences and Personality
In Studies 24, we identified four dimensions of music prefer-
ences that generalized across time, populations, methods, and
geographic region. In Study 5, we characterized each dimension in
terms of a variety of different music attributes. Having identified
and characterized the music-preference dimensions, we could ad-
dress a central question underlying this research: How are music
preferences related to existing personality characteristics?
Method
Participants. To examine the external correlates of the four music-
preference dimensions, we administered a number of tests of personality,
self-views, and cognitive ability to a sample of college students. Partici-
pants were from Studies 2 and 3 and the retest subsample from Study 2
(Ns 1,704, 1,383, and 118, respectively). In both Studies 2 and 3 (S2 and
S3), participants completed measures of personality and self-views, and
participants in the retest subsample (SS2) completed a test of cognitive
ability.
Measures of personality. Personality was assessed with a variety of
measures. To assess personality at a broad level, we included the Big Five
Inventory (BFI; John & Srivastava, 1999). The BFI consists of 44 items
that tap five broad personality domains. Items were rated on a 5-point scale
with endpoints at 1 (Disagree strongly)and5(Agree strongly).
The Personality Research FormDominance (Jackson, 1974) was ad-
ministered as a measure of interpersonal dominance strivings. Using a
truefalse response format, participants indicated their agreement with 16
statements.
We included the Social Dominance Orientation Scale (Pratto, Sidanius,
Stallworth, & Malle, 1994). This questionnaire consists of 14 items, which
tap individual differences in orientation to socially conservative ideals and
attitudes. Participants were asked to indicate their feeling toward each
statement using a 7-point Likert-type scale with endpoints at 1 (Very
negative)and7(Very positive).
The Brief Loquaciousness and Interpersonal Responsiveness Test
(Swann & Rentfrow, 2001) was administered to assess individual differ-
ences in interpersonal communication styles. Specifically, this test discrim-
inates between individuals who tend to express their thoughts and feelings
as soon as they come to mind (blirtatious [from the acronym for the test,
BLIRT] individuals) and individuals who tend to keep their thoughts to
themselves (nonblirtatious individuals). Participants indicated the extent to
which they agreed with eight items using a 5-point scale with endpoints
at1(Strongly disagree)and5(Strongly agree).
Self-esteem was assessed with the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (Rosen-
berg, 1965). This is a widely used measure of self-esteem and consists
of 10 statements. Participants were asked to indicate, using a 5-point
Figure 7. General attributes of each of the music-preference dimensions.
1247
MUSIC PREFERENCES
Likert-type scale with endpoints at 1 (Not at all)and5(Extremely) the
extent to which each statement was characteristic of themselves.
The Beck Depression Inventory (Beck, 1972) was included as a measure
of depression. This test assesses individual differences in depression and
captures the degree to which individuals have experienced depressive
thoughts and feelings during the preceding week. Participants responded
to 13 items, each with four statements, and indicated which statement best
described their feelings over the past week.
Self-views. We were also interested in how individuals self-views
relate to their music preferences. Using a 5-point Likert-type scale with
endpoints at 1 (Disagree strongly)and5(Agree strongly), participants
were asked to indicate the extent to which they saw themselves as politi-
cally liberal, politically conservative, physically attractive, wealthy, ath-
letic, and intelligent.
Cognitive ability. The Wonderlic IQ Test (Wonderlic, 1977) was ad-
ministered as a measure of verbal and analytic reasoning ability. The test
includes 50 items, and participants were given 12 min to complete as many
items as possible. Research among college samples has found that the test
is predictive of college grades (McKelvie, 1989) and ratings of self-
perceived intelligence (Paulhus, Lysy, & Yik, 1998).
Results and Discussion
To examine the relationship between music preferences and
personality, scale scores on each of the four dimensions were
computed. We then computed correlations between the music-
preference dimensions and scores on the measures of personality,
self-views, and cognitive ability. The patterns of correlations be-
tween the music-preference dimensions and the external correlates
are shown in Table 3.
4
The correlations presented in Table 3 reveal a fascinating pat-
tern of links between music preferences and personality, self-
views, and cognitive ability. For example, the Reflective and
Complex dimension was positively related to Openness to New
Experiences, self-perceived intelligence, verbal (but not analytic)
ability, and political liberalism and negatively related to social
dominance orientation and athleticism. These correlations, along
with item-level analyses of the BFI, suggest that individuals who
enjoy listening to reflective and complex music tend to be inven-
tive, have active imaginations, value aesthetic experiences, con-
sider themselves to be intelligent, tolerant of others, and reject
conservative ideals.
4
Research with musicians has suggested that men and women prefer to
play different musical instruments (Dibben, 2002; ONeill, 1997; ONeill
& Boulton, 1996), so it is reasonable to suppose that the relationships
between music preferences and personality could be moderated by sex.
However, when correlations between music preferences and personality
were computed separately for men and women, the magnitude and pattern
of the correlations were virtually identical for both sexes. Thus, the
correlations presented in Table 3 include both men and women.
Figure 8. Lyric attributes of each of the music-preference dimensions.
1248
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
The Intense and Rebellious dimension was positively related to
Openness to New Experiences, athleticism, self-perceived intelli-
gence, and verbal ability. Interestingly, despite previous findings
that this dimension contains music that emphasizes negative emo-
tions, individuals who prefer music in this dimension do not appear
to display signs of neuroticism or disagreeableness. Overall, indi-
viduals who prefer intense and rebellious music tend to be curious
about different things, enjoy taking risks, are physically active, and
consider themselves intelligent.
The external correlates of the Upbeat and Conventional dimen-
sion reveal positive correlations with Extraversion, Agreeableness,
Conscientiousness, conservatism, self-perceived physical attrac-
tiveness, and athleticism and negative correlations with Openness
to New Experiences, social dominance orientation, liberalism, and
verbal ability. Our analyses suggest that individuals who enjoy
listening to upbeat and conventional music are cheerful, socially
outgoing, reliable, enjoy helping others, see themselves as physi-
cally attractive, and tend to be relatively conventional.
The Energetic and Rhythmic dimension was positively related
to Extraversion, Agreeableness, blirtatiousness, liberalism, self-
perceived attractiveness, and athleticism and negatively related to
social dominance orientation and conservatism. Thus, individuals
who enjoy Energetic and Rhythmic music tend to be talkative, full
of energy, are forgiving, see themselves as physically attractive,
and tend to eschew conservative ideals.
As one would expect for such a broad array of constructs, the
magnitude of correlations varied greatly. To test the generalizabil-
ity of the correlations across samples, we computed columnvector
correlations for each of the four dimensions. Specifically, we
transformed the correlations using Fishers r-to-z formula and then
computed the correlation between the two columns of transformed
correlations. As shown in the bottom row of Table 3, the pattern of
correlations for each of the music dimensions was virtually iden-
tical across samples; columnvector correlations ranged from .851
for the Energetic and Rhythmic dimension to .977 for the Reflec-
tive and Complex dimension.
5
One noteworthy finding was the absence of substantial correla-
tions between the music-preference dimensions and Emotional
Stability, depression, and self-esteem, suggesting that chronic
emotional states do not have a strong effect on music preferences.
Within each music dimension, however, there are undoubtedly
songs that capture different emotional states. Therefore, the fact
that no relationship was found between music preferences and
chronic emotions does not indicate that emotions are not related to
5
It should be noted that strong columnvector correlations could be
generated merely from the inclusion of a mixture of constructs, some of
which correlate strongly and some of which correlate weakly with the
music-preference dimensions.
Figure 9. Music attributes of each of the music-preference dimensions.
1249
MUSIC PREFERENCES
music preferences. One possibility is that existing personality
dimensions influence the music-preference dimensions that indi-
viduals generally prefer and that emotional states influence the
mood of the music that individuals choose to listen to on any
given day. To gain a firm grasp of the link between music pref-
erences and emotions, future research should examine the emo-
tional valence of the music people choose to listen to while in
different emotional states.
General Discussion
The primary purpose of this research was to examine the land-
scape of music preferences, thereby laying the groundwork for a
theory of music preferences. In a series of studies, we examined
lay beliefs about music, the structure underlying music prefer-
ences, and the links between music preferences and personality.
One goal of this research was to determine how much importance
individuals give to music relative to other leisure activities. Over-
all, the results from Study 1 indicate that music is at least as
important as most other leisure activities. Participants believed that
their music preferences revealed a substantial amount of informa-
tion about their own personalities and self-views and the person-
alities of other people. Furthermore, participants reported listening
to music very frequently in a variety of different contexts. These
latter findings converge nicely with recent work by Mehl and
Pennebaker (2003), who sampled peoples everyday activities and
found that individuals listened to music during approximately 14%
of their waking lives, roughly the same amount of time as they
spent watching television and half the amount of time they spent
engaged in conversations. Thus, our data support empirically what
might seem self-evident to many: Music is important to people,
and they listen to it frequently.
Using multiple samples, methods, and geographic regions, three
independent studies converged to reveal four dimensions of music
preferences. The findings presented in Studies 24 are important
because they are the first to suggest that there is a clear, robust, and
meaningful structure underlying music preferences. In addition,
the results from Study 5 provide valuable information about the
music attributes that differentiate the music-preference dimen-
sions: The dimensions can be distinguished by their levels of
complexity, emotional valence, and energy level.
Although early research on music preferences suggested links
between certain music genres and certain personality characteris-
Table 3
External Correlates of the Music-Preference Dimensions
Criterion measure M (SD)
Reflective and
Complex
Intense and
Rebellious
Upbeat and
Conventional
Energetic and
Rhythmic
S2 S3 S2 S3 S2 S3 S2 S3
Personality
Big Five
Extraversion 3.42 (0.85) .01 .02 .00 .08* .24* .15* .22* .19*
Agreeableness 3.80 (0.62) .01 .03 .04 .01 .23* .24* .08* .09*
Conscientiousness 3.57 (0.64) .02 .06 .04 .03 .15* .18* .00 .03
Emotional Stability 3.11 (0.81) .08* .04 .01 .01 .07 .04 .01 .01
Openness 3.75 (0.61) .44* .41* .18* .15* .14* .08* .03 .04
Interpersonal dominance 1.52 (0.25) .07* .06* .04 .06* .05 .08* .04 .05
Social dominance 2.70 (1.00) .16* .12* .06* .04 .06* .14* .09* .10*
Blirtatiousness
a
2.95 (0.70) .00 .00 .01 .07* .04 .01 .08* .11*
Self-esteem 3.05 (0.69) .02 .00 .02 .01 .07* .05 .06* .04
Depression 0.87 (0.34) .01 .03 .03 .03 .08* .07* .02 .04
Self-views
Politically liberal 3.17 (1.22) .15* .09* .03 .08* .20* .17* .07* .14*
Politically conservative 2.83 (1.21) .09* .03 .04 .03 .24* .23* .06* .09*
Physically attractive 3.69 (0.91) .00 .03 .04 .05 .07* .09* .15* .08*
Wealthy 2.86 (1.11) .04 .06 .03 .00 .08* .05 .02 .01
Athletic 3.33 (1.26) .07* .08* .06* .07* .13* .12* .11* .07*
Intelligent 4.22 (0.71) .10* .06* .07* .08* .05* .02 .02 .01
Cognitive ability (Wonderlic)
b
Verbal 19.09 (3.72) .19* .19* .18* .01
Analytical 6.11 (2.16) .08 .05 .02 .08
Column vector correlations .977 .863 .923 .851
Note. Ns 1,704, 1,383, and 118 for S2, S3, and SS2, respectively. Means and standard deviations are averaged across samples. Dashes in cells indicate
data were not collected. S2 sample from Study 2; S3 sample from Study 3; SS2 sub-sample from Study 2.
a
Blirtatiousness tendency to express thoughts and feelings as soon as they come to mind (from the acronym for the Brief Loquaciousness and
Interpersonal Responsiveness Test [BLIRT]; see Swann & Rentfrow, 2001).
b
SS2.
* p .05.
1250
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
tics, the picture they provided was not complete. Using a broad and
systematic selection of music genres and personality dimensions,
the results from Study 6 cast more light on the variables that link
individuals to their music of choice. Across two samples of college
students, relationships between music preferences and existing
personality dimensions, self-views, and cognitive abilities were
identified.
Developing a Theory of Music Preferences
The research reported here fits into a broader agenda of devel-
oping a theory of music preferences. What should be asked of such
a theory? One important question pertains to the formation of
music preferences. How do music preferences develop? What
factors influence their development? A second question relates to
the trajectory of music preferences. How, when, and why do music
preferences change? A theory of music preferences should also
address the impact of music on behavior. How do music prefer-
ences influence behavior and how do individuals make use of
music in their everyday lives?
The research presented here indicates that personality, self-
views, and cognitive abilities could all have roles to play in the
formation and maintenance of music preferences. The results from
Study 6 are consistent with the idea that personality has an impact
on music preferences. Just as the social and physical environments
that people select and shape reflect their personalities (Buss, 1987;
Gosling et al., 2002; Snyder & Ickes, 1985), so too do their
musical environments. Our findings show that, for example, pref-
erence for cheerful music with vocals (the Upbeat and Conven-
tional dimension) was positively related to Extraversion whereas
preference for artistic and intricate music (e.g., the Reflective and
Complex dimension) was positively related to Openness to New
Experiences. Future research that includes narrower facets of per-
sonality is needed to provide a finer grained picture of the effects
of personality on music preferences.
Music preferences also appear to be shaped by self-views.
Theorists concerned with social identity and the self have pointed
out that the social environments that individuals select serve to
reinforce their self-views (e.g., Gosling et al., 2002; Swann, 1987,
1996; Swann et al., 2002; Tajfel & Turner, 1986), and our findings
suggest that people may select music for similar reasons. This can
happen in two ways. First, music preferences could be used to
make self-directed identity claims (Gosling et al., 2002). That is,
individuals might select styles of music that reinforce their self-
views; for example, individuals may listen to esoteric music to
reinforce a self-view of being sophisticated. Our findings provide
evidence consistent with this idea: Individuals with a conservative
self-view preferred conventional styles of music (the Upbeat and
Conventional dimension), whereas individuals with an athletic
self-view preferred vigorous music (the Intense and Rebellious
dimension).
Music can also be used to make other-directed identity claims
(Gosling et al., 2002). That is, individuals might select styles of
music that allow them to send a message to others about who they
are or how they like to be seen; for instance, individuals who listen
to heavy metal music at a loud volume with their car windows
rolled down may be trying to convey a tough image to others.
Evidence for other-directed identity claims has been provided by
research suggesting that people use music as a badge for others to
see (North & Hargreaves, 1999). Thus, music preferences may
operate at different levels, both reinforcing how one sees oneself
and sending messages to others.
Music preferences also appear to be influenced by cognitive
ability. The relationship between cognitive ability and music pref-
erences is consistent with the idea that people prefer music that
provides optimal levels of stimulation. Berlyne (1971, 1974) hy-
pothesized that individuals prefer aesthetic stimuli that produce
moderate amounts of stimulation to objects that produce too much
or too little stimulation. Previous research on aesthetic preferences
in literature and visual arts has supported this notion, and this
suggests that individual differences in cognitive complexity mod-
erate preferences for particular aesthetic stimuli. Whereas cogni-
tively complex individuals tend to prefer complex aesthetic stim-
uli, less cognitively complex individuals tend to prefer simple
aesthetic stimuli (Barron, 1955; France`s, 1976; Kammann, 1966).
The relationship between intelligence and preference for complex
music supports this previous work and suggests that the optimal
level of stimulation for highly intelligent individuals is produced
by complex music whereas the optimal level of stimulation for less
intelligent individuals is produced by comparatively simpler mu-
sic. Future research that examines individual differences in cog-
nitive complexity and music complexity could shed light on the
possible mechanisms underlying the formation and maintenance of
music preferences.
If music preferences are partially determined by personality,
self-views, and cognitive abilities, then knowing what kind of
music a person likes could serve as a clue to his or her personality,
self-views, and cognitive abilities. The participants in Study 1
certainly believed that knowing a persons music preferences
could reveal valid information about what he or she is like. In
addition, the findings from Study 6 suggest that knowing peoples
music preferences can provide information about their Openness,
Extraversion, political orientation, and intelligence. It is also pos-
sible that music preferences reveal information about other facets
of personality, such as values and goals. Future research is needed
to examine the role of music preferences in personperception
processes.
In addition to the influences of personality, self-views and
cognitive abilities, a full theory of music preferences will need to
examine many other possible determinants. It seems likely that
cultural and environmental influences will influence the music an
individual likes. For example, individuals growing up in small
rural towns in Texas will probably be exposed to a very different
set of music than individuals growing up in metropolitan New
York. It also seems likely that the patterns of influences will vary
across the life course such that individuals may first adopt the
preferences of their parents; later become influenced by their
peers; and then, as they develop more autonomy, their personali-
ties may play a larger role. If so, we would expect stronger links
between music preferences and personality in older rather than
younger participants. Thus, one important direction for future
research would be tracking the trajectory of music preferences
across the life course.
A theory of music preferences should also be applicable across
groups and contexts. Thus, one major goal is to examine the extent
to which the specific structure identified in this research general-
izes to other groups. Is there a similar structure underlying music
preferences across different age groups? Would a similar structure
1251
MUSIC PREFERENCES
emerge in Asia, Europe, Canada, and elsewhere? New styles of
music are being created all the time and in different cultures, so it
is reasonable to assume that the four music-preference dimensions
found here would not be found among a group of elderly people
living in a remote part of Indonesia. However, even though music
genres come and go, there may be a finite number of music-
preference dimensions that satisfy or reflect certain psychological
needs. In other words, regardless of the time period or culture,
there may be a limited number of styles of music that cluster
together to form Reflective and Complex, Intense and Rebellious,
Upbeat and Conventional, and Energetic and Rhythmic music
dimensions. Thus, even if the specific structure identified in this
research is not universal, a good theory of musical preferences will
be able to explain how, when, and why the structures might differ.
A theory of music preferences should also explain how individ-
uals make use of music. One possibility is that individuals use
music as a means of regulating their emotions in everyday life. Do
individuals seek out music that is consistent with their current
mood or select music to change their mood? The findings from
Study 6 suggest that chronic affect does not influence peoples
standing on any of the music-preference dimensions, but emotional
states may influence the mood of the music chosen within their
preferred dimension. For example, when a person high on the
Reflective and Complex dimension is feeling cheerful, she may
listen to jazz music that is lively, but when she is feeling sad she
may choose the blues. This leads to another interesting question:
Does emotional and physiological arousal influence music prefer-
ences? Previous research on music preferences has suggested that
arousal does play a role (McNamara & Ballard, 1999). Moreover,
research linking emotional states and physiological arousal has
indicated that anger tends to be associated with a high heart rate,
happiness with a moderate heart rate, and depression with a low
heart rate (Averill, 1969; Cacioppo, Klein, Bernston, & Hatfield,
1993; Ekman, Levenson, & Frieson, 1983). Our findings dovetail
nicely with this research. Judges in Study 5 perceived angry music
as highly energetic, happy music as moderately energetic, and
depressing music as least energetic. One possibility is that people
choose a tempo of music that is consistent with the heart rate that
characterizes their current or desired mood.
There is clearly a long way to go before a theory of music
preferences can be articulated fully. However, the research pre-
sented here has provided a foundation on which future research
can build, and we have suggested just a few of the many directions
that such research can take. Ultimately, we hope that research will
begin to inform our understanding of music, a phenomenon that
pervades many aspects of everyday life but has hitherto been
virtually ignored in mainstream social and personality psychology.
Conclusion
It is clear to us that music can contribute much to the under-
standing of many psychological phenomena. From personality and
the self to social cognition and emotions, adding music to the
research gamut can open a new window into peoples everyday
lives. To facilitate this goal, we have provided an initial map of the
music-preferences terrain and identified some potential landmarks
for future exploration.
More broadly, integrating facets of peoples everyday lives into
the research repertoire will undoubtedly cast light on important
psychological processes that have remained in the shadow of
mainstream topics in social and personality psychology. Music is
only one of those facets. Thus, we urge social and personality
psychologists to broaden their research foci to include aspects of
peoples daily lives and to develop an ecologically sensitive de-
piction of social behavior.
References
Aaker, J., Benet-Martı´nez, V., & Garolera, J. (2001). Consumption sym-
bols as carriers of culture: A study of Japanese and Spanish brand
personality constructs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
81, 249264.
Arnett, J. (1992). The soundtrack of recklessness: Musical preferences and
reckless behavior among adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Research,
7, 313331.
Averill, J. R. (1969). Autonomic response patterns during sadness and
mirth. Psychophysiology, 5, 399414.
Barron, F. X. (1955). The disposition toward originality. Journal of Ab-
normal and Social Psychology, 51, 478485.
Beck, A. T. (1972). Depression: Causes and treatments. Philadelphia:
University of Pennsylvania Press.
Benet-Martı´nez, V., & John, O. P. (1998). Los cinco grandes across
cultures and ethnic groups: Multitraitmultimethod analyses of the Big
Five in Spanish and English. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-
ogy, 75, 729750.
Bentler, P. M. (1990). Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psy-
chological Bulletin, 107, 238246.
Berlyne, D. E. (1971). Aesthetics and psychobiology. New York: Appleton-
Century-Crofts.
Berlyne, D. E. (1974). The new experimental aesthetics. In D. E. Berlyne
(Ed.), Studies in the new experimental aesthetics: Steps toward an
objective psychology of aesthetic appreciation. New York: Halsted
Press.
Besson, M., Faita, F., Peretz, I., Bonnel, A. M., & Requin, J. (1998).
Singing in the brain: Independence of lyrics and tunes. Psychological
Science, 9, 494498.
Bharucha, J. J., & Mencl, W. E. (1996). Two issues in auditory cognition:
Self-organization of octave categories and pitch-invariant pattern recog-
nition. Psychological Science, 7, 142149.
Blood, A. J., & Zatorre, R. J. (2001). Intensely pleasurable responses to
music correlate with activity in brain regions implicated in reward and
emotion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 11818
11823.
Blood, A. J., Zatorre, R. J., Bermudez, P., & Evans, A. C. (1999).
Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate with
activity in paralimbic brain regions. Nature Neuroscience, 2, 382387.
Buss, D. M. (1987). Selection, evocation, and manipulation. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 12141221.
Cacioppo, J. T., Klein, D. J., Bernston, G. G., & Hatfield, E. (1993). The
psychophysiology of emotion. In M. Lewis & J. M. Haviland (Eds.),
Handbook of emotions (pp. 119142). New York: Guilford Press.
Cattell, R. B. (1966). The scree test for the number of factors. Sociological
Methods and Research, 1, 245276.
Cattell, R. B., & Anderson, J. C. (1953a). The I.P.A.T. Music Preference
Test of Personality. Champaign, IL: Institute for Personality and Ability
Testing.
Cattell, R. B., & Anderson, J. C. (1953b). The measurement of personality
and behavior disorders by the I.P.A.T. music preference test. Journal of
Applied Psychology, 37, 446454.
Cattell, R. B., & Saunders D. R. (1954). Musical preferences and person-
ality diagnosis: A factorization of one hundred and twenty themes.
Journal of Social Psychology, 39,324.
1252
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
Chaffin, R., & Imreh, G. (2002). Practicing perfection: Piano performance
as expert memory. Psychological Science, 13, 342349.
Chey, J., & Holzman, P. S. (1997). Perceptual organization in schizophre-
nia: Utilization of the Gestalt principles. Journal of Abnormal Psychol-
ogy, 106, 530538.
Clynes, M. (1982). Music, mind, and brain: The neuropsychology of music.
New York: Plenum Press.
Crozier, W. R. (1998). Music and social influence. In D. J. Hargreaves &
A. C. North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 6783). New
York: Oxford University Press.
Deutsch, D. (Ed.). (1999). The psychology of music (2nd ed.). San Diego,
CA: Academic Press.
Diamond, J. (2002). The therapeutic power of music. In S. Shannon (Ed.),
Handbook of complementary and alternative therapies in mental health
(pp. 517537). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Dibben, N. (2002). Gender identity and music. In R. A. R. MacDonald,
D. J. Hargreaves, & D. Miell (Eds.), Musical identities (pp. 117133).
New York: Oxford University Press.
Dorow, L. G. (1975). Conditioning music and approval as new reinforcers
for imitative behavior with the severely retarded. Music Therapy, 12,
3040.
Drayna, D., Manichaikul, A., de Lange, M., Sneider, H., & Spector, T.
(2001, March 9). Genetic correlates of musical pitch recognition in
humans. Science, 291, 19691972.
Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Frieson, W. V. (1983, September 16).
Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Sci-
ence, 221, 12081210.
France`s, R. (1976). Comparative effects of six collative variables on
interest and preference in adults of different educational levels. Journal
of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 6279.
Funder, D. C. (2001). Personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 52,
197221.
Gosling, S. D., Ko, S. J., Mannarelli, T., & Morris, M. E. (2002). A room
with a cue: Judgments of personality based on offices and bedrooms.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 379398.
Gough, H., & Heilbrun, A. (1983). The Adjective Check List manual. Palo
Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
Gowensmith, N. W., & Bloom, L. J. (1997). The effects of heavy metal
music on arousal and anger. Journal of Music Therapy, 1,3345.
Hargreaves, D. J., & North, A. C. (Eds.) (1997). The social psychology of
music. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hilliard, R. E. (2001). The use of cognitivebehavioral music therapy in
the treatment of women with eating disorders. Music Therapy Perspec-
tives, 19, 109113.
Hogan, R. (1998). Reinventing personality. Journal of Social and Clinical
Psychology, 17,110.
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor
analysis. Psychometrika, 30, 179185.
Hu, L., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance
structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struc-
tural Equation Modeling, 6, 155.
Jackson, D. N. (1974). Manual for the Personality Research Form. Go-
shen, NY: Research Psychology Press.
Jellison, J. A., & Flowers, P. J. (1991). Talking about music: Interviews
with disabled and nondisabled children. Journal of Research in Music
Education, 39, 322333.
John, O. P., Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (1991). The basic level in
personality-trait hierarchies: Studies of trait use and accessibility in
different contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60,
348361.
John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History,
measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. John
(Eds.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 102138).
New York: Guilford Press.
Jo¨reskog K. G., & So¨rbom, D. (1989). LISREL 7: A guide to the program
and applications (2nd ed.). Chicago: SPSS.
Kammann, R. (1966). Verbal complexity and preferences in poetry. Jour-
nal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 5, 536540.
Kemp, A. E. (1996). The musical temperament: Psychology and person-
ality of musicians. New York: Oxford University Press.
Krumhansl, C. L. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. New
York: Oxford University Press.
Krumhansl, C. L. (2000). Rhythm and pitch in music cognition. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 126, 159179.
Krumhansl, C. L. (2002). Music: A link between cognition and emotion.
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11,4550.
Little, P., & Zuckerman, M. (1986). Sensation seeking and music prefer-
ences. Personality and Individual Differences, 7, 575577.
Loehlin, J. C. (1998). Latent variable models: An introduction to factor,
path, and structural analysis (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Marin, O. S. M., & Perry, D. W. (1999). Neurological aspects of music
perception and performance. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of
music (pp. 653724). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
McCown, W., Keiser, R., Mulhearn, S., & Williamson, D. (1997). The role
of personality and gender in preferences for exaggerated bass in music.
Personality and Individual Differences, 23, 543547.
McKelvie, S. J. (1989). The Wonderlic Personnel Test: Reliability and
validity in an academic setting. Psychological Reports, 65, 161162.
McNamara, L., & Ballard, M. E. (1999). Resting arousal, sensation seek-
ing, and music preference. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology
Monographs, 125, 229250.
Mehl, M. R., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003). The sounds of social life: A
psychometric analysis of studentsdaily social environments and natural
conversations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 857
870.
Murphy, G. L. (1982). Cue validity and levels of categorization. Psycho-
logical Bulletin, 91, 174177.
North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (1999). Music and adolescent identity.
Music Education Research, 1,7592.
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & McKendrick, J. (1997, November 13).
In-store music affects product choice. Nature, 390, 132.
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & McKendrick, J. (2000). The effects of
music on atmosphere in a bank and a bar. Journal of Applied Social
Psychology, 30, 15041522.
North, A. C., Hargreaves, D. J., & ONeill, S. A. (2000). The importance
of music to adolescents. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70,
255272.
ONeill, S. A. (1997). Gender and music. In D. J. Hargreaves & A. C.
North (Eds.), The social psychology of music (pp. 4660). New York:
Oxford University Press.
ONeill, S. A., & Boulton, M. J. (1996). Boys and girls preferences for
musical instruments: A function of gender? Psychology of Music, 24,
171183.
Oyama, T., Hatano, K., Sato, Y., Kudo, M., Spintge, R., & Droh, R. (1983).
Endocrine effect of anxiolytic music in dental patients. In R. Droh & R.
Spintge (Eds.), Angst, Schmerz, Musik in der Anasthesie [Anxiety, pain,
music in anesthesia] (pp. 143146). Basel, Switzerland: Editiones
Roche.
Paulhus, D. L., Lysy, D., & Yik, M. S. M. (1998). Self-report measures of
intelligence: Are they useful as proxy measures of IQ? Journal of
Personality, 66, 525554.
Pedhauzer, E. J., & Schmelkin, L. P. (1991). Measurement, design, and
analysis: An integrated approach. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Peretz, I., Gagnon, L., & Bouchard, B. (1998). Music and emotion:
Perceptual determinants, immediacy and isolation after brain damage.
Cognition, 68, 111141.
Peretz, I., & Hebert, S. (2000). Toward a biological account of music
experience. Brain and Cognition, 42, 131134.
1253
MUSIC PREFERENCES
Pratto, F., Sidanius, J., Stallworth, L. M., & Malle, B. F. (1994). Social
dominance orientation: A personality variable predicting social and
political attitudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67,
741763.
Radocy, R. E., & Boyle, J. D. (1979). Psychological foundations of musical
behavior. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
Rauschecker, J. P. (2001). Cortical plasticity and music. In R. J. Zatorre &
I. Peretz (Eds.), Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences: Vol. 930.
The biological foundations of music (pp. 330336). New York: New
York Academy of Sciences.
Rider, M. S., Floyd, J. W., & Kirkpatrick, J. (1985). The effect of music,
imagery, and relaxation on adrenal corticoids and the re-entrainment of
circadian rhythms. Journal of Music Therapy, 22,4658.
Rosenberg, M. (1965). Society and the adolescent self-image. Princeton,
NJ: Princeton University Press.
Rozin, P. (2001). Social psychology and science: Some lessons from
Solomon Asch. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5,214.
Sloboda, J. A. (1985). The musical mind: The cognitive psychology of
music. New York: Oxford University Press.
Snyder, M., & Ickes, W. (1985). Personality and social behavior. In G.
Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 2,
pp. 883947). New York: Random House.
Standley, J. (1992). Meta-analysis of research in music and medical treat-
ment: Effect size as a basis for comparison across multiple dependent
and independent variables. In R. Spintge & R. Droh (Eds.), Musical
medicine (pp. 364378). St. Louis, MO: MMB Music.
Steiger, J. H. (1989). EzPATH: Causal modeling. Evanston, IL: SYSTAT.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1987). Identity negotiation: Where two roads meet.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 10381051.
Swann, W. B., Jr. (1996). Self-traps: The elusive quest for higher self-
esteem. New York: Freeman.
Swann, W. B., Jr., & Rentfrow, P. J. (2001). Blirtatiousness: Cognitive,
behavioral, and physiological consequences of rapid responding. Jour-
nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 11601175.
Swann, W. B., Jr., Rentfrow, P. J., & Guinn, J. S. (2002). Self-verification:
The search for coherence. In M. Leary & J. Tagney (Eds.), Handbook of
self and identity (pp. 367383). New York: Guilford Press.
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup
behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of inter-
group relations (pp. 124). Chicago: Nelson-Hall.
Tarrant, M., North, A. C., & Hargreaves, D. J. (2000). English and
American adolescents reasons for listening to music. Psychology of
Music, 28, 166173.
Todd, N. P. M. (1999). Motion in music: A neurobiological perspective.
Music Perception, 17, 115126.
Wigram, T., Saperston, B., & West, R. (1995). The art and science of music
therapy: A handbook. Chur, Switzerland: Harwood Academic.
Wonderlic, E. F. (1977). Wonderlic Personnel Test manual. Northfield, IL:
Wonderlic and Associates.
Zwick, W. R., & Velicer, W. F. (1986). Comparison of five rules for
determining the number of components to retain. Psychological Bulletin,
99, 432442.
1254
RENTFROW AND GOSLING
Appendix
Exemplar Songs for Each of the 14 Music Genres
Genre Song Artist/Composer Genre Song Artist/Composer
Music Dimension 1: Reflective and Complex
Blues Nobody Loves Me But My Mother B. B. King
Spoonful Howling Wolf
Hideaway John Mayall and Blues Breakers
40 Days and 40 Nights Muddy Waters
Rays Blues Ray Charles
Train My Baby Robert Lockwood Jr.
In Step Stevie Ray Vaughan
Mama He Treats Your Daughter Mean Susan Tedeschi
Already Gone Robert Cray
T-Bone Blues T-Bone Walker
Folk Precious Memories Bill Monroe
Blowing in the Wind Bob Dylan
For What Its Worth Buffalo Springfield
Become You Indigo Girls
Fire and Rain James Taylor
Riverboat Set: Denis Dillons Square
Dance Polka, Dancing on the
Riverboat
John Whelan
Packin Truck Leadbelly
Ride Nick Drake
Sounds of Silence Simon and Garfunkel
House of the Rising Sun Joan Baez
Classical Six Suites for Cello: Suite 1 Johann Sebastian Bach
Symphony No. 9, Op. 125: 4th
movement (PrestoAllegro assai;
Ode to Joy)
Ludwig van Beethoven
Gianni Schicci: O mio babbino caro Giacomo Puccini
The Tale of Tsar Sultan: Flight of the
Bumblebee
Nikolai Andreyevich Rimsky-
Korsakov
Clair de Lune Debussy
Marriage of Figaro, K. 492: Overture Wolfgang Amadeus Mozart
Madama Butterfly: Un bel di vedremo Giacomo Puccini
Ave Maria Franz Schubert
The Four Seasons: Spring Antonio Vivaldi
Die Walku¨re: Ride of the Valkyries Richard Wagner
Jazz What a Difference a Day Makes Billie Holiday
Time Out Dave Brubek
The Feeling of Jazz Duke Ellington
Stella by Starlight Herbie Hancock
Giant Steps John Coltrane
The Look of Love Diana Krall
All Blues Miles Davis
Afternoon Pat Metheny
Summer in the City Quincy Jones
The Girl from Ipanema Stan Getz
(Appendix continues)
Music Dimension 2: Intense and Rebellious
Alternative Narcissus Alanis Morrisette
Song 2 Blur
Its the End of the World REM
Coming Down the Mountain Janes Addiction
Why Go Pearl Jam
Bullet With Butterfly Wings Smashing Pumpkins
Bleed American Jimmy Eat World
Verse Chorus Verse Nirvana
Linger Cranberries
Everlong Foo Fighters
Heavy metal Fight Song Marilyn Manson
Points of Authority Linkin Park
Angel of Death Slayer
Symphony of Destruction Megadeath
Welcome to the Jungle Guns N Roses
Crazy Train Black Sabbath
Crawling in the Dark Hoobastank
Rollin Limp Bizkit
Too Bad Nickleback
War System of a Down
Rock Mary Janes Last Dance Tom Petty
Jump Van Halen
Jealous Again Black Crows
Voodoo Child Jimi Hendrix
Brown Sugar Rolling Stones
YYZ Rush
Money Pink Floyd
Living on the Edge Aerosmith
San Berdino Frank Zappa
Living Loving Maid (Shes Just a
Woman)
Led Zeppelin
Music Dimension 3: Upbeat and Conventional
Country A Better Man Clint Black
Please Come to Boston David Allen Coe
If the South Would Have Won Hank Williams Jr.
Rusty Cage Johnny Cash
Ready to Run Dixie Chicks
Girls With Guitars The Judds
Whiskey River Willie Nelson
Im Out of Here Shania Twain
If the World Had a Front Porch Alan Jackson
When Love Finds You Vince Gill
Appendix (continued)
Genre Song Artist/Composer Genre Song Artist/Composer
Music Dimension 3: Upbeat and Conventional (continued)
Religious Amen Larnell Harris
Rock of Ages Praise Band
Where There is Faith 4Him
Awesome God Rich Mullins
Lord I Lift Your Name on High DC Talk
Smell the Color 9 Chris Rice
If We Ever Take 6
Come, Now Is the Time to
Worship
WOW Worship
All Rise Babbie Mason
Your Love, Oh Lord Third Day
Pop Im a Slave (4 U) Britney Spears
We Fit Together O-Town
Dont Make Me Love You Christina Aguilera
Material Girl Madonna
Shake Your Body (Down to the
Ground)
The Jacksons
Tell Me That Im Dreaming Backstreet Boys
Independent Women Part 1 Destinys Child
Im Real (Remix) Jennifer Lopez featuring Ja Rule
Bye Bye Bye Nsync
My Love Grows Deeper
(Everyday)
Nelly Furtado
Music Dimension 4: Energetic and Rhythmic
Funk Superbad Part 1 James Brown
Celebration Kool and the Gang
Thats the Way (I Like It) KC and the Sunshine Band
Tear the Roof off the Sucker (Give
Up the Funk)
George Clinton and Parliament
Its Not the Crime Tower of Power
Dynamite Sly and the Family Stone
Pick Up the Pieces Average White Band
Shaft Isaac Hayes
Ecstasy The Ohio Players
Sir Duke Stevie Wonder
Music Dimension 4: Energetic and Rhythmic (continued)
Hip-Hop All Good De La Soul
Public Enemy #1 Public Enemy
Can I Kick It? A Tribe Called Quest
Dont See Us The Roots
Hypnotize Notorious B.I.G.
Funky for You Common
Easy Street Eazy-E
Shes a Bitch Missy Misdemeanor Elliot
2 of Amerikaz Most Wanted Tupac Shakur (featuring Snoop
Doggy Dogg)
The Next Episode Dr. Dre (featuring Nate Dogg,
Snoop Dogg)
Soul Everything Is Everything Lauryn Hill
Cant Get Enough of Your Love
Babe
Barry White
If You Dont Know Me By Now Marvin Gaye
Cry For You Jodeci
L-O-V-E (Love) Al Green
Chain of Fools Aretha Franklin
Bag Lady Eryka Badu
Aint No Sunshine When Shes
Gone
Bobby Blue Bland
Untitled (How Does it Feel) Dangelo
Id Rather Be With You Bootsy Collins
Electronica Kalifornia Fatboy Slim
Ibiza Mix Paul Oakenfold
Violently Happy Bjork
Radiation Ruling the Nation Massive Attack
Trans-Europe Express Kraftwerk
Roll It Up The Crystal Method
Never Let Me Down Again Depeche Mode
Why Cant It Stop Moby
Watercolors LTJ Bukem
What Does Your Soul Look Like DJ Shadow
Received August 5, 2002
Accepted October 15, 2002