Hamline University Hamline University
DigitalCommons@Hamline DigitalCommons@Hamline
School of Education and Leadership Student
Capstone Theses and Dissertations
School of Education and Leadership
Summer 2021
The Texts Matter: Essential Text Characteristics For The Texts Matter: Essential Text Characteristics For
Comprehension Intervention In The Intermediate Grades Comprehension Intervention In The Intermediate Grades
Tia Clasen
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.hamline.edu/hse_all
Part of the Education Commons
Recommended Citation Recommended Citation
Clasen, Tia, "The Texts Matter: Essential Text Characteristics For Comprehension Intervention In The
Intermediate Grades" (2021).
School of Education and Leadership Student Capstone Theses and
Dissertations
. 4551.
https://digitalcommons.hamline.edu/hse_all/4551
This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the School of Education and Leadership at
DigitalCommons@Hamline. It has been accepted for inclusion in School of Education and Leadership Student
Capstone Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@Hamline. For more
information, please contact [email protected].
THE TEXTS MATTER:
ESSENTIAL TEXT CHARACTERISTICS FOR COMPREHENSION INTERVENTION
IN THE INTERMEDIATE GRADES
By
Tia Botsford Clasen
A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of
Doctorate in Education
Hamline University
St. Paul, Minnesota
2021
Dissertation Chair: Dr. Jennifer Carlson, School of Education, Hamline University
Readers:
Dr. Panayiota Kendeou, College of Education and Human Development, University of
Minnesota - Twin Cities
Dr. Kristen McMaster, College of Education and Human Development, University of Minnesota
- Twin Cities
1
DEDICATION
To my son, Samuel,
for your selflessness and countless sacrifices that this journey should come to fruition.
To my daughter, Alana,
For always being my inspiration to better myself.
To Mother and Jim,
Though I cannot see you, I can feel you with me, and I hope I have made you proud.
To all of my students whom I had the great fortune to teach,
Your struggles have stayed with me, your successes have launched me, and I am forever blessed
for having had the opportunity to learn from and with you.
To Dr. SueAnn Gruver,
For your mentorship, guidance, and support. Golly I wish you were here at the finish line!
To Andrea,
I know you are looking at my journey from above and saying, you can do this, girl.
Hell yeah, I could. I did it for both of us.
Finally, to my amazing and supportive husband, Scott,
For your love, patience and amazingly good food while I wrote, slept, and sometimes cursed.
I am forever grateful for you.
2
It was books that taught me that the things that tormented me most were the very things that
connected me with all the people who were alive, who had ever been alive.
- James Baldwin
Literacy is a tool of liberation, both personal and cultural.
-Gloria Ladson-Billings
I do believe something very magical happens when you read a book.
- J.K. Rowling
3
ACKNOWLEDGEMENT
A special thanks to Elly Orcutt at the University of Minnesota for her support in answering my
thousand questions.
4
TABLE OF CONTENTS
CHAPTER ONE: THE PROBLEM 14
Introduction to the Research Questions 14
Background of the Problem 18
Statement of the Problem 21
Research Questions 22
Context and Significance of the Research 23
Key Terms 25
Overview of Methodology 27
Limitations 29
Summary 31
CHAPTER TWO: THE LITERATURE REVIEW 32
Introduction and Significance of the Topic 32
Research Questions 34
Theoretical Framework 34
Reading Comprehension Pedagogy and Achievement in the United States 41
Informational Text as a Means to Building Content Knowledge 49
The Importance of Prior Knowledge in Reading Comprehension 51
The Acquisition of Knowledge Through Reading 55
5
The Role of Vocabulary Acquisition on Comprehension of Informational Texts 57
The Effects of Race, Class, and Language on Reading Comprehension and Reading
Achievement 60
The Effects of Socioeconomic Status on Reading Comprehension and Reading
Achievement 63
The Effects of Multilingualism on Reading Comprehension and Reading Achievement 67
New Literacies, Online Reading, and the Changing Nature of Reading Comprehension 72
The Role of Engagement, Motivation, and Attitude in Reading Comprehension 78
The Role of Culturally Relevant Texts on Successful Comprehension 85
Conclusion 89
CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY 91
Introduction 91
Rationale for the Study 92
Research Paradigm 98
Research Setting and Participants 103
Research Setting 103
Interruption to the Research Setting 107
Overall and In-Person Student Participation in Summer Targeted Services 110
Demographics of In-Person Student Participation in Summer Targeted Services 114
6
Human Subject Review 115
Institutional Review Board 115
Study 1: Data Collection with Teachers To Inform Text Creation 116
Teacher Participants in the Research Study 117
Focus Group Materials 119
Natural Language Processing (NLP) Analysis of Texts 120
Focus Group Discussion 123
Study 2: Data Collection with Students To Inform Text Development 125
Student Participants in the Research Study 126
Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Data Collection 128
General Science Knowledge Data Collection 129
Data Collection on Text Reading and Paraphrasing 131
Data Analysis 136
Conclusion 140
CHAPTER FOUR: ANALYSIS AND RESULTS 142
Introduction 142
Study 1: Data Analysis To Inform Text Creation 144
Natural Language Processing (NLP) Analysis of Texts 145
Focus Group Qualitative Data Analysis 147
7
Analysis of the Texts Created and Used 149
Study 2: Student Data Analysis 152
Student Participant Data 155
Student Pre-Assessment Data. 157
Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS) 157
Science Pre-Assessment 163
Student Paraphrase Data 166
Student Engagement in Reading and Paraphrasing 168
Qualitative Observational Data Analysis 169
Quantitative Observational Data Analysis 176
Student Paraphrase Coding 181
Paraphrase Filter 181
Paraphrase Numerical Coding 182
Paraphrase Quality Data 183
Comparison of Paraphrase Data to Text Indices Data 191
Comparison of Paraphrase Data to Pre-Assessment Data 194
Paraphrase Data and Science Pre-Assessment Data 194
Paraphrase Data and Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Data 199
Student Perceptions of Text and Paraphrase Difficulty 203
8
Analysis of Text and Paraphrase Perception Data with Paraphrase Quality Data 212
Qualitative Student Interview Data 217
Conclusion 218
CHAPTER FIVE: DISCUSSION, RECOMMENDATIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS 220
Introduction 220
Discussion of the Findings 223
Essential Characteristics of Informational Texts 224
Motivation and Engagement With Digital Text 228
Student Engagement in Watching, Reading, and Paraphrasing. 228
Texts as Factors in Engagement and Motivation 230
Essential Text Characteristics in a Digital Environment 231
Diversity of Students in Grades 3 and 4 233
Delimitations to the Research Design 240
Limitations to the Research Design 242
Limitations of Study 1 243
Focus Group Work 243
Limitations of Study 2 244
The Summer Targeted Services Program. 244
Comprehension Intervention Research Focus 246
9
Student Participation Due to Absence 248
Researcher and Teacher 249
The Use of This Study in the Creation of the iSTART-Early Interface 240
Recommendations for Future Research 252
Implications for Classroom Practice 257
Concluding Thoughts 261
REFERENCES 264
APPENDICES
Appendix A: Teacher Focus Group Demographic Survey Responses 309
Appendix B: Indices Relevant to Reading Comprehension 310
Appendix C: Summer Targeted Services Text Data 312
Appendix D: Focus Group Initial Questions 313
Appendix E: Science Assessment (Modified from NAEP) 314
Appendix F: Student Observation Form Text Reading and Responding 316
Appendix G: Coding Table for Student Paraphrase Responses 325
Appendix H: De-Identified Student Pre-Assessment Data 326
Appendix I: Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Data 327
Appendix J: Themes from Student Observation 328
Appendix K: Student Text and Paraphrase Perception Data 330
10
Appendix L: Student Interview Constructed Responses 331
LIST OF TABLES
Table 3.1. District Demographics as of June, 2020 103
Table 3.2. Demographics of Third and Fourth Grade Students as of June, 2020 103
Table 3.3. Demographics of Student Participants in Summer Targeted Services 110
Table 3.4. Grade 3 and 4 Students’ Participation in On-Site Programming 111
Table 3.5. Demographics of In-Person Student Participation
in Summer Targeted Services, June 2020 113
Table 3.6. Research Study Student Participant Demographics 125
Table 4.1. Text Titles and Characteristics 145
Table 4.2. Teacher Focus Group Themes 146
Table 4.3. Components of the Coh-Metrix Text Ease and Readability Assessor
(T.E.R.A.) 148
Table 4.4. Mean Scores and Standard Deviation for
Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. Components 150
Table 4.5. De-Identified Student Data 154
Table 4.6. Mean Recreational Reading Attitude Score Data by Gender
and Grade Level 159
Table 4.7. Mean Academic Reading Attitude Score Data by Gender
and Grade Level 160
11
Table 4.8. Mean Total Reading Attitude Score Data by Gender
and Grade Level 161
Table 4.9. Science Pre-Assessment Data 163
Table 4.10. Mean Science Pre-Assessment Data by Gender
and Grade Level 164
Table 4.11. Themes from Student Observations 169
Table 4.12. Engagement Indicators 175
Table 4.13. Engagement Observed in Paraphrasing Activity 177
Table 4.14. Paraphrase Filter Data 181
Table 4.15. Paraphrase Quality Data 182
Table 4.16. Paraphrase Quality Data by Text 182
Table 4.17. Paraphrase Quality Data by Student 185
Table 4.18. Paraphrase Quality Data by Gender 186
Table 4.19. Paraphrase Quality Data by Grade Level 189
Table 4.20. Paraphrase Quality Data for “A Visit to Mars” and “Wildfires 191
Table 4.21. T.E.R.A. Component Scores for “An Visit to Mars” and “Wildfires” 192
Table 4.22. Comparison of Students’ Paraphrase Quality Scores to
Science Pre-Assessment Scores 193
Table 4.23. Paraphrase Data by Science Pre-Assessment Score Percentage 194
Table 4.24. Comparison of Students’ Paraphrase Data to ERAS Scores 198
Table 4.25. Paraphrase Data by Elementary Reading Attitude Survey
Total Reading Percentile 199
12
Table 4.26. Text and Paraphrase Perception Data by Student and
Individual Text 203
Table 4.27. Text and Paraphrase Perception Data by Gender 209
Table 4.28. Comparison of Text and Paraphrase Perception Data to Paraphrase
Quality Data 212
13
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure 2.1. - Reader, Text, and Context 34
Figure 2.2. - Reader, Text, Context and Situation 36
14
CHAPTER ONE: THE PROBLEM
There ain’t no journey what don’t change you some.
-David Mitchell
Introduction to the Research Questions
What is one trait that humans here on Earth solely possess? Humans have very
sophisticated language skills. We are the only animal species on Earth that has a writing system.
We create text - sometimes for our own personal purposes (such as a diary), but mostly to
communicate ideas to others, which they acquire by reading. Therefore, “the writing system
matters” (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018, p. 7). Reading authors’ texts is extremely important:
the amount of reading and the variety of texts in which students engage is a powerful predictor of
their future success (Sparks, Patton, & Murdoch, 2014; Cunningham & Stanovich, 2003;
Herbers, et al., 2012; Israel & Reutzel, 2017).
Comprehension - the ultimate goal of reading - is being able to both understand text and
make new meaning from it. It provides the ability to communicate across time and space and the
ability to create and share ideas in concrete and lasting form. Therefore, scaffolding and support
for comprehension early on is critical to later success as a reader and as a contributing member
of our human society. Strong readers are strong comprehenders; they read more often, and the
amount and the variety of reading in which they engage directly contributes to their academic
achievement skills and their ability to obtain a solid education (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997;
Rapp, van den Broek, McMaster, Kendeou, & Espin, 2007; Sparks, 2014). However, many
students struggle in reading; and every year that goes by in their educational career where they
continue to struggle, a door potentially closes. Poor reading skills can lead to less motivation to
15
read, which may in turn lead to less time reading and less development of comprehension skills
(Cain & Oakhill, 2011; Guthrie, 2015; Troyer, Kim, Hale, Wantchekon, & Armstrong, 2019).
My interest in reading in general and comprehension in particular has been a career-long
passion. Now enjoying the 25th year of my education career, my passion for the power of
literacy is as strong as it was when I began my career. I continue to cringe when I hear students
say “I hate to read,” as reading can literally transform lives. Teaching both elementary school and
middle school, I witnessed firsthand the effects of ineffective instruction and intervention with
regard to reading comprehension, and students completely disengaged in the texts themselves.
Continuing to witness this as a district administrator compelled me to create this study which
focuses on the following question: What are the essential characteristics of informational texts
that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? I further guided this research
by further focusing on the following sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
16
Reading is powerful. It has provided me the ability to travel through time and through
worlds; it has offered me the opportunity to see my current world through others’ eyes. I focused,
when finally becoming a teacher, on how I could get others to love reading as much as I did, and
have been continuously learning throughout my career about literacy in general, and particularly
how people learn to read.
My love of reading is one reason why I wanted to become a teacher. I earned my
bachelor's degree in elementary education in 1995, and spent considerable time learning more
about literacy, as I knew the importance that the skill of reading had in school and outside of
school. In 2000, I earned a masters degree in curriculum and instruction and again focused on
reading, conducting a short but in-depth action research case study on one of my eighth grade
students who was a struggling reader and had been his entire school career. Experiencing the joy
and success of reading myself, I wanted him to improve his skills in reading and to learn to love
it. Ultimately, I wrote my masters thesis on the effects of ability grouping on advanced readers,
as I believed the advanced readers were falling through the cracks, not being afforded the
opportunity to grow as in their achievement as much as they could potentially grow. They
deserved to enrich their life story as much as any other student.
I am an English teacher at heart, and deeply embrace the power of story. The story of my
journey in education - as a student, a teacher, and now as an administrator - reflects my deep
belief in and passion for education; however, my journey has not been a traditional one. When I
was in the classroom, my students often asked me, “If you could go back and do it all over again,
what would you do differently?” It was a great question, one that requires deep and continual
reflection. In 1995, at 32 years of age, with a second-grade daughter, I graduated summa cum
17
laude with a bachelors degree in elementary education after experiencing the pain and shame of
dropping out of college and spending eleven years away from learning. Although my career
started late, all of my life experiences have helped me connect to my students in ways I would
not have been able to otherwise. Being a strong reader certainly provided an avenue for me to hit
the restart button. I was able to change my world for the better.
My story reflects my conviction in strong reading skills as a way to change the world.
Students throughout my thirteen years in the classroom heard my mantra over and over again,
that knowledge is power, and much knowledge comes from reading. Friere (1970) points out that
students need to read words and the world, and teaching students to become literate means that
we must be committed to teaching them the “skills, attitudes, and commitments needed to
become citizens who will work for social justice… in the world” (Banks, 2003, p. 18). As a
teacher and now a district administrator, I have always believed in the power of literacy as a key
factor in the success of my students’ futures and, as Maxwell (2013) asked his readers to reflect
on why s/he is doing a particular study, I thought it was a good exercise to help ground my work,
so I asked it of myself. White (2017) calls out the fact that doctoral students “have a calling they
are answering” (p. 16), and in answering Maxwell’s (2013) question, I realized my calling. I
want to contribute to the body of research on reading comprehension. In order to do this,
educators must look carefully at the texts we are placing in front of our students. Not all texts are
a one-size-fits-all; our students are not a one-size-fits-all; not all tasks and environments are
exactly the same. Furthermore, our students’ lived experiences are not exactly the same; children
have been building their knowledge of language and learning before they ever begin formal
schooling (Luo, Tamis-LeMonda, & Mendelsohn, 2020). We must carefully match texts to the
18
task and the student. This includes the texts of new literacies, such as text messaging and
blogging, and the diverse students from all walks of life who put so much faith, everyday, in our
ability as educators.
At the culmination of this journey, I want to make my mark with something that can
change the trajectory for a child who is struggling with reading comprehension, because that
struggle can lead to a much larger struggle in learning content in other disciplines, and learning
about the world in general. I want that child’s future to be brighter, filled with more promise and
absent of as many obstacles as I can possibly eliminate. This sounds clichè, but it is anything but.
My research will provide the foundation for the development of a reading comprehension
intervention tool aimed to improve the comprehension skills of young readers. This research will
add to the body of research on matching the reader to the text and the task and will provide me
the opportunity to come to a deeper understanding of reading comprehension and the impact of
successful reading across a school career and in life, and how we are - or are not - creating
equitable experiences for students that will provide a brighter future.
Background of the Problem
The act of reading is an interaction between reader, text, and context (Anderson, Hiebert,
Scott, & Wilkinson, 1985; Hartman, Morsink, & Zheng, 2010; Kintsch, 1998; Kintsch, 2005;
Paris & Newman, 2009; Pearson & Cervetti, 2015). Strong acquisition of reading skills early on
leads not only to stronger reading achievement later on in school, but also builds other cognitive
skills, such as an increased vocabulary and content knowledge. That ongoing interaction between
reader, text and context continues to build both skill and knowledge which, in turn, can lead to
19
even greater success - in academics and in life (Pressley & Allington, 2015; Sparks, Patton, &
Murdoch, 2014). Reading is not just the ability to decode words; a person is literate if they can
understand the text they are reading. In our age of information, literacy is an essential skill
needed to find the most accurate information in the shortest amount of time possible in order to
problem-solve and communicate to others across the globe. Literacy is vital to the success of
global societies (Leu, 1997; Leu, et al., 2008). It is the foundation for the acquisition of
knowledge outside of one’s personal sphere of experience, for engagement within and between
cultures, and for a strong global economy. In 2018 alone, according to the World Literacy
Foundation (2018), the effects of illiteracy cost the global economy approximately $1.04 trillion,
and contributed to inequality in basic needs, such as hygiene, health, and safety. Underpinning
these alarming statistics are the names and faces of people living in poverty, incarcerated, or
exploited at least partially as a result of low literacy skills.
Although reading comprehension is vital to a student’s reading skills and a predictor of
future success, in the United States, a large number of students struggle in reading, as is
evidenced by the 2019 National Assessment of Education (NAEP) reading assessment. That
report showed that 65% of fourth graders in the United States scored below proficiency, which is
down from 2017 and statistically insignificant from results a decade ago (National Center for
Education Statistics, 2019). Further, a substantial gap continues to exist for fourth grade students
based on race and income inequality, with only the gap between White and Hispanic fourth
graders decreasing, albeit an insignificant amount (National Center for Education Statistics,
2019). The NAEP assessment, first administered in 1969 in science, writing, and citizenship, was
created to provide data on outputs in education - in other words, student learning - rather than
20
inputs, such as per-pupil expenditures and attendance. It was changed significantly in 1986 to
measure learning in reading, mathematics and science (National Center for Educational Sciences,
2019). The reading assessment is designed to measure students’ reading comprehension skills on
both literary (narrative) and informational texts.
As the goal of reading is to both understand and create new meaning, these statistics are
alarming, yet not new: Cunningham and Stanovich, in 2003, emphasized that the achievement
gap for nearly two-thirds of the nation’s fourth graders will continue to widen as a result of their
weak comprehension skills. These struggles can lead to a lifetime of struggle. According to the
National Center for Education Statistics, in 2019, one in five adults in the United States have low
literacy skills, which translates into approximately 43 million adults. Additionally, advancements
in technology have altered the digital landscape and continue to demand increased effective
reading skills for different texts. This study examined the essential text characteristics that can be
used to explicitly teach comprehension strategies to a diverse population of third and fourth
grade students reading below grade level and what essential text structures are needed for
comprehension strategy instruction in a digital format, thereby providing comparisons between
an offline (traditional, static printed text) format and an online (digital text) format. Additionally,
this study examined what additional considerations are needed when selecting appropriate texts
for a racially, ethnically and socioeconomically diverse population of students within a
classroom in need of reading comprehension intervention.
21
Statement of the Problem
Reading achievement in the United States in general, and in Minnesota specifically, has
remained stagnant over the last decade, with only one-third of fourth graders meeting grade-level
proficiency on assessments designed specifically to analyze reading trends and growth (National
Center for Education Statistics, 2019). This low level of proficiency has tremendous effects not
only on the individual reader, but collectively on global society. Further, classrooms across the
United States continue to become more diverse with regard to race, ethnicity, language, and
socio-economic status.
Technology, as well, continues to advance rapidly, with the Internet and other
technologies continually increasing in literacy worlds (Leu, Kinzer, Coiro, Castek, & Henry,
2013). With this rapidly changing technology come increased demands being made on reading
skills - skills that must be taught to be successful in the digital world of the Internet and
Information and Communication Technologies (ICTs). While studies continue to confirm the
impact of intervention on reading achievement, the what that is placed in front of students - the
texts themselves, and their essential characteristics with specific regard to comprehension
intervention - need deeper research in order to understand the role they play in the interventions.
How a person comprehends static text is not isomorphic with how they comprehend digital text;
thus we cannot utilize our same understanding of offline reading comprehension (static or printed
texts) with online reading comprehension (Dalton & Proctor, 2008; Leu, et al., 2011; Coiro,
2020). Therefore, the format of those texts placed in front of students for reading comprehension
22
intervention, especially with regard to an interactive, digital format versus a traditionally static
print format, is extremely important to research.
Research Questions
Due to the struggles that many students continue to face with regard to reading
comprehension, a better understanding of successful reading comprehension intervention is
needed. The purpose of this research is to identify essential text characteristics for texts that will
eventually be included in an online, automated reading strategy tutor. This tool will provide
instruction and practice designed to improve deep comprehension for a diverse population of
struggling third and fourth grade students, a set of grade levels that are a critical, yet
under-supported time of reading development. This research will guide the development of an
age-appropriate text library for informational text that is appropriate and interesting for students
in grades three and four. To that end, this mixed-methods study explores reading comprehension
and the role that texts and new literacies play in answering the central research question:
What are the essential characteristics of informational texts that can be used for
training reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students?
This research is further guided by the following two sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
23
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
Context and Significance of the Research
The term diectic refers to a word, phrase or expression for which the meaning is
dependent upon the context in which it is used. While not a new linguistic term, it takes on new
force as digital tools and new technologies help to cement literacy as deictic in nature. “ . . . the
definition of literacy has expanded from traditional notions of reading and writing to include the
ability to learn, comprehend, and interact with technology in a meaningful way” (Selfe cited in
Coiro, 2003, p. 458).
Toward that end, my research serves as a foundational component of a larger research
study to develop an online reading strategy tutor that will help improve the reading
comprehension of students in third and fourth grade. This is a critical period of reading
development, as students are being presented with texts from which they have to learn, but have
content with which they are unfamiliar, which could lead to comprehension plateaus (Castles,
Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Goldman, Snow, & Vaughn, 2016; Guthrie, et al., 2004; Sweet & Snow,
2003). This online tool will support the strategies of comprehension monitoring, paraphrasing,
inferencing, question asking, explanation, and summarization. This tool is aimed to improve the
reading comprehension ability of students by providing individualized comprehension
instruction in a digital format during times of independent practice, as the findings from the
National Reading Panel (2000) show that the benefits of comprehension strategy instruction
24
begin to be shown after limited instructional time and, as Willingham (2006) explains, once the
strategy, or “trick” as he calls them (p. 39) is introduced and explained, students begin to use
them and discover they can be used in other situations (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018;
Willingham, 2006). The online tutoring tool, once created, will allow students to practice what
they have learned, receiving both formative and summative feedback that is generated
automatically and immediately, which itself is part of the digital reading experience.
The tool will provide solid benefits for teachers as well. It will be easy to use and
adaptable to their curriculum - teachers will be able to load specific texts for individual students
and monitor their performance, providing more support as needed in a blended-learning format.
In addition, teachers will continue to hone their educational technology skills and knowledge.
Finally, the tool will advance the field of education’s understanding of how students learn
to comprehend, especially in a multimodal, digital format. The tool will leverage student
engagement in somewhat of a game-based format, which is an emerging field of understanding
for education (Squire, 2008; Gee, 2003; Steinkuehler, 2008), and will further inform teacher
practice, as it will provide the opportunity for teachers to choose texts that are reflective of the
individual student. This will increase student motivation as well.
Digital tools, including the vast wealth of texts easily available on the Internet, provide
the reader with a multitude of information to process in those meaningful ways - sometimes
simultaneously. For example, Strømsø, Bråten, and Samuelstuen (2003) showed that university
students who were more successful at course-end used reading strategies that helped them make
more connections across texts. The students’ skills were a culmination of a dozen or more years
of literacy instruction, practice, and assessment. In everyday life, as well, people of all ages seek
25
information digitally to answer questions and make sense of information. One can neither ignore
the changing face of literacy nor can we ignore those who struggle with their literacy skills. For
those students who struggle in reading, intervention on comprehension must be inclusive of the
materials with which a diverse body of students will interact on a daily basis, and must continue
to reflect how those texts, and those students, will continue to change over time. The texts matter.
Using the deictic terms today and tomorrow, increasing a student’s literacy skills today
leads to increased success tomorrow. But each today leads to a different tomorrow. As educators,
we know that what we do today influences how a student learns - and what success that student
will enjoy - tomorrow.
Key Terms
The following terms will provide clarity for the context presented throughout this study.
They are listed in alphabetical order.
Comprehension. Comprehension is defined as “the process of simultaneously extracting
and constructing meaning through interaction and involvement with written language” (Rand
Study Group, 2002, p. 11). Toward this end, a reader must decode, identify the words, and use
prior knowledge and experience to make meaning. Comprehension is constructive in nature.
New literacies. The term new literacies for the purpose of this study is defined as both
texts on the Internet and texts used as part of Information and Communication Technologies
(ICTs), and the skill needed to comprehend such texts (Leu, et al., 2011). While “new” is a
relative term, and “new literacies” was coined in or around 2008, texts today are not only
multimodal, they are deictic; therefore, the contexts continually change.
26
Literacy. The term literacy for the purpose of this study is defined as the ability to
identify, understand, interpret, create, communicate and compute, using printed and written
materials associated with varying contexts. Literacy involves a continuum of learning in enabling
individuals to achieve their goals, to develop their knowledge and potential, and to participate
fully in their community and wider society. Further, literacy is plural, being practiced in
particular contexts for particular purposes and using specific languages (United Nations
Educational, Scientific, and Cultural Organization, 2018).
Offline text. The term offline text refers to informational text that is printed and not in a
digital format. Such texts include trade books, magazines, newspapers, and textbooks (Coiro,
2011a).
Skill. The term skill has been used differently throughout time and across different
disciplines,including psychology and education, which has led to a lot of confusion both in
research and practice (Almasi & Fullerton, 2012; Afflerbach, Pearson, & Paris, 2008). Reading
skills are fluid and automatic, and are defined as “automatic actions that result in decoding and
comprehension with speed, efficiency, and fluency and usually occur without awareness of the
components or control involved” (Afflerbach, Pearson, & Paris, 2008, p. 368).
Strategy. The term strategy, like the term skill, has been inconsistently used in schools,
which can lead to confusion for students, teachers, and families (Afflerbach, Pearson, & Paris,
2008). A strategy is defined as “a deliberate, goal-directed attempt to control and modify the
readers efforts to decode text, understand words, and construct meanings of texts” (Afflerbach,
Pearson, & Paris, 2008, p. 368). We use strategies deliberately when we want to achieve a
27
particular goal, and in this case, the goal is comprehension of a text. A strategy differs from a
skill in that it is deliberate, rather than automatic.
The terms listed above are used universally throughout Chapters Two through Five, and it
is important to build clarity of these terms in the research. Other terms used in this study will be
defined in context as they arise in the discussion of the methodology employed, the analyses of
the data, limitations and recommendations for further study. The following section provides a
brief overview of the methodology utilized for this study.
Overview of Methodology
This research study focused on texts used in a digital comprehension intervention for
third and fourth- grade students in a small, urban Midwest public school district who are reading
below grade level and were identified by elementary school and district staff for additional
comprehension support. The student demographics in this district are quite diverse.
Approximately 40% of the elementary student body identifies as Latinx; approximately 30% of
the student body identifies as White, approximately 13% of the student body identifies as Black
Non-Hispanic; 10.4% identifies as two or more races, and the remainder of the student body
identifies as Asian, Native American, or Hawaiian/Island Pacific. The focus on third and fourth
grade students was deliberate as this is a critical time of reading comprehension development,
yet comprehension has historically been undersupported (Sparks, et al., 2014). Further, the
research was done during summer targeted services, which allowed for flexibility with regard to
time and materials for students.
28
This study used both Design-based implementation research (DBIR) and Mixed Methods
research. DBIR includes the active involvement of those who will ultimately be implementing
the strategy intervention which, for the purpose of this study, are both the teachers and their
students. The study used mixed-methods research as a natural continuation of DBIR. There were
two iterative cycles, the number determined based on previous findings in the qualitative
research focusing on teachers. The qualitative research employed a teacher/literacy expert focus
group, facilitated by the researcher, whose members worked on identifying the core parameters
for informational text development and selection. This group looked at informational texts
created to match science standards, and provided feedback on their appropriate use with third
and fourth grade students. The second part of the qualitative research focused on student
observation with texts and a short interview with all students who participated in the research.
The quantitative research was two-phased.
In the first phase, a focus group of teachers/literacy specialists was convened to focus on
parameters for informational text development and selection. The group helped to formulate the
parameters that encapsulated the essential characteristics of informational texts. This included,
but was not limited to, such characteristics as content, themes, the interests of the students, and
the developmental appropriateness of such texts with regard to third and fourth grade readers.
The feedback was both qualitative and quantifiable with regard to appropriate use.
The second phase involved a quasi-experiment where the texts were evaluated for
appropriateness by students in grades 3 and 4. Students were asked to read subsets of the texts
developed and engaged in paraphrasing, a strategy that has shown to be an effective
comprehension strategy (Hagaman & Casey, 2016; Hagaman, Casey, & Reid, 2016; Hagaman,
29
Casey, & Reid, 2012; Kletzien, 2009). Raters scored the level of the students’ responses. The
researcher observed the students as they were interacting with the texts and typing their
paraphrases. After each text, students were to rate the text on interest and ease, and to rate the
paraphrasing activity on interest and ease. Finally, the researcher interviewed the students to
ascertain whether they could remember how to paraphrase and if they thought paraphrasing
helped them become a better reader. As with any research study, there were limitations, which
will be outlined in the next section.
Limitations
The spring and summer of 2020 was unlike any other in recent history. The United States
found itself in the middle of the coronavirus pandemic, and public schools across the nation had
to quickly shift to remote, or distance, learning. This shift in Minnesota took place in early
March of 2020. The district in which this study took place began distance learning on April 5,
2020, the Monday after its spring break. The pandemic continued, and schools were unable to
open for the rest of the school year. However, districts had a bit more leeway with regard to their
summer targeted services structure, albeit adhering to strict social distancing and state mask
policies.
The district in which this study took place revised its plans for summer targeted services
programming. Many families in the district had limited access to digital technology; many
families had unreliable access to the Internet access and many had no access to the Internet
during the time of the full distance learning. Further, elementary students had a hard time
engaging with on-screen learning even if access was not an issue. Therefore, families of
30
elementary students recommended for summer targeted services were given the option to send
their students in person to school, or participate in summer programming remotely (distance
learning). While much more equitable, this choice limited the number of students who would
participate in this study; 121 of the 255 elementary students registered for summer targeted
services, or 47.5%, opted for in-person learning, which is less than half of the total elementary
students enrolled. From that pool of 121 students, 17 dropped; either they never attended or
stopped attending and were dropped from the roster once the summer targeted services program
began.
This study focused on students in grades 3 and 4. Summer targeted services enrollment
for these grades was as follows:
Grade 3: of the 33 total students registered, four dropped, and 14 of 29 remaining
participated on site, for a 48.3% on-site participation
Grade 4: of the 55 total students registered, 11 dropped, and 18 of 44 remaining
participated on site, for a 40.9% on-site participation
Shulman (1999) states, “Research on educational practice is inherently consequential. It
is often cross-cultural, carried on when the social class, language, ethnicity, or commitments of
those conducting the research are incongruent with those whom they study” (p. 165). I, as a
White, middle class, native English-speaking educator may not look like my students, or have
the same home language as them, or many of the same life experiences as them. I recognize that
and am committed to changing the trajectory of the narrative.
31
Summary
Because the act of reading is an interaction between reader, text, and context (Anderson,
Hiebert, Scott, & Wilkinson, 1985; Hartman, Morsink, & Zheng, 2010; Kintsch, 1998; Kintsch,
2005; Paris & Newman, 2009; Pearson & Cervetti, 2015), it was important for me as a researcher
to observe students interacting with the text. A data point (such as a score on an assessment) is
one piece of evidence; a better understanding of the origin of that data point comes from the
students themselves. More engaged readers are more motivated, have higher reading
self-efficacy, and tend to utilize strategies to continue comprehending the text (Castles, Rastle, &
Nation, 2018; Massey & Miller, 2017; Wigfield, et al., 2008; Willingham, 2017). Further, it is
important to understand the interactions and experiences a diverse body of students have with
texts, in order to provide the best texts possible in a reading comprehension intervention. The
role of engagement and motivation is found to be a greater problem for students of color, for
whom dropout rates are the highest (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Rumberger, 1987;
Rumberger, 1995). The texts, indeed, matter.
Chapter Two further explores the literature on reading comprehension, the changing face
of literacy with regard to online texts, and the engagement of diverse populations of students in
the act of making and creating meaning through reading.
32
CHAPTER TWO: THE LITERATURE REVIEW
You don’t have to burn books to destroy a culture. Just get people to stop reading them.
- Ray Bradbury
Introduction and Significance of the Topic
The ability to comprehend at high levels continues to be considered an extremely
important skill, both in school and throughout one’s life. It provides the opportunity for social
empowerment, the creation of more equitable aspects of learning, community, work, and
personal life, leading to a better life for everyone, especially those who are the least advantaged
(Coiro, 2020; Leu, et al, 2015). Therefore, it is imperative that we raise the reading achievement
of every student. For the purposes of this study, reading comprehension is defined using the
RAND Reading Group Study (2002) definition: “the process of simultaneously extracting and
constructing meaning through interaction and involvement with written language” (p. 11).
However, the scope of what is read has been shifting from solely on traditional paper pages
(offline) to both offline and digital formats, whether online or on an e-reader or other electronic
device (Goldman, Braasch, Wiley, Graesser, & Brodowinska, 2012; Hartman, Morsink & Zheng,
2010; H. J. Kaiser Family Foundation, 2010). Both formats must be included in a
comprehensive, evidence-based system of early intervention designed to ensure struggling
readers read and comprehend at levels commensurate with their successful grade level peers, and
that all readers, regardless of ability, continue to grow their reading prowess.
33
This review of the literature explored what is known about the complex skill of reading
comprehension and how that knowledge has continued to expand over the years. Specifically, it
explored the interaction of student, text, context, and environment, as every reading experience
uniquely involves a person, a text, and a purpose. The text itself, and its characteristics, is the
main factor we as researchers and educators have control over; it is our responsibility to ensure
every text placed in front of a student optimizes the interaction they have with that text to
improve reading comprehension. To that end, this literature review focused on the role of
informational text in successful reading comprehension with regard to building knowledge. Next,
the role that culture, socioeconomic status, and language play in reading achievement in the
elementary grades was examined as it intersects with successful reading comprehension and
reading achievement. Further, this review explored the literature on how students interact with
texts in a digital format and what that interaction means for students who struggle in reading.
With a focus on intervention for struggling third and fourth grade readers, an identified critical
time in the acquisition of reading skills (Feister, 2010; Goldman, Snow, & Vaughn, 2016; Sweet
& Snow, 2003), this review next explored new literacies and their implication on the changing
nature of reading comprehension, including new skills and strategies that are needed to read
successfully in a digital world. The chapter closes with a review of literature that explored the
role that motivation and engagement have with regard to reading comprehension in the
elementary grades.
34
Research Questions
The main research question of this study focused on identifying and optimizing the text
factor in the reader-text-context interaction: What are the essential characteristics of
informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? As the
nature of texts continually change in a digital world, the question was further guided by the
following sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion, topics) for
the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching reading comprehension
strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation, summarization, and question asking in a digital
format?
Because comprehension is a process that is an interaction between reader, text, and
context, the research question was also guided by the following second sub-question:
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a racially and
ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students, and/or (b) students who read
below grade level?
Theoretical Framework
Researchers, policy makers, and educators have spent the better part of six decades trying
to improve the reading achievement of students in the United States, during both literacy-specific
instructional time at the elementary level and during instruction in specific content areas at the
35
secondary level. There have been major shifts in thinking about reading comprehension,
especially in the last 55 years, but overall there have been three major factors considered
throughout all of it: the reader, the text, and context (Fox & Alexander, 2017; Pardo, 2004;
Pearson & Cervetti, 2015; RAND Reading Study Group, 2002). This interaction of those three
factors, and the resulting reading comprehension, is illustrated in Figure 2.1. Throughout the 55
years that were explored for the purposes of this study, different periods of time had a different
major focus on one of those factors as the most important factor accounting for successful
comprehension, and thus different theoretical models were created.
Figure 2.1
Reader, Text, and Context
In the 1950s and 1960s, models of reading comprehension were more text-centric; in the
1970s, the models tended to be more reader-focused. The decade of the 1980s brought with it
arguments between those who wanted the emphasis to be on bottom-up processes (decoding),
36
and top-down processes (language comprehension), giving rise to the ‘reading wars’ - phonics
versus whole language instruction. The simple view of reading (SVR) provided some clarity
about these two core components and their interrelation. Gough and Tumner (1986) illustrated
SVR with a mathematical equation; the SVR posits that reading (R) equals the product of
decoding (D) and comprehension (C), or R=DxC. The simple view of reading can be useful in
understanding that in order to become a skilled reading comprehender, neither decoding nor
linguistic comprehension (which differs from reading comprehension in that it encompasses
listening and language comprehension) is sufficient by itself. It takes both the skill of decoding
and the skill of language comprehension to become a skilled comprehender of written text.
However, reading (R) is much more complex than the simple view can represent. Reading
comprehension is the interplay of the cognitive processes of the reader on the text itself,
including the text features, the background knowledge of the reader, and the goals and purposes
for reading a particular text (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018). As a result, the late 1980s and
beyond brought a greater focus on context (Pearson & Cervetti, 2015).
More recently, the element of context has been expanded to include the situation, which
is more of a sociocultural element and includes time, place and purpose of the act of reading,
which could include the stated or implied task for reading (Fox & Alexander, 2017; Hartman,
Morskin, & Zheng, 2010). This updated interaction can be seen in Figure 2.2. The characteristics
of all of these elements (text, reader, context, and situation) all interact, thus affecting the process
of comprehension. Important characteristics of the reader can include motivation, mood,
self-efficacy with reading skills, and content knowledge. Characteristics of the text could include
complexity, the content of the text, the genre of the text, even the font. Characteristics of context
37
could include the activity or task, such as finding evidence to support a main idea or evaluating
an argument, or a nurturing reading environment. Finally, characteristics of situation as part of
context could include a high-stakes testing situation, reading for pleasure, working with a
partner, interrogating the text for absent narratives, engaging in game-based literacy, or making
use of background knowledge to build understanding. Reading comprehension, as Castles,
Rastle, and Nation (2018) illustrate, is multi-dimensional, multi-faceted, and encompasses a
range of processes which are dependent upon many variables (p. 8).
Figure 2.2
Reader, Text, Context, and Situation
The theoretical framework for this study is guided by the construction-integration (C-I;
Kinstch & van Dijk, 1978; Kintsch, 1983; Snow, 2002) model of comprehension that provides a
more individualized understanding of reading comprehension, such as variance by age, skill,
38
format, and life experience (Fox & Alexander, 2017; Israel & Reutzel, 2017; Kintsch, 1998;
Paris & Hamilton, 2005). The C-I model provides a more balanced view of reader and text in
comparison with earlier models of reading comprehension, and also provides an extremely
important focus on context, which can be thought of as “a filter through which people perceive
the world” (Kintsch, 1988, p. 1). Further, as Pearson and Cervetti (2015) pointed out, this model
illustrates the current dominant model of comprehension in the literature today.
The C-I model views readers as active meaning-makers who are creating “mental
representations of texts” (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Pearson & Cervetti, 2015; van den
Broek, 2010). To comprehend a text does not necessarily mean that a person regurgitates
verbatim what was read; the meaning is created by forming what is known as a situation model
(Kintsch, 1998) which continues to grow as the reader continues to read, and culminates with a
rich understanding of the text that goes beyond simply what was actually stated in the text
(Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Pearson & Cervetti, 2015). Simply put, the reader is making
their individualized meaning from the text, based on the actual words in the text but also using
the readers own background knowledge or prior experiences, including but not limited to rules
of grammar, knowledge of events, specific vocabulary, past experience, even emotions. This can
explain why, even though a reader may have loathed Cold Mountain (1997), the novel won the
Pulitzer Prize for Literature.
The construction-integration model uses both the bottom-up processes, which begin with
foundational skills needed for decoding (Angosto, Sánchez, Álvarez, Cuevas, & León, 2013),
and top-down processes, which involve larger chunks of text, prior knowledge, and memory,
guiding comprehension. Both are an integral part of comprehension, which is highly interactive
39
(Kintsch, 2005). Further, this model is used to explain how a reader tackles text: If the reader is a
fluent reader of the particular text, fewer strategies are needed. The reader is actively
constructing meaning, using the words on the page and their prior knowledge through such
critical skills as inference. However, even proficient adult readers come to words they do not
know in their own reading, or read a passage that is difficult and does not make sense, but as
Goodman, Goodman, and Allen (2017) point out, “proficient readers are more flexible in their
use of language knowledge and reading strategies than are less proficient readers” (p. 89). At the
point of struggle is when adept readers utilize strategies, such as re-reading. Kintsch (2005)
states, “only when the normal flow of comprehension breaks down does strategic problem
solving take over” (p. 126). Therefore, both skills and strategies are needed for successful
comprehension.
Because the landscape of reading and the resulting reading comprehension skills required
to traverse that landscape continue to evolve with continually changing technologies, the
construction-integration model of reading comprehension allows for the rapidly changing
construct of new literacies and the theories surrounding it. The construction-integration model of
reading comprehension values the individual meaning-making based on the format of the text,
the environment in which the text is being read, the purpose for reading the text and the readers
prior knowledge and experiences. Therefore, the C-I model can embrace the new literacies that
are part and parcel of our lives and help researchers and practitioners recognize that changes to
literacy are taking place at many levels (Leu, et al., 2013) and that we must prepare our students
through sound pedagogy. By understanding that comprehension is personal and individual, and
understanding that new literacies allow for readers to forge their own paths through various
40
linked texts, the construction-integration model of reading comprehension provides the space
needed for exploration of this new horizon of reader, text, context, and situation within a digital
world of reading intervention.
This review of literature began with research on reading comprehension instruction in the
United States, and discusses the resulting elementary student achievement in reading. Literacy,
specifically the component of reading under the literacy umbrella, continues to evolve as the
context and situation in which one reads continues to evolve (Hartman, et al., 2010; Leu, et al.,
2015; Leu, et al., 2013). This review of literature, therefore, explored several important facets
with regard to context and situation. First, research on reading as building content knowledge
was explored, especially as it relates to the intermediate grades in elementary school, when
students are increasingly exposed to expository texts and expected to comprehend them
successfully as a way to learn new content. Next, a deeper understanding of the similarities and
differences of online versus offline reading comprehension was warranted and further explored.
Additionally, this review of the literature focused on the role that motivation to read and
engagement of the reader play in successful reading comprehension, with regard to the content
and the format of the texts.
Finally, this literature review explored the additional considerations of culture and when
choosing texts for a diverse group of readers, especially those who struggle in reading for various
reasons, and whose achievement has led to an achievement gap in reading between them and
their majority group, grade-level peers. Before discussing the review of literature focusing on
context and situation, however, a review of the literature on comprehension pedagogy and
41
reading achievement in the United States provided foundational context for why this research
study took place.
Reading Comprehension Pedagogy and Achievement in the United States
Historically, reading comprehension had been framed as an interaction between reader
and text (Anderson, Hiebert, Scott, & Wilkinson, 1985; Hartman, Morsink, & Zheng, 2010;
Pearson & Cervetti, 2015), and also the interaction between the reader, the text that has been
written, and what is happening at that moment in time (Almasi & Fullerton, 2012; Butcher &
Kintsch, 2003; Pardo, 2004; Pearson & Cervetti, 2015; Snow, 2002). Hence, reading
comprehension is an extremely complex process, which can be mostly indirectly observed
(Johnston, 1984; Pearson & Cervetti, 2017). Continued creation of new technologies, however,
have forwarded the progress of understanding of reading comprehension through such new
technologies as eye movement photography, which are beginning to advance the opportunities
for real-time observation of the human mind (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Israel & Reutzel,
2017; Shotter & Raynor, 2015). And, outside of those new technologies and infancy of new
understandings of how the mind works, there have historically been many definitions of
comprehension in the United States, and many periods of time in which the definition has had
little consensus (Paris & Hamilton; 2009). These varying definitions and periods lacking
consensus have affected the body of research on reading comprehension, leading to
wide-reaching implications, both for reading comprehension instruction and assessment, and the
creation of federal literacy policies in the United States (Kapinus & Long, 2015; Pearson, 2007;
Sarroub & Pearson, 1998). Those federal policies, in turn, have had far-reaching ramifications on
42
research on education in general, and reading comprehension in particular which, again, affect
reading comprehension instruction and assessment (Buly & Valencia, 2003; Durkin, 1978).
The review of the literature for this study constructed a synthesis of the past 55 years of
reading comprehension instruction. It focused on literacy reform, beginning with the Elementary
and Secondary Education Act (ESEA) of 1965, providing a contemporary focus on where we are
in the United States with regard to our students’ ability to comprehend visual/written texts. This
55-year period witnessed serious shifts in the way educators and researchers think about
processes and implications of reading comprehension for both educational and policy purposes.
These implications have had a strong impact on districts, schools, teachers and students (Pearson
& Cervetti, 2015) in such areas as assessment, intervention, evaluation, and even funding.
The Elementary and Secondary Education Act created national policy which shaped how
students in high poverty settings, and those whose first language was not English, were to be
educated (Kapinus & Long, 2015; Spring, 2017). This policy led to a focus on assessment. The
first National Assessment of Education Progress (NAEP), also known as The Nation’s Report
Card, which was created in 1968, analyzed trend data in order to address the needs of students
who were not achieving at adequate levels in schools as they existed (Lagemann, 2000). The
NAEP framework utilizes both narrative and informational texts in its assessment of reading
comprehension, recognizing that while some reading behaviors are text-agnostic, other reading
behaviors can vary based on the text with which the individual student interacts (National
Assessment Governing Board, 2019). The NAEP data showed that, from 1971 to 1999, average
reading scores of 9-year olds and 13-year olds grew only four points, from 208 to 212 and from
255 to 259, respectively; and average reading scores of 17-year olds grew three points, from 285
43
to 288 (National Center for Education Statistics, 2015). The NAEP framework for reading has
evolved over the years. The current framework used is from 2009; although there was an
opportunity for a new framework to be developed in 2019, one is just now being created. The
nation continues to use the 2009 framework, although students have been accessing that
assessment on an online platform since 2017 (National Assessment Governing Board, 2019).
Prior to the 1970s, the term literacy was not used in the formal, academic way it is today.
Lankshear and Knobel (2011) stated that reading, at that time, was a vehicle to get to the more
important “business of school learning” (p. 3), while the term literacy was relegated to informal
teaching of reading to more marginalized populations of adults who were illiterate as a result of
what was then termed “debilitating or dysfunctional conditions and circumstances” (p. 3).
Furthermore, as with the term comprehension, literacy has had varying agreement on a definition
and continually changes (Hillerich, 1976; Turnbill, 2002). For the purposes of this study, literacy
is defined as “the ability to identify, understand, interpret, create, communicate and compute,
using printed and written materials associated with varying contexts. Literacy involves a
continuum of learning in enabling individuals to achieve their goals, to develop their knowledge
and potential, and to participate fully in their community and wider society” (UNESCO, 2018).
In other words, a literate person uses reading, writing, speaking, listening, and thinking in order
to make sense of, learn, and create new meaning. Reading comprehension is a critically essential
component of literacy and, as such, it is imperative that research informs strong and explicit
teaching of reading comprehension.
Dolores Durkin, in 1978, published an iconic study focused on reading comprehension
instruction. She undertook this study because, in her observations of elementary school
44
instruction, she found almost no comprehension instruction taking place. She was compelled to
begin this study as a result of the National Institute of Education (NIE) completing a Request for
Proposal (RFP) outlining the need for a “Center for the Study of Reading whose central concern
would be comprehension” (Durkin, 1978, p. 483), outling an assumption that comprehension
instruction was, in fact, taking place. Durkin’s purpose for the study was to ascertain whether
elementary classrooms were providing comprehension instruction and, if so, how much time was
being allocated to that instruction, especially in the middle and upper elementary grades. Her
study revealed little attention to vocabulary and analysis of text structure, and an overwhelming
reliance on workbooks and assignment sheets both during the reading block and social studies
instructional time. In fourth grade, in particular, Durkin found that a fourth grade teacher was
“clearly an assignment giver, not an instructor” (Durkin, 1978, p. 505). Further, with regard to
informational text found in social studies and science content, “teaching children to be better
readers of content subject textbooks never entered into any of the observed activities” (Durkin,
1978, p. 520). Durkin recommended more “observational studies” and identification of practices
that lead to students becoming better readers.
Yet, reading scores continued to stay flat, as the NAEP trend scores illustrate. Due in part
to the relatively stagnant scores, Congress convened the National Reading Panel in 1997, and the
Panel submitted its iconic report, Teaching Children to Read: An Evidence-Based Assessment of
the Scientific Research Literature on Reading and its Implications for Reading Instruction to
Congress in 1999 (National Reading Panel & National Institute of Child Health and Human
Development, 2000). Neither the Panel itself nor the report were without their critics, but both
reported positive results for five of six “strategies” studied (Yatvin, 2003). Those strategies,
45
while never being identified as the essential strategies in the report, subsequently became known
as the Five Pillars of Reading. They are: phonemic awareness, phonics, fluency, comprehension,
and vocabulary, with comprehension being identified as the “essence” of reading (p. NRP, 2000,
p. 228).
Comprehension is identified as a critical and multidimensional component of learning in
general, and a strong predictor of later learning success because learning, especially in a school
setting, is largely derived from what students read (Cunningham & Stanovich, 2003; Herbers, et
al., 2012, Israel & Reutzel, 2017; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Sparks, Patton & Murdoch, 2014).
However, despite the critical nature of strong reading comprehension skills, elementary readers
have historically acquired active comprehension strategies more informally (NRP, 2000, p. 232).
Early literacy instruction, in kindergarten and grade one especially, is mostly focused on the first
two pillars: phonemic awareness and phonics (Buly & Valencia, 2003; Duke, 2000; Durkin,
1978; Paris & Hamilton, 2009). The skills that encompass these two pillars precede the ability to
decode, which is a skill necessary for comprehension of written text, and many researchers have
focused their studies on the necessity and importance of decoding skills (for reviews, Castles,
Rastle, & Nation, 2020; Snowling & Hulme, 2005; Storch & Whitehurst, 2002).
Systematic phonics instruction, especially when implemented early (prior to first grade),
can improve decoding, spelling, and comprehension (Castles, Rastle & Nation, 2020). However,
while necessary, decoding is not the single skill needed for reading comprehension (Nation,
2005; Paris & Hamilton, 2009). Comprehension skills are essential for the creation of meaning
from text, and both decoding skills and comprehension skills contribute significantly to reading
comprehension performance (Kendeou, et al., 2009; Shanahan & Shanahan, 2008; Storch &
46
Whitehurst, 2002). Buly and Valencia outlined in their 2003 paper cautions and considerations
for state policy, and emphasized that the high-stakes tests utilized by states for educational
accountability purposes focus on comprehension, yet the stagnant achievement results suggest
“there is little data to suggest that working with early readers primarily on decoding will lead to
increased reading performance of comprehension when those readers are tested in the 4th grade”
(p. 6). Williams (2015) pointed out that the instructional activities teachers take advantage of in
teaching phonemic awareness and phonics involve little emphasis on comprehension, especially
with and of informational and expository texts.
Yet, even in the primary grades, teachers can make use of rich texts and model think-
alouds and other instructional strategies to begin building the background knowledge - in
comprehension skills and strategies as well as in general information knowledge - that students
will learn more fully as they matriculate through elementary school. Students become more
cognitively developmentally ready in third and fourth grade to improve such higher-order skills
as the use of comprehension strategies (Del Giudice, 2014; Del Giudice, 2018; Pearson &
Billman, 2016). Furthermore, expository and informational texts are replete with “timeless verb
constructions...generic noun constructions…[certain] text structures...and graphical elements,
such as diagrams” (Duke, 2000, p. 205) - all essential text components that, when learned to
navigate successfully, lead to strong reading comprehension later because, as McNamara, Ozuru,
and Floyd (2011) emphasize, using knowledge to comprehend depends in part on the text genre
and text features. The types of texts chosen for skill instruction are important; the youngest
readers can be immersed in a wide variety of both narrative and informative texts which, when
used to teach foundational reading skills, can also tap into the multidimensional processes of
47
reading comprehension needed to understand written information. Additionally, use of such texts
will provide much needed background knowledge as students get older.
Beginning in fourth grade, the use of informational texts increases as students are
expected to use these texts in order to learn in content areas such as science and social studies
(Jeong, Gaffney, & Choi, 2010; Ness, 2011). Yet these texts may present unfamiliar information
in complex structures, including language and vocabulary. Additionally, these texts often require
more demanding processing skills because of their increasingly complex structure, and they
require more complex strategies in order to comprehend, such as inference, summarization,
analysis, and metacognition, which is the evaluation of one’s understanding of what was read
(McNamara, Ozuru, & Floyd, 2011; Santoro, Baker, Fien, Smith, & Chard, 2016). Furthermore,
the issue may be exacerbated as students matriculate through the grade levels. Greenleaf and
Valencia (2016) discovered that “the vast majority of high school students in the diverse, urban
settings where [they] worked had problems with comprehension, not decoding, of the texts they
might encounter in school” (p. 237). Their study illustrated that students were woefully
inexperienced with the more complex text types and structures, and had inadequate prior
knowledge needed to comprehend those texts. Yet students, even those as young as first grade,
can learn to read while they read to learn, utilizing reading comprehension strategies, which are
higher-order processes and skills, to aid word reading skills such as decoding, which are
lower-order processes and skills (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Landi, 2009; Perfetti & Hart,
2001). Sound explicit reading or meaning-making instruction, such as posing questions; teaching
prediction, inference and paraphrasing skills; and synthesizing as students read a text, may help
students build meaning and promote their word skills (McKeown, Beck, & Blake, 2009; Pearson
48
& Billman, 2016). Doing so at an early age will help students, who are interacting with
increasingly complex text as they matriculate through the grades, be successful in their learning.
This study was conducted in a very diverse school district in suburban Minnesota;
therefore, an analysis of students’ reading achievement in Minnesota specifically was warranted.
Minnesota historically enjoyed a higher than average achievement on the NAEP assessment in
comparison to the nation. However, Minnesota has also had one of the largest achievement gaps
in the nation. Therefore, for the purposes of this study, which was situated in the state of
Minnesota, an analysis was warranted of recent NAEP results for the state. In looking at the 2019
achievement results of fourth graders for Minnesota specifically, 39% of students achieved
proficiency or above in fourth grade reading, which is not statistically significant from the
nation’s average of 35%, and the scores are slightly down from 2017 (National Center for
Education Statistics, 2019). Although the Minnesota Department of Education believes in
“striving for excellence, equity, and opportunity” (Minnesota Department of Education, 2020),
Minnesota ranked 12th in fourth grade reading achievement in 2019 which, while being a
relatively consistent ranking overall for the state since 2003, is well below its ranking of fifth in
2002 (National Center for Education Statistics, 2019). Therefore, the challenges with reading
comprehension that have plagued our schools nationwide are the same challenges that have
plagued Minnesota for some time.
Currently, all state, national and international assessments employ online tools for
assessing offline literacy skills - those skills traditionally used by students when reading print
material. The digital age in which we interact, and the increased ability to access information
anywhere, anytime, has strong implications for knowledge acquisition in general, and reading
49
comprehension skills specifically, due to myriad opportunities for interacting with new and
diverse text formats and information that one may not have previously encountered. While new
learning opportunities are beneficial, they also present new challenges (Coiro, 2020; Coiro,
2011a; Goldman, Snow, & Vaughn, 2016). Students must not only be able to adeptly navigate
new text formats, they must also continually build their knowledge of increasingly complex
information and content, which occurs with successful comprehension of any text, no matter the
format.
Informational Text as a Means to Building Content Knowledge
This study focuses on comprehension as it relates to informational text through the
answering of the following questions:
What are the essential characteristics of informational texts that can be used for
training reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students?
The research is further guided by the following two sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
50
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
It was imperative, therefore, to review the body of literature on informational text as a means to
build both content and world knowledge, to understand the importance of the role of prior
knowledge, and to both understand the role vocabulary plays in successful reading
comprehension.
By the time students are in third and fourth grade, they are at an extremely important
stage in their reading development, as the proportion of informational text to narrative text has
sharply increased from the primary grades to the intermediate grades, especially beginning in
grades 3 and 4 (Gaffney & Choi, 2010; McNamara, et al., 2011; Mol & Bus, 2011), and the
knowledge gained from reading informational text contributes greatly to comprehension
(McNamara, et al., 2011). However, the amount of time spent in reading instruction using
informational text is extremely small (Beerwinkle & Duke, 2000; Jeong, et al., 2010; Simpson,
1996; Strong, 2020; Wijekumar, Walpole, & Aguis, 2018), and the instructional activities used
with informational text do not require deep comprehension, such as completing a worksheet with
low level questions and activities like fill-in-the-blank or matching activities (Duke, 2000;
Gaffney & Choi, 2010). In addition, the genre of informational or expository text has structures
that differ widely from narrative texts, and comprehension is influenced by the text structure that
is utilized to convey the information (Kendeou & Van den Broeck, 2007; McNamara, et al.,
2011). This section provides the rationale behind the urgency to use informational text more
51
frequently and with increased rigor, both by the teacher in their instructional practices, and by the
learner in the types of learning in which they are asked to engage. Prior knowledge, how it is
acquired through the act of reading, and the role of vocabulary acquisition was explored as it
relates to successful reading comprehension, particularly of informational text.
The Importance of Prior Knowledge in Reading Comprehension
Prior knowledge plays a pivotal role in reading comprehension and in content learning,
because people learn things from what they read, and they apply that knowledge to new texts in
order to comprehend them (Afflerbach, 1990; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017). Because
reading is both an iterative and interactive process, the knowledge and experience a person has
and continues to gain helps to build new knowledge and new sense-making (Goodman, et al.,
2017; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2017; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Pearson & Billman, 2016; Rupley,
1975; Willingham, 2017; Willingham, 2006). The greater the background knowledge, the more a
student is able to draw on it as they read, and the more profound the effects on reading
comprehension (Afflerbach, 1990; Castles, Rastle & Nation, 2018; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016;
Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; Pearson & Billman, 2016; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson,
2017; Willingham, 2017; Willingham, 2006). The research has shown that when a person is
reading and can make connections to the text from existing knowledge they have on a subject,
comprehension is enhanced (Afflerbach, 1990; Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Kendeou,
McMaster, & Christ, 2016; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Spilich, Vesonder, Chiesi, & Voss, 1979).
The more comprehension is enhanced, the more the student is learning, and this new learning
contributes to a student’s body of knowledge. This cycle of continuous improvement can have
52
far-reaching effects on the successful achievement of students. Indeed, it is essential, for as
students matriculate through the grades, they must grapple with more complex, nuanced, and
challenging texts that require analyzing, interrogating, and synthesizing information found in and
across texts (Duhaylongsod, Snow, Selman, & Donovan, 2015; Moje, 2008; Shanahan &
Shanahan, 2008; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017).
Prior knowledge is not limited to information about a subject. Lipson (1982) stated that
“verbal knowledge, knowledge of text structure, knowledge about social interaction and human
intentionality, and knowledge of causal relations” (p. 244) are all prior knowledge structures,
along with prior world knowledge, and a reader brings these prior knowledge structures to any
new reading task. In her 2000 study on informational text, Nell Duke attended not only to the
contexts and intended audiences of texts, but to “specific linguistic features of text” (p. 205),
including grammatical constructions, graphical elements, definitions, and specific text structures,
such as comparative/contrastive and cause/effect structures. Further, she argued that a person
learns how to read a particular genre, like informational text, through experience with that genre,
and that this genre development can, and should, begin early on in a child’s life (pp. 206-207).
Doing so provides children with the opportunity to not only learn from informational and
expository texts, but to learn about them as well, including the structures and vocabulary
contained within them. This exposure builds the knowledge base needed to navigate the
increasingly complex texts that are used to teach content as students progress through the grades.
Prior knowledge also includes domain-specific vocabulary, such as the term birdie in
golf. Research continually shows the importance of academic vocabulary to the successful
navigation of informational, domain-specific, or content-specific texts in an academic setting
53
(Bailey & Heritage, 2008; Nagy, Townsend, Lesaux, & Schmitt, 2012). A person who is
navigating a text with domain-specific vocabulary with which they are familiar spends less
cognitive load determining the meaning of such words - which, as is the case with the golf term
birdie could have multiple meanings depending upon the topic - and more of a cognitive load
comprehending the text deeply, including such higher-order and cognitively complex skills as
determining main idea and authors purpose, and determining the importance of details
(Afflerbach, 1990). In school, many words are discipline-specific, such as cytoplasm,
polynomial, or federalism, and understanding them within the context of reading is crucial to
meaning-making and comprehension (Nagy, et. al, 2012). A student who has great interest in
history may already have a strong understanding of the term federalism and will have an easier
time comprehending a text focusing on that concept.
It is not just the amount of prior knowledge a person has that is important; it is also the
quality of the prior knowledge that determines successful reading comprehension (Goodman, et
al., 2017; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016). When reading a text, a person is continually using
previous knowledge to confirm or disconfirm information being presented in the text, whether
presented explicitly or implicitly. If presented implicitly, the reader must rely on inferential
processes to confirm or refute the information presented. Thus, a reader can create inaccurate
meaning from the text and will ultimately rely on inaccurate prior knowledge in future contextual
situations (Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Lipson, 1982). Accurate prior knowledge, however, can
be built early on in a child’s education. As Pearson and Billman (2016) emphasize, readers as
young as first grade are naturally curious about the world around them. Young students need
opportunities to engage in learning experiences that support learning new concepts and, as
54
importantly, support learning the words that represent those new concepts (content vocabulary),
and opportunities that help them learn and practice reading comprehension strategies such as
predicting, visualizing and asking questions (p. 24). Engaging in such activities by integrating
both disciplinary content and literacy provides an opportunity for young learners to learn to read
and read to learn, constructing meaning during reading to build what will become accurate prior
knowledge. Further, for some young readers, informational text is more engaging and interesting
than narrative text (Duke, 2000) and provides those students the opportunity to see and build
themselves as readers.
Yet research continues to show that, while the Common Core State Standards (2010),
with which the Minnesota State Standards in English Language Arts (2010) are closely
correlated, call for an increase in the use of informational text, there is still a dearth of
informational text being used for reading instruction in the lower elementary grades. Literacy
instruction in those grades continues to focus on phonemic awareness, phonics, and decoding,
and texts used to teach those skills do not include rich concepts that provide an opportunity for
students to make new meaning (Duke, 2000; Goldman, et al, 2016; Jeong, et al., Pearson &
Billman, 2016). Students, as they matriculate from the primary grades to the intermediate grades
and beyond, are asked to read and comprehend texts that are much more complex - in content,
structure, and vocabulary - yet they have not encountered these complex concepts, structures,
and specific vocabulary before, so they have not acquired the knowledge and skills needed to
navigate effectively and efficiently through these texts. Students can be further stymied in their
progress to grow as strong readers if they are not provided with the opportunities to learn new
content, text structures and syntax, and context correctly, creating new and accurate knowledge
55
for the next learning experience. It is easier for students to grasp main ideas by learning new
information correctly at the outset, rather than to have to correct inaccurate information
(Afflerbach, 1990; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Lipson, 1982).
The Acquisition of Knowledge Through Reading
Reading can open up brand new worlds, and make the reader thirst for more. Reading can
answer questions and generate new questions. In other words, reading is a way to fuel curiosity
and learn new things. The premise upon which informational text is written is to inform, and
students’ abilities to read and comprehend texts directly affect their capacity to learn new content
and build new knowledge - which, in turn, affects their future possibilities (Wanzek, et al., 2018).
Students in third and fourth grade are expected to determine main ideas from informational texts,
to integrate ideas across two texts on the same subject, to draw inferences by using key
information from a text, and to explain how an author uses reason and evidence to support their
points (Minnesota Department of Education, 2010). These standards require deeper
comprehension of informational, expository and technical texts students encounter in school,
which in turn, require that a certain set of skills and strategies be taught in elementary school,
such as prediction and inference, to determine the meaning of texts - both explicit and implicit,
(Goodman, et al., 2017; Graesser, 2015; Kendeou, McMaster, & Christ, 2016; Wanzek, et al.,
2018). Because reading achievement scores have remained relatively stagnant over the last three
decades, reading researchers continue to argue for a shift from a focus on the teaching of more
generic comprehension strategies to a focus on instruction which makes use of disciplinary
56
strategies (Goldman, Snow & Vaughn, 2016; Goldman & Snow, 2015), in order for students to
better traverse the often unique features of texts in different disciplines.
As stated previously, beginning in third grade, students are expected to determine the
main idea of an informational text and provide supporting evidence. While this is an extremely
important skill, determining the main idea is a complex and often difficult task; when the main
idea is not stated explicitly within the text, the reader must construct the main idea through
comprehension of the supporting evidence found within the text (Afflerbach, 1990; Coiro, 2011,
van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983), and through inferences made in the text (Kendeou, McMaster, &
Christ, 2016; Wancek, et al., 2018). Chall’s stages of reading development (1983) outlines
reading as a process which changes as readers gain skill and proficiency. She outlined six stages
from pre-reading to mature reading. An extremely critical transition time is found between Stage
2 and Stage 3 (Chall & Jacobs, 2003; Chall, et al., 1990), when students are increasingly using
reading as a tool for learning. As Chall and Jacobs (2003) state, “If children are unable to make
the transition from Stage 2 to 3, their academic success is usually severely challenged” (para. 3).
Duncan and colleagues (2007) emphasized that academic achievement gains are cumulative,
meaning that learning is a process that involves improvement of already existing skills and
mastery of new ones. Students transitioning from Chall’s Stage 2 to Stage 3 are encountering
texts that are more complex and varied, including words and concepts that are new and
unfamiliar to the everyday lived experiences of most students and where, if they are successful,
they will continue to build their foundational knowledge.
Foundational concepts and knowledge makes students better readers of both narrative and
informational texts, and across content areas, because reading is a process of constructing
57
meaning and continuing to build knowledge (Cervetti & Hiebert, 2015; Pearson & Cervetti,
2015). While these texts provide challenge for all readers, students from low-income families
and multilingual learners, in particular, face an increased challenge, as basic oral language skills
and myriad word meanings are critical for understanding increasingly complex and difficult texts
(Duncan, et al., 2007; Herbers, et al., 2012; Towsend, Barber, & Carter, 2020), and therefore
critical for building new knowledge. Yet students from low-income families and multilingual
learners from low-income families in particular face challenges in procuring access to the types
of resources needed to build language and literacy skills, including building a strong academic
vocabulary, in order to succeed in school.
The Role of Vocabulary Acquisition on Comprehension of Informational Texts
When one comprehends a text, they learn new things. Students leaving third grade and
moving to fourth grade are encountering more complex texts from which they are to learn new
information. Unfortunately, the now infamous term “fourth grade slump” (Chall & Jacobs, 2003;
McNamara, et al., 2011; Sweet & Snow, 2003; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017) refers to
myriads of students who are not achieving at grade level. However, Chall and Jacobs (2003)
found that the students’ decline was not in overall comprehension, but had its roots in a decline
in word recognition and word understanding (Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017). As Beck,
Perfetti, and McKeown (1982) aptly state, “words are just labels for concepts” (p. 508), and for
decades, researchers have found that knowledge of word meanings - the concept behind the label
- is strongly related to successful comprehension (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; McKenna &
Dougherty Stahl, 2015; Rupley & Nichols, 2005; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986). The more words that
58
students know, the more they are able to comprehend a multitude of texts at increasingly
complex levels. The act of reading is a complicated process, requiring countless connections
being both made in the brain and activated as one traverses a sentence, a paragraph, an article, or
a tome. Words are verbal and visual representations of concepts, and as a reader travels through a
text, they simultaneously comprehend and create new meaning, thus creating new knowledge
and new concepts in the mind. Readers do so by building a mental dictionary, which “uses more
precise definitions” (Willingham, 2017, p. 79), and they continually build new knowledge.
For the purposes of this study, vocabulary is defined “as the number of printed words that
are both decoded and understood” (White, Graves, & Slater, 1990, p. 281). In other words, a
student’s working vocabulary consists of the number of words the student can read and
understand. Knowledge of word meanings is strongly correlated with comprehension (Carroll,
1993; Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; Rupley & Nichols, 2005). Building a strong vocabulary,
however, means more than knowing a lot of words. Rapid lexical access during the act of reading
and within a certain situation is critical to understanding a particular text, as many words have
multiple meanings which are not universally appropriate in every situation (Ash & Baumann,
2017; Beck, Perfetti, & McKeown, 1982; Mezynski, 1983). Lexical access and quality is how
accurately and how quickly a person can retrieve a stored mental representation of a word
(Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Perfetti, 2007; Perfetti & Hart, 2002); such lexical access and
quality includes both spelling (i.e. the difference between face, farce, and fare), and multiple
meaning of the same word (i.e. the difference between the meaning of eating jam and sitting in a
traffic jam). The greater understanding of words and word meaning, the more cognitive resources
are freed up for comprehension and meaning-making (Beck, Perfetti, & McKeown, 1982;
59
Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Perfetti, 2007). Vocabulary instruction, therefore, must include
instructional strategies that increase the accuracy of word meanings, fluency with regard to
lexical access (how quickly a student can retrieve the correct meaning of the word), and a strong
understanding by the teacher of the effects that such accuracy and fluency have on
comprehension (McKeown, Beck, Omanson, & Perfetti, 1984).
However, as Michael Graves (2015) pointed out, research indicates that not much robust
vocabulary instruction is taking place in classrooms - the kind of vocabulary instruction that
engages students in challenging and deep meaning-making word work (p. 124). Indeed, Rupley
and Nichols (2005) summarized the historical teaching of vocabulary through an exercise done
with college students earning their teaching degree, whereas they asked the students to describe
their recollections of vocabulary instruction: “. . . receiving an arbitrary list of words on
Monday, looking up the definitions of the words in the dictionary on Monday and Tuesday, doing
some type of skill work . . . on Wednesday and Thursday, and taking a test on Friday” (p. 240).
Indeed, there is a vast difference between memorizing a list of words for a test (the definitional
knowledge) and learning vocabulary at a contextual knowledge level, which has a stronger
connection to text and therefore a stronger ability to aid comprehension (Cunningham &
Stanovich, 1997; Rupley & Nichols, 2005; Watts, 1995).
The absence of robust vocabulary instruction and strong word work, such as learning
prefixes, affixes, roots and suffixes, is quite unfortunate, as the size of students’ vocabularies
varies among different linguistic and socioeconomic groups. Systemic barriers, such as poverty
and income segregation, which will be discussed further in the next section, contribute to the fact
that students from lower socioeconomic groups have less access to literacy resources and
60
opportunities (Luo, Tamis-Lemonda, & Mendelsohn, 2019; Neuman & Celano, 2001; Neuman &
Moland, 2016), and therefore have different language skills when they reach school age (Hoff,
2013) and smaller vocabularies than their more advantaged peers. English Learner (EL) students
typically have smaller English vocabularies than their English-speaking peers and have different
language development trajectories despite linguistic strengths, and those gaps widen with age,
affecting student achievement (Graves, 2015; Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff, 2013; White, Graves, &
Slater, 1990).
The Effects of Race, Class, and Language on Reading Comprehension and Reading
Achievement
Schools are often seen as the hearts of a community. The community which is served by
the district in which this study took place is a small, first ring urban/suburban district, and
because of its proximity to a large urban area, the school district is very diverse in terms of race,
class, and home language. While the success of a community can be attributed to data such as
business growth, household income and other economic indicators, the future success of a
community is based largely in part on the education success of its members which, in turn, is
based largely on how well they can read (Angiulli, SIegel, & Maggi, 2004). Disparities in
achievement based upon race, socioeconomic status and language, unfortunately, have been a
stubborn issue in schools in the United States, and in the district in which this study took place.
Therefore, a review of the literature with regard to the effects of native language, socioeconomic
status, and race was warranted.
61
This study aimed to contribute to the body of research on reading comprehension by
answering the following questions:
What are the essential characteristics of informational texts that can be used for
training reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students?
This research was further guided by the following two sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
Children from all walks of life, no matter the race, income status, language, or culture,
gain their funds of knowledge through language - in the home, with friends and families, in the
community, and through other relationships (Conley, 2017; Moje, et al., 2004; Orellana,
Reynolds, & Martínez, 2010). They experience life in their neighborhoods, learn skills from
elders; they learn the idioms of their language, and they learn the norms of traversing their part
of the world. However, issues of race, class, language, and dialect keep underserved populations
in the United States from enjoying the same reading achievement in schools as their White,
middle-class peers (Almasi, Palmer, & Hart, 2010; National Center for Education Statistics,
2019; Troyer, et al., 2019). The literacy achievement gap begins at kindergarten entry and
62
persists throughout a child’s school career (Hoff, 2013; Neuman & Celano, 2001; Neuman &
Moland, 2019) because of multiple societal factors faced in early childhood. There continues the
need to examine the effects of institutional systems and circumstances that marginalize these
underserved populations and keep them from achieving at rates similar to their peers of the
majority population. Additionally, there continues the need for research on the effects of placing
students at the center of the curricula for students from different ethnic, cultural, linguistic and
socioeconomic backgrounds, and creating new opportunities for learning based on lived
experiences that challenge the continuing view of education found within mainstream value
orientations of a largely White teacher community (Heath, 1999), and that embrace the social
community in which children also learn.
Learning is personal as well as social, as Goodman, et al. (2017) so succinctly pointed
out. A teacher can support or hinder a child’s learning, as there is “no simple one-to-one
correspondence between what teachers teach and what students learn” (p. 91), and studies have
shown that expectations, especially of White teachers, can be lower for English learners and
other underserved populations of students, such as students of color, as teachers’ expectations are
framed by their own class- and experience-based beliefs (Jiménez, et al., 2015; Marx, 2000;
Redding, 2019). Indeed, instruction often is not inclusive of or responsive to students’ lived
experiences and the assets that come with the varying cultures, languages, genders, and races that
are and can be enjoyed in classrooms of diverse students (Almasi, et. al, 2010; Emdin, 2016;
Moje, 2000). Diversity also continues to play a role in the relationship between text and reader
(Pearson & Cervetti, 2017). There can be quite a disconnect between a student’s lived
experiences outside of school and the experiences they have within the school walls. Students
63
from varying backgrounds and cultures read, write, and speak for different purposes outside of
school, and are quite successful, while in school they are still judged as lacking skill (Hoff, 2013;
Shuman, 1986).
Yet, ironically, there is, at least partially, a remedy for this that has been in existence for
nearly 30 years, and has been articulated theoretically (for a review of the literature, see
Fairbanks, Cooper, Webb, & Masterson, 2017). Students will connect more deeply to and learn
more from texts in classrooms where teachers employ instruction that is culturally relevant and
responsive, including “bringing the relevance of the text to the child’s own experience to help
him or her make sense of the world” (Fairbanks, et al., 2017, p. 460; Kourea, Gibson, &
Werunga, 2017). Epistemology, or our conception of knowledge and how it is created, has
extremely deep undertones. As Neumann, Pallas, and Peterson (1999) explain, epistemologies
are “created through personal and social interactions attuned to the nature of thought (in
Lagemann & Shumann, 1999, p. 258). Much of what we know is rooted in our lived experience.
It’s been said that a person does not go to school to learn what they already know. However, our
lived experiences can help us learn more, but those lived experiences must be validated and
honored in classrooms through belief systems and strong instructional pedagogy (Fairbanks, et
al., 2017; Ladson-Billings, 2014; Ladson-Billings, 1995).
This section explored, in particular, the effects of socioeconomic status and language on
reading comprehension and reading achievement, both within and outside of the classroom.
Additionally, this section explored the effects on reading comprehension of the inclusion of texts
that reflect the cultures and the lived experiences of young readers, and the role those texts have
with regard to reading success.
64
The Effects of Socioeconomic Status on Reading Comprehension and Reading Achievement
Children begin learning the moment they are born. Oral language plays an important role
in that early learning. Additionally, access early on in a child’s development to print resources,
such as stories, narrative and informational books, poetry and even board books, has a strong
positive effect on a student’s vocabulary and background knowledge, and, as they continue to
matriculate through school, their comprehension of what they are reading, and differences in
print exposure are already apparent prior to any formal education in school (Allington, et al.,
2010; Evans, Kelley, Sikora, & Treiman, 2010; Mol & Bus, 2011; Neuman & Moland, 2019;
Rupley & Nichols, 2005). Because reading is an iterative process, these effects are both
immediate and long-term (Neuman & Moland, 2019). The more a child reads and comprehends
what they are reading, the more they learn, the more vocabulary they build, and the better they
can read increasingly complex texts. The more success they have in reading, the more they will
continue to read; they are motivated by that success, which in turn increases reading achievement
(Allington, 2014; Pribesh, Gavigan, & Dickinson, 2011; Taylor, Frye, & Maruyama, 1990;
Troyer, et al., 2019). Students who read much and read often repeat this cycle and success begets
success.
In the United States, however, children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds
underperform compared to their middle class counterparts, a well-documented problem that has
been persistent in public education for decades (Allington, et al., 2010; Hoff, 2013; Reardon &
Portilla, 2016; for a review see Neuman & Celano, 2001). This inequality, however, has been
historically attributed to individuals rather than to systems (Duncan, Morris, & Rodrigues, 2011;
65
Neuman & Celano, 2001), including the American education system and the racial and income
segregation of communities and neighborhoods. Emphasis must be placed at this time on the
difference between an inequity of systems, and the oft-quoted, oversimplified argument of the
“culture of poverty” (Lewis, 1966). The latter argues an ongoing, generational view of poverty
that includes dysfunction in families and communities, distorting “the concept of culture and
absolv[ing] social structures—governmental and institutional—of responsibility”
(Ladson-Billings, 2017, p. 82), whereas the former embraces an environmental view of poverty
emphasizing place, and focusing on the impact of social structures, such as the American
educational system and the systemic racial and income segregation of communities and
neighborhoods. A review of the literature focusing on literacy achievement with regard to
income disparity and income segregation was warranted, as lower-income neighborhoods
continue to be populated disproportionately by families of color (Neuman & Moland, 2019).
Contrasts in neighborhoods are stark, and the effects are deleterious for children.
Higher-income neighborhoods are able to provide resources and opportunities that are easily
accessible by families to help ready their students for school. Children in lower-income
neighborhoods have limited access to those same types of resources - in their homes and in their
communities (Allington, et al., 2010; Ladson-Billings, 2017; Mol & Bus, 2011; Neuman &
Moland, 2017). These resources include health and safety resources, green space with parks and
other recreational amenities, resource-rich schools that attract high performing teachers, and
school library media centers that have a plethora of both print and electronic materials.
Although access to print resources, which speaks to the environment in which a person
lives, continues to prove beneficial, the body of research is now just blossoming on the
66
socioeconomic makeup of communities and the effects therein that the lack of resources and
quality schools in low-income communities has on literacy achievement (Lareau & Goyette,
2014; Neuman & Celano, 2001; Neuman & Moland, 2019; Owens, Reardon & Jencks, 2016;
Rideout & Katz, 2016). For the purposes of this study, the term income segregation is used to
describe the system of “residential sorting” (Bischoff & Reardon, 2014) that leads to inequities
in access for children. Generally speaking, in low-income neighborhoods, libraries are scarce.
Families inconsistently have home libraries of books. Schools in lower income neighborhoods
have smaller media centers and classroom libraries than those found in middle class
neighborhoods, and they are quite smaller than those in upper-class neighborhoods (Allington,
2014; Pribesh, Gavigan, & Dickinson, 2011). One can only think for a moment about the annual
tradition of a school book fair and its impact on school and classroom libraries to visualize the
disparities between lower-income neighborhood schools and their middle- and upper-class
counterparts. Further - and sadly ironic - is the fact that many school library media centers are
open less days of the week than their middle-class and upper-class counterparts, yet middle- and
upper-class students frequent them less than lower-income students (Pribesh, Gavigan &
Dickinson, 2011; Worthy, Moorman & Turner, 1999). The sheer accessibility to a variety of
books and other texts has a great impact on reading achievement, as it is hard to read a book that
does not exist within one’s community.
Access to books is linked to voluntary reading, especially when there is a wide variety of
books from which to choose (Allington, 2014; McQuillan & Au, 2001). Voluntary reading means
more time spent reading which, as has been discussed previously, has a direct impact on reading
comprehension and reading achievement. Additionally, differences in access to a wide variety of
67
books and other print materials impact opportunities for learning and thinking that are related to
reading achievement, reading comprehension, and reading growth (Neuman & Celano, 2001).
Allington (2014) conveys an important argument, when he states, “. . . when you live in
a ‘book desert’, as do too many children from low-income families, one should not expect that
these children will engage in much voluntary reading” (p. 21). Yet, simply increasing the access
to texts that are of interest to children has a dramatic effect on reading comprehension, growth,
and achievement, as has been discussed previously (see also Lindsay, 2013). Additionally,
providing extended time to read quality literature in the classroom, especially classrooms which
are composed of students whose families have limited economic means, can have great effects on
reading comprehension, growth, and achievement. However, schools that serve largely
lower-income students typically are in poorer physical condition, have fewer instructional
resources, less rigorous curricula, less books per child, poorer quality books per child, fewer
computers per child, lower expectations for achievement, and teachers and media specialists with
fewer formal qualifications; this is overwhelmingly due to the social structures, including school
funding issues (Neuman & Celano, 2001; Owens, Reardon, & Jencks, 2016), and all of these
factors come into play with regard to achievement.
The Effects of Multilingualism on Reading Comprehension and Reading Achievement
As has been established in this review of the literature, strong literacy skills are a crucial
component of academic achievement. The majority of assessments that students in the United
States take to show mastery or proficiency in literacy/English language Arts/reading
achievement are in English. In the state in which this study was conducted, public school
68
districts are mandated to administer the state standardized reading assessment beginning in
grades three through eight, and once again in grade 10. However, classrooms across America
continue to increase in diversity, and diversity of language. Nearly 45 million immigrants lived
in the United States in 2018, the highest number recorded since any census records have been
kept (Batalova, Blizzard, & Bolter, 2020). However, most of that population is over the age of
40. According to Batalova, Blizzard & Bolter (2020), Fewer than 1 percent of immigrants were
under age 5 in 2018. . . Five percent of immigrants were aged 5 to 17 years” (para. 18). Most of
the English Language learner (EL) students in schools are children of immigrants who were born
in the United States, and much of the rise in overall diversity in schools comes from the rise of
English learners (ELs), or multilingual learners (MLLs) (Brown, 2017; Slavin & Cheung, 2005).
These learners live in families where the home language is different from the language dominant
in school-based instruction in the United States, which is English, and it is important to
emphasize that a majority of ELs live in poverty (Brown, 2017; Zong & Batalova, 2015).
English Language learners are often thought about in the collective; however, they are
anything but. They come from all over the world, from cultures that speak myriad languages, and
they all fall along a continuum of diverse, educational needs. These students must also take a
yearly language proficiency assessment, gauging their proficiency in the English language in
four domains: reading, writing, speaking, and listening, until they attain scores that show a level
of proficiency where they can be exited from service (Minnesota Department of Education,
2020). While the goal of these regulations is college- and career readiness in the English
language, the system cannot and should not ignore the language assets that students have and use
outside of those that are assessed through formal schooling. They include rich language that
69
serves many communicative and cultural purposes and are found in normal, daily routines,
situations, and interactions (Goodman, et al., 2017; Tong, Irby, Lara-Alecio & Koch, 2014).
Strong literacy skills are a crucial component of academic success, especially in subjects
that are academically and conceptually demanding, such as science. Furthermore, the lack of
strong literacy skills is evidenced by graduation and the high dropout rate among students who
struggle in reading (Ardasheva, Y., Norton-Meier, L., & Hand, B, 2015; Cook, Pérusse, & Rojas,
2012; Hoff, 2013). In 2016 the national graduation rate for ELs was 67%, which is up from
2010, but still lagging far behind the non-EL average of 85% (U. S. Department of Education,
“Graduation Rates,” n.d., para. 1). Latinx dual language learners make up the largest population
of ELs in the United States, although the largest majority of them are United States citizens, born
in the United States (Hoff, 2013; U. S. Department of Education “Who Are English Learners,”
n.d., para. 1), and for the purposes of this study, this statistic is important: Latinx students make
up the majority population of the ELs learners in the district in which this study took place.
A review of the literature showed that the development of literacy skills is quite similar
whether a student is an EL learner or a student for whom English is their first language, and the
learning of literacy skills in a student’s native language can form the foundation for a successful
transfer of skills to a second language (Angiulli, et al., 2004; Brown, 2017; Palmer, Shackelford,
Miller, & Leclere, 2007), but much depends upon the way transfer is taught. Like non-EL
students who are from lower socioeconomic families, “[l]iteracy is not learned differently in
school and out of school. If there is a difference in success, it is the way literacy is taught in
school, not the way it is learned” (Goodman, et al., 2017, p. 93). Proficient bilingual readers have
been explicitly taught both transfer skills and strategies, vocabulary acquisition strategies, and
70
were also taught to recognize the similarities and differences in each language (Brown, 2017;
Palmer, et. al, 2007). There is a relationship between multilingual children’s oral language skills
in their heritage language and the acquisition of literacy in English, especially when the student’s
first language uses phonetic orthographies as English does (Slavin & Cheung, 2005). The skills
of phonological awareness, awareness of morphology and higher-order comprehension that a
child learns in their heritage language transfer to the learning of English literacy as well, and
when measuring language knowledge in both languages combined, bilingual children are equal
to, or sometimes even exceed, their monolingual counterparts in vocabulary acquisition (Hoff,
2013, p.10), concepts of print (Bialystok & Feng, 2011), and greater phonological awareness
(Bialystock, 2003). This framing has strong implications for both instruction and learning, and
ultimately academic success, especially when taken from an asset-based point of view.
While proficiency in the English language of ELs lags behind their English monolingual
peers, especially with regard to English vocabulary knowledge and English word-learning
strategies, strong instruction can help ELs catch up (Brown, 2017). However, the instruction
must be carefully planned to reduce cognitive overload. EL students are learning content in
tandem with language, and texts, especially informational texts in content areas, can be “lexically
dense” - that is, have a high number of content words per sentence (Ardasheva, et al., 2015),
which is difficult for many students, and can slow text processing and comprehension, especially
for EL students who are acquiring both general and specific academic language. Furthermore,
although explicit comprehension strategy instruction is beneficial, it, too, can overload EL
learners, especially young EL students. Therefore carefully planned instruction that includes
71
modified approaches is more beneficial, such as visual and linguistic scaffolding, graphic
organizers, cooperative grouping, and concrete examples (Brown, 2017; Ardasheva, et al., 2015).
Explicit vocabulary instruction and the minimization of the use of out-of-context
instruction and memorization activities are two additional ways to strengthen comprehension
instruction for all students, especially ELs and their low-socioeconomic peers. Out-of-context
activities, such as the use of low-level worksheets and word lists counter the fact that people use
literacy and/or language everyday, as literacy encompasses speaking and listening as well as
reading and writing. Literacy is firmly rooted in social situations. Even reading a book in a
solitary environment is a social situation, because the reader is interacting with the authors
words, thoughts, purposes and intent. Communication is taking place. However, in a classroom,
that social situation rooted in the dominant language may be unfamiliar to students for whom
English is not their dominant language. Strong reading comprehension is linked to strong oral
language skills, including a wide vocabulary and strong understanding of syntax (August &
Shanahan, 2006; Hoff, 2013). The dominant language of American schools is English, and
English is prolific in examples of figurative language, especially idioms, which teachers use in
roughly 1 out of 10 words in their instruction (Palmer, et al.). Castles, et al.(2018) highlight the
nuances of the English language, such as idioms and other figurative language, and their
importance for second language learners: “For second-language learners, reading comprehension
processes are not deficient in themselves, but limitations in reading comprehension might follow
from differences in knowledge relative to children whose first language is the majority language”
(p. 29).
72
An EL student may be deemed deficient in literacy, when the truth is that they may be
lacking in the understanding of the nuances of the English language, and skills of academic
literacy - reading, writing, speaking and listening across content in that dominant language of
schools in the United States. However, bilingual children have experiences in another language
that provide background knowledge for understanding but are not reflected in their English
vocabularies (Hoff, 2013). Given the proper support, instruction, motivating activities connected
to the students’ lives, and adequate time, EL students can learn English at levels required to
achieve academic success in school (Angiulli, et al., 2004; Hoff, 2013; Palmer, et al., 2007;
Slavin & Cheung, 2005) and can use the language when needed throughout their professional
and personal lives in a global society.
New Literacies, Online Reading, and the Changing Nature of Reading Comprehension
The Internet, one of our most transformative technologies, has rapidly become the
defining place to find information and to communicate with others; indeed, the scale of change
that the Internet has brought is unprecedented with regard to the use by the number of people in
the number of places and in the short amount of time (Castek, et al, 2015; Coiro, Knobel,
Lankshear, & Leu, 2008; Roser, Ritchie, & Ortiz-Ospina, 2020; Wyatt-Smith & Elkins, 2008). In
2016, 76% of the population in the United States was considered Internet users (Roser, Ritchie,
& Ortiz-Ospina, 2020, para 6). Technology has changed the way texts are displayed and
connected; therefore, technology has changed the way we acquire and read information. K-12
students in the United States, like adults, are increasingly using the Internet to find and process
their information (Goldman, Braasch, Wiley, Graesser, & Brodowinska, 2012), and both the
73
Internet and wi-fi create new spaces for social interaction, which requires new social practices
and understanding, including comprehension of those social practices, such as blogging, text
messaging, and the use of email (Leu, et al., 2015).
While the term online reading continues to be used, it is gradually becoming engulfed in
a more precise term, which not only eliminates the separation of online reading and offline
(printed) reading, but encapsulates more what the reader is doing, which is utilizing strategic
searching often to solve a problem or do research on an issue (Wharton-McDonald & Erickson,
2017), and “connect[ing] social practices, people, technology, values, and literate activity” (Selfe
& Hawisher, 2004, p. 2, as quoted in Wyatt-Smith & Elkins, 2008). This term is called new
literacies ( Castek, Coiro, Henry, Leu & Hartman, 2015; Coiro, 2011a; Coiro, 2020; Coiro,
Knobel, Lankshear, & Leu, 2008; Gallego & Hollingsworth, 1992; Haetman, Morsink & Zheng,
2010; Leu, et al., 2008; Leu, et al., 2015), which encompasses social practices, the act of writing
(using and creating knowledge), and cultural contexts, including the literacies found in students’
communities and personal lives (Coiro, 2011a; Gallego & Hollingsworth, 1992; Wyatt-Smith &
Elkins, 2008), as well as technical skills.
The term itself is often used in different ways, but it overarchingly reflects the belief that
the idea of literacy (used here in the singular) is rapidly changing and transforming as new
technologies emerge to include multiple literacies (used in the plural) of community literacy,
disciplinary literacy, and social literacy, which is collaborative in nature (Gallego &
Hollingsworth, 2992; Hartman, et. al, 2010; Leu, et al., 2015; Wyatt-Smith & Elkins, 2008), and
for which multiple texts are inherently used in relation to other texts in the act of comprehending
(Hartman, et al., 2010). The Internet is a global network capable of providing infinite, connected
74
information in a myriad of different text structures (blogs, wikis, videos, chats) with a simple
click of a key on a keyboard. Literacy is no longer a static construct; it changes continually, and
comprehension of all of the various texts, including evaluation with regard to accuracy and
validity of those texts, becomes more complex and requires additional skills, placing more
processing demands on the reader (Baker, 2010, Coiro, Knobel, Lankshear, & Leu, 2008; Leu, et
al., 2015). However, traditional education, especially in the elementary years, has lagged in its
ability to teach the skills required to navigate the demands that the new literacies require
(vanDijk & van Deursen, 2014).
If skills required to become adept at online comprehension are not taught at an early age
in school, the situation is even more dire, as there is still a strong digital divide outside of school,
never more publicly pronounced than during the COVID-19 pandemic. According to recent
research from the Pew Research Center, “35% of households with children ages 6 to 17 and an
annual income below $30,000 a year do not have high-speed Internet connection at home,
compared with just 6% of such households earning $75,000 or more a year,” and this issue is
more pronounced for low-income students of color (Auxier & Anderson, 2020, para. 4). This is
not a new issue; the Pew Research Center reported similar data in 2012 (Leu, et al., 2015), and is
an important factor in the disenfranchisement of already marginalized students, as the home has
become a key factor in learning digitally (Leu, et al, 2015; Rogers, 2016; Wyatt-Smith & Elkins,
2008).
While the term digital divide was coined during the Clinton administration to define the
inequitable access to computers, the Internet, and other information and communication
technologies (ICTs), the term has evolved over the last several decades to include inequalities in
75
technology skills, how those skills are acquired, and how the technologies are used in daily life
(Rogers, 2016). In order to eliminate today’s digital divide, students must be taught how to
navigate digital texts, and must be provided with ample opportunities for practice with engaging
tasks. The types of digital texts available through blogs, social media, websites and the like, have
altered the landscape of what we read, and students even in elementary school must be taught
how to navigate it in order to access, process, communicate, and comprehend the overwhelming
abundance of information that is at their fingertips.
A testament to this altered landscape is the recent National Council of Teachers of
English (NCTE) chat on Twitter and announced on an NCTE blog post: “Which qualities do you
believe are the most important to be literate in this digital age?” (NCTE, 2020). However, as
evidenced by the plethora of posts on social media and conflicting news reports, anyone can post
just about anything on the Internet and tout it as “real” or “fake” information. As consumers of
these texts, it is incumbent upon us as readers and users of the information to discern the
accuracy of informational digital texts in order to make sense of them. This is part of
comprehension: evaluating the validity of the information read in texts.
Unfortunately, students in the United States do not receive adequate, explicit digital
reading comprehension skill instruction (Leu, et al., 2008), even though students can become
better readers through the explicit teaching of comprehension strategies - strategies that are used
for offline (printed) texts but that can be transferred to online reading environments (Almasi, et
al., 2010; Brown, Pressley, Van Meter, & Schuder, 1996; Guthrie & Cox, 2001; Pressley &
Allington, 2015). Digital reading skills have as their foundation the traditional printed text
reading skills of vocabulary, word recognition, response to literature, and comprehension, but
76
offline reading comprehension skills and online, digital reading comprehension skills are not
isomorphic ( Leu, et al., 2013; Coiro, 2007; Mokhtari, Kymes, & Edwards, 2009). A person
needs both types of comprehension skills to fully comprehend what they read online, and a
broader understanding of reader, text, context and purpose, as well as environment, is needed by
both the literary community and educators (Coiro, 2020). Specifically, Coiro (2003) outlined
new reading practices gained through the use of the Internet to read, such as different interaction
with text (e.g. hyperlinks), types of background knowledge; and new activities, such as creating
and publishing multimedia projects and participating in digital conversations.
However, no state reading assessment to date includes the assessment of digital reading
comprehension skills, nor does the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP), even
though the Common Core State Standards (CCSS), which the state in which this study took place
is completely aligned, and actually has included additional media literacy standards, requires that
students use technology and digital media to comprehend (English Language Arts
Standards>Anchor Standards>College and Career Readiness Anchor Standards for Reading,
n.d.; English Language Arts, n.d.). However, The Organisation for Economic Cooperation and
Development (OECD) has shown it values reading in a digital environment. The OECD
launched the Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA) for the first time in the
late 1990s, and in 2018 the PISA reading assessment focused on “reading in a digital
environment” (Leu, et al., 2008; Graesser, 2015; OECD, 2018). That said, the PISA assessment
is not utilized by any state for educational accountability purposes under the Every Student
Succeeds Act (ESSA), due to the fact that PISA uses a random sampling of 15-year old students
from schools throughout districts and states, and from countries that volunteer to take part in the
77
assessment (OECD, 2018). This assessment has no consequences on schools and districts, unlike
state standardized tests, which currently assess only offline reading skills but are used by states
for accountability purposes to satisfy federal law. State accountability tests have public
consequences and with them, the adage that what gets tested gets taught. In other words, state
accountability tests impact the content and materials that teachers teach and students learn
(“What Gets Tested Gets Taught,” 2018). With no current assessment of digital reading skills,
online reading comprehension and strategy instruction is being under-taught, and under-assessed.
As Leu, et al. (2008) emphasize, “To continue ignoring online reading comprehension in
reading assessments and during classroom reading instruction is to reify a static and increasingly
less-relevant understanding of reading comprehension in a world that has gone online, global,
and networked” (p. 335). Stated previously, students are increasingly using the Internet and other
online and technology tools to find and process information, as the Internet and social media
apps have a wealth of informational text. If students struggle in reading, however, they are
inclined to dislike reading and to read less (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Herbers, et al.,
2012). New literacies present challenges, especially for students who already struggle with
traditional print reading, in terms of fluency and critical reading habits (Dalton & Proctor, 2008).
The less they read, the less motivated they are to read and the further they fall behind, leading to
long-term academic trajectories that are hard to disrupt (Herbers, 2012). Motivation and
engagement play a large part in student success, and technology can provide a helpful
environment for that motivation and engagement, especially when students are taught how to
navigate that environment. Digital texts can increase the engagement in the reading process for
struggling readers through the use of multimedia, graphic images, and even the use of Command
78
+F, and various digital text features and approaches, such as multimedia and gaming support
engagement as well (Dalton & Proctor, 2008; Leu, et al., 2013). Further, computer-based reading
and learning provide the arena for independence, choice, and different formats in which to learn,
and can provide opportunities for practice, intentional scaffolding, and tutoring (Hooshyar,
Ahmad, Yousefi, Fathi, Abdollahi, Horng, & Lim; 2016). Multiple text options and opportunities
for engagement through different online tools can influence students’ self-regulation with regard
to reading (Azevedo & Cromley, 2004; Kulikowich, 2008; Massey & Miller, 2017) and provide
the environment in which reading can be engaging and students are motivated to learn.
The Role of Engagement, Motivation, and Attitude in Reading Comprehension
Because comprehension is inherently an interaction between the text and the reader, this
section explores the role that engagement and motivation in the act of reading imparts on reading
achievement. As stated previously, successful reading achievement leads to success in school and
beyond. Two other factors that relate to school success are the engagement of the student in
school, in school subjects, and the motivation to learn (Duke, 2000; Guthrie, 2015; Guthrie, et
al., 2004; Reschly & Christenson, 2012; Troyer, et al., 2019), including the motivation to read
and engagement in the act of reading, both within the school walls and outside of school,
especially in the summer (Duke, 2000; Duke & Martin, 2015; Guthrie, et al., 2006; Mol & Bus,
2011; Troyer, et al., 2019).
Scholars have differed on their use of the terms motivation and engagement - some using
the terms interchangeably, some keeping the two terms more distinct (Reschly & Christenson,
2012; Unrau & Quirk, 2014), but both terms have been used to describe factors that lead to
79
reading behaviors and reading achievement. Further, there is not full agreement in the research
on the precise definitions of the two terms. Motivation is generally thought to be an internal and
malleable process which includes emotions as well as thoughts and beliefs that is an influential
force, but that can be shaped through the use of different factors (Guthrie, et al., 2006; Troyer, et
al., 2019; Unrau & Quirk, 2014; Varuzza, Sinatra, Eschenauer, & Blake, 2014). Engagement, like
motivation, also has many definitions but is connected to a person and their individual actions
(Reschly & Christenson, 2012; Unrau & Quirk, 2014); as well, engagement also is thought to be
malleable (Fredricks, Blumenfeld & Paris, 2004). That motivation and engagement are seen as
being malleable is significant for education researchers and educators alike, as it means that both
can change as a result of student experience. For the purpose of this study, engagement is viewed
as action and motivation as intent - somewhat separate but very much related concepts. Further,
and more importantly, both concepts are a significant predictor of students’ reading
comprehension and achievement (Guthrie, et al., 2010; Reschly & Christenson, 2012; Troyer, et
al., 2019; Varuzza, et al., 2014).
Engagement inherently links different contexts to the students, including lived
experiences outside of school; relationships with others, such as peers; and the school
experiences themselves, including relationships with teachers. Students spend the majority of
their formal school experience in classrooms with teachers, so the environment that teachers
create in their classrooms, including the instructional practices they employ, affect how students
see themselves as learners and help to shape their attitude toward reading (Varuzza, et al., 2014).
When a student is engaged in a learning experience, they are active: they are active listeners,
active participants in discussion, active writers, actively engrossed in a text; in other words, they
80
are actively learning. Varuzza et al. (2014) also found that students enjoyed reading activities that
utilized oral language interaction - in other words, discussion that helped the students further
understand and enjoy the readings. The study conducted by Varuzza and his colleagues was of
particular interest, as their study involved large populations of Hispanic, low socioeconomic and
English language learners (Varuzza, et al., 2014), which is critical to this study and to this
literature review. It has been established in this literature review that low socioeconomic
students, students of color (especially Black and Brown students) and students for whom English
is not their first language continue to struggle in reading achievement. Further, as Guthrie and
Cox (2001) explained, engaged readers “are intrinsically motivated to read for the knowledge
and enjoyment it provides” (p. 284). They read often, learning about new worlds as they traverse
their texts. They enjoy the act of reading, especially of and with texts that are of great interest to
them.
Motivation, on the other hand, is more nuanced, as it encompasses values and beliefs,
along with behaviors (Guthrie, 2015; Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; Troyer, et al., 2019; Unrau &
Quirk, 2014; Varuzza, et al., 2014). While motivation is a difficult concept to measure, as it is
seen as multidimensional (De Naeghel, Van Keer, Vansteenkiste, & Rosseel, 2012; Varuzza, et
al., 2014), as it encompasses feelings, emotions, and desires, it has been found to be a strong
predictor of comprehension assessment scores, strategy use and engagement (Varuzza, et al,
2014). Motivation has classically been delineated into two types: intrinsic motivation and
extrinsic motivation (De Naeghel, et al., 2012; Massey & Miller, 2017; Unrau & Quirk, 2014;
Guthrie, 2015). While extrinsic motivation can propel our behavior, it is intrinsic motivation that
has a long-term impact on reading achievement (Guthrie, 2015).
81
It is important to understand the concept of reading motivation in order to help students
stay motivated to read, as different motivations can present themselves in either positive or
negative forms, either driving students to reading or pushing them away from reading (Guthrie,
2015; Guthrie, et al., 2006; Naeghel, Van Keer, Vansteenkiste, & Rosseer, 2012). Motivation has
been historically viewed in the United States as very individual (Massey & Miller, 2017; Guthrie
& Wigfield, 2000), but reading comprehension strengthens as a result of the student being
engaged in the text, motivated to comprehend with intentional reading purposes, and engaging
with tasks within the context of interesting texts (Coiro, 2011a; Duke & Martin, 2015; Guthrie, et
al, 2006), all of which can be influenced within a classroom context.
Engaged and motivated readers are also strategic; less engaged readers show less use of
reading strategies to comprehend a text (Guthrie & Cox, 2001; Wigfield, et al, 2008). Strategy
instruction can increase engagement and motivation along with reading comprehension, using
several approaches, such as self-monitoring, gleaning main ideas, questioning, summarizing,
paraphrasing, and inference (Wigfield, et al., 2008). Relevant to this research are those adopted
in Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI; Guthrie & Cox, 2001; Guthrie et al., 1996,
2007; Wigfield et al., 2014). Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction is focused on informational
texts to explicitly teach reading strategies, and the elements of CORI are sound instructional
practices. Implemented in CORI are six teaching practices to increase student motivation: 1)
providing thematic content information, 2) optimizing choice, 3) explicit teaching of reading
strategies, 4) hands-on activities, 5) providing interesting texts, and 6) collaboration (Guthrie &
Cox, 2001; Guthrie, et al., 1996, 2007). Other studies have shown that these six are effective,
along with an additional variable, which is having reading goals (Guthrie & Cox, 2001; Wigfield,
82
et al., 2008). Students who experience teaching practices like those found in CORI have shown
larger comprehension gains and demonstrated both greater reading strategy use and reading
motivation than those given only strategy training or traditional reading instruction. These effects
have been found at both third grade (Guthrie et al., 2004) and fourth grade (Wigfield et al.,
2008).
Motivation to read begins to decline as children get older, beginning near the end of the
elementary years (De Naeghel, et al., 2012; Skinner & Pitzer, 2012; Troyer, et al., 2019; Varuzza,
et al., 2014; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017). However, this decline in motivation is more
pronounced for students for whom reading is a struggle; they take less pleasure from reading and
this has a cumulative effect on both reading achievement and the motivation to learn
(Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017). When students are not given choice in reading, or when
students feel the texts are irrelevant or they do not find themselves or their experiences reflected
in the books they read, their intrinsic motivation to read declines, as they feel devalued not only
in the classroom, but in the larger society for which the classroom is an extension (Bishop,
1990). This issue can be mitigated, however, through the structure of a positive learning
environment. Skinner and Pitzer (2012) called attention to the belief that students “come with a
wellspring of intrinsic motivation that does not have to be acquired and cannot be lost” (p. 33).
Gay (2010) spoke eloquently about the creation of learning spaces that celebrate students,
especially students of color, asking teachers to engage students “to and through their personal
and cultural strengths, their intelletctual capabilities, and their prior accomplishments” (p. 26).
The creation of a culturally inclusive, more engaging classroom environment that includes strong
83
pedagogy, can build student agency and offset the natural decline in reading motivation that
students experience as they get older (De Naeghel, et al., 2012).
Culture and ethnicity play a central role in engagement, especially given the
underachievement of African American, Latinx, and American Indian students in the United
States and the achievement gap of these populations and their White peers (Bingham & Okagaki,
2012). Engagement and motivation take on a more urgent role for students who are experiencing
structural racism within impoverished communities, negative stereotypes, and systemic racism in
formal schooling whereby students of color do not feel included or acknowledged for who they
are (Reynolds, Sneva, & Beehler, 2010; Tatum, 2006; Thomas, 2018), as their prior knowledge
and lived experiences are less likely to be prominent in many of the instructional materials and
assessments used in classrooms (McCullough, 2013). Additionally, many students of color lack
positive role models within an academic setting - including teachers of color and positive
representation in texts read in school, as it is as important for students to make personal
connections to their school experiences and learning as it is to provide students with new
experiences and learning (Bishop, 1990. Christ & Sharma, 2018; Tatum, 2006).
Bishop (1990) coined the terms mirrors, windows, and sliding glass doors (p. 1) to
describe the endless possibility of experiences students of color could have in classrooms that
would further engage and motivate them. Texts provide windows, or a way of seeing and
viewing new or imagined worlds; they provide sliding glass doors which, like windows offer
new and exciting views of the world but also offer the opportunity for students to see themselves
in those new worlds; and they provide mirrors, wherein readers can see their “lives and
experiences as part of the larger human experience” (Bishop, 1990, p. 1). Reading and learning
84
from relevant texts, then, can be not only a self-affirming experience, but an experience that
strengthens students’ self-efficacy, particularly for students of color.
Related to motivation and engagement is reading attitude. The attitudes that students
bring to the act of reading are inextricably linked to motivation and engagement. Attitudes are
tied to feelings and emotions, especially with regard to school in general and the act of reading
specifically (Alexander & Filler, 1976; Fiedler & Beier, 2014; Logan & Johnston, 2009;
McKenna, Kear & Ellsworth, 1995; Smith, 1990; Wade, 2012). Reading attitude, especially with
regard to students’ perceptions of themselves with regard to the act of reading, influences the
amount of independent reading, involvement in activities related to reading, and use of cognitive
strategies (Akbari, Ghonsooly, Ghazanfari, & Shahriari, 2017; Logan & Johnston, 2009;
McKenna, et al., 1995; Varuzza, et al., 2014), as attitude contributes to a student embracing or
evading a situation that involves reading (McKenna, et al., 1995).
McKenna, et al. (1995) stated that “an individual’s attitude toward reading will develop
over time principally as the result of three factors: normative beliefs, beliefs about the outcomes
of reading, and specific reading experiences (p. 939). Further, reading attitude encompasses
social norms, especially in a school setting where students are forming and re-forming identities
and group affinities, students are looking for affirmation from teachers, and there is a perceived,
if not real, competition for different statuses (e.g. academic, athletic, artistic, popularity).
Unfortunately, as stated previously, positive reading attitudes can decline as students matriculate
through the grades (Martínez, Aricak, & Jewell, 2008; McKenna, et al., 1995; Smith, 1990;
Varuzza, et al., 2014).
85
Attitudes form as a result of experience, success and failure, and cultural expectations,
including the expectations by gender (Akbari, et al., 2017; Logan & Johnston, 2009; McKenna,
et al., 1995) as well as by ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and first language (Akbari, et al.,
2017; McKenna, 19997; McKenna, et al., 1995; Mohd-Asraf & Abdullah, 2016; Wade, 2012).
Therefore, the importance of attitude toward reading warranted a review of the literature
focusing on gender, socioeconomic, ethnic, and language differences.
With regard to traditional gender, girls were found to have more positive attitudes toward
reading, they read more, and had higher reading achievement than boys (Akbari, et al., 2017;
Logan & Johnston, 2009; McKenna, et al., 1995; Mohd-Asraf & Abdullah, 2016; Swalander &
Taube, 2007). This was also the case when socioeconomic status was a factor (McKenna, 1997).
With regard to ethnicity and race, McKenna, et al. (1997), citing work from McDermott (1974)
and Long (1963), stated that students of color could sense that teachers judged them and their
abilities differently than the majority population, ignoring their cultural identity and forcing
mainstream cultural norms upon them that felt oppressive (p. 939). Akbari, et al. (2017)
emphasized that reading is a skill, no matter the first or home language; however, the attitude
toward the home language was highly correlated to the attitude toward the second language (L2),
and gender also played a role in the attitude toward L2 reading (Mohd-Asraf & Abdullah, 2016).
Cultural norms, whether economic, linguistic, ethnic or genderized, are a factor with regard to
reading attitude.
86
The Role of Culturally Relevant Texts on Successful Comprehension
The term culture is nuanced in meaning. Merriam Webster has several definitions of the
term that reference shared experiences and features of groups of people and also institutions
(Merriam-Webster, n.d.). Examples can include teen culture, school culture, hip-hop culture, and
a myriad more. However, culture is often used when referring to the lived experiences of people
of color. In that vein, the term culture can carry with it harmful baggage with regard to
assumption and judgment and, as a result, the assignment of greater or lesser worth, with terms
coined such as cultural deprivation, cultural disadvantage, and culture of poverty (Erickson,
2012, p. 561). When used, these terms assign a preconception of ability and validity to students
based on their lived experiences. It is true that the faces that greet teachers in public schools in
the United States continue to be more diverse with regard to ethnicity, culture, and language, but
each of those students come with assets that others do not have. According to the National
Center for Education Statistics (2019), of the projected 50.7 million public K-12 students in
2020, only 46.1% are White; the second most populous group identify as Hispanic, at 27.6%,
followed by 15% who identify as Black, 5% who identify as Asian, 4.5% who identify as two or
more races, 1% who identify as American Indian, and .3% who identify as Pacific Islanders
(NCES, 2019, What are the demographics of public school students), with the population of
Latinx, Black and Asian students increasing every year (Glass, 2019).
As established previously through a review of the literature, there is an alarming, yet
persistent gap in reading achievement between students of color, students from low
socioeconomic families, and EL students and their White, middle-class peers and, if not
87
mediated by the time students are in grades 3 and 4, the chances that students will catch up to
their grade level peers is slim (Barber, et al., 2018). However, as Erickson (2012) emphasizes,
outside lived experiences often “don’t fit well with the cultural practices of . . . classrooms” (p.
562); therefore, it becomes incumbent upon schools and teachers to become responsive to the
students’ strengths and interests. While motivation for reading begins to decline in elementary
school, the beliefs and practices that teachers bring into an inclusive classroom environment,
including culturally relevant text selection and pedagogy, can support motivation and
engagement, which in turn support positive reading achievement (Barber, et al., 2018; De
Naeghel, et al., 2012; Christ & Sharma, 2018; Guthrie, 2015). Guthrie and Cox (2001) stated the
use of relevant books quite succinctly: “For reading instruction, text is central” (p. 291). Using
literature in reading instruction that is positively reflective of and relevant and interesting across
different cultures and experiences, that reflects the students in the classroom and draws upon
their backgrounds and languages, and with which the students can identify, has a positive impact
on reading achievement and on readers’ self esteem (Barber, et al., 2018; Ebe, 2010;
Ladson-Billings, 1995; Kourea, Gibson, & Werunga, 2017; Tatum, 2006, Troyer, et al., 2019).
High quality, engaging materials matter. When students have an opportunity to interact
with high quality literature that reflects them and their lived experiences, reading comes to life, it
takes on new meaning and urgency, and it becomes meaningful to the students (Keis, 2005).
Troyer, et al., (2019) used the term “high reading quality” (pp. 1204, 1206) to describe texts that
were matched to both the students’ reading ability and their interests, including their experiences
outside of school, which Ladson-Billings (1992, 1995) describes as “culturally relevant” with
regard to texts and teacher practice. Bringing students’ lived experiences into the classroom, and
88
finding materials to match them, celebrates the students and their cultures - in other words, it
legitimizes them and their experiences. It “place[s] education into culture, rather than
place[s]culture into education” (Pewewardy, 1993, p. 78) which, of course, is less surface culture
and more authentic learning. However, as Ladson-Billings (1992) echoed, it is imperative that
leaders in education, whether it be at national, state, district or classroom levels, use the culture
of the students to help them understand themselves and to understand and interact with the world
around them and to “conceptualize knowledge” (Ladson-Billings, p. 314) to challenge existing
assumptions and structures in order to create endless opportunities for the future. In order to do
that, students must be provided with texts that reflect themselves, and their lives, in ways that
open doors.
Although the texts matter, there continues to be a struggle to find quality literature written
by and about Black, Indigenous, and People of Color (BIPOC). Each year, the Cooperative
Children’s Book Center, housed at the University of Wisconsin-Madison, which is a research
library in service to librarians, teachers, early childhood providers, and others, provides
information on diversity in publications for children and teens (Cooperative Children’s Book
Center, n.d.). In June of 2020, they released the 2019 CCBC Diversity Statistics, which is later
than usual due to the COVID-19 pandemic (The Numbers are In: 2019 CCBC Diversity
Statistics, 2020, para.1). While three of the five identified ethnicities showed a slight increase in
the number and percentage of books that represented BIPOC, the increases were quite slight:
0.5% increase for books that had significant Black/African content (from 11.7% to 12.02%),
0.2% increase for First/Native Nations (from 1.0% to 1.2%), and 0.5% increase for Asian/Asian
Americans (from 8.5% to 9%). There was a decrease of 1.0% in the number of books that
89
significantly represented Latinx students (from 7.3% to 6.3%), and 2019 was the first year that
Pacific Islander was a category of itself, with 0.1% of the total number of books representing
students identifying in this category (The Numbers are In: 2019 CCBC Diversity Statistics,
2020, Percentage of Total Books Received by CCBC: About [US Publishers]).
The low numbers of books written by and for people of color are disconcerting given the
fact that no one-size fits all curriculum benefits all children (Crowe, Connor, & Petscher, 2009).
Our students of color, low socioeconomic students, and students for whom English is not their
first language continue to achieve at lower levels than our White middle-class students.
Culturally relevant texts are those texts that students can connect to on many levels, drawing on
their life experiences and background knowledge to create new meaning (Ebe, 2010). Yet the
number of books that are by BIPOC authors or about BIPOC main characters, which books could
contribute to increased individual interest and which would lead to “increased attention, greater
concentration, pleasant feelings of applied effort, and increased willingness to learn” (Krapp,
Hidi, & Renninger, 1992, p. 9 as cited by Worthy, Moorman, & Turner, 1999, p. 15), continues to
lag far beyond those books by White authors and about White main characters or about
non-human characters. In fact, in 2019, nearly three-quarters of the children’s and young adult
books published in 2019 featured white children or non-human characters (The Numbers are In:
2019 CCBC Diversity Statistics, 2020). The diversity of the students in our public schools,
however, and the diversity of their lived experiences and those experiences of their families, call
upon us to find as many books as we can that reflect our students in positive ways. Keis (2006),
cited Rich (1986) by stating the following:
90
When those who have the power to name and to socially construct reality choose not to
see you or hear you . . . when someone with the authority of a teacher describes our
society and you’re not in it . . . the experience becomes a moment of psychic
disequilibrium, as if you looked into a mirror and saw nothing. (p. 199)
Finding and tapping into the assets that our students bring to the classroom everyday, and finding
and intentionally using high quality, culturally relevant texts in the classroom, is to create an
opportunity for our students to become engaged and motivated to learn more, and ergo to grow
and flourish, because they will be validated, seen, and heard (Keis, 2006; Ladson-Billings, 1992;
Ladson-Billings, 1995; Troyer, 2018).
Conclusion
The act of reading is quite complex, situated in cognitive, social, linguistic, and
socio-cultural contexts between the reader, the text, the context, and the situation. To further
exacerbate the complexity, the United States has been long grappling with the ugly reality of
reading achievement disparities between students of the dominant culture of White, western
European, middle class culture and those students who are not of that culture - in particular,
African American, Latinx, students of limited economic means, and students for whom English
is not their first language. Quite often, one student can identify in all of those categories. Further,
the review of the literature established that a crucial period of reading proficiency is grades 3 and
4; if a student is not proficient in their literacy skills by that time, they continue to fall further
behind. Further, the landscape of literacy and texts continues to change with fast-paced advances
in technology.
91
The goal of reading is to understand what is read; in other words, comprehending the text
- in order to create new knowledge. Reading is fundamental to successful achievement in school.
Therefore, it is imperative that education researchers and practitioners alike fully understand the
barriers that keep students from successful comprehension, and create inviting, engaging, and
affirming environments, opportunities, and interventions, both offline and online, that eliminate
those barriers. In doing so, students will see themselves as strong and competent learners.
In that light, it was the goal of this research to study a particular intervention, the
paraphrasing intervention, with students in grades three and four in an online environment, and
the effects it has on student engagement and efficacy, as well as their comprehension of the
informational texts they were provided as part of this research. The specific rationale for and
methodology used in this study is discussed in Chapter Three.
92
CHAPTER THREE: METHODOLOGY
No research is ever quite complete. It is the glory of a good bit of work that it opens the way for
something still better, and this repeatedly leads to its own eclipse.
-Mervin Gordon
Introduction
The purpose of this mixed-methods research study was to explore specific components of
reading comprehension with regard to the role of texts and reading digitally. This study
researched the impact of informational texts on reading comprehension skills. Specifically, the
study looked at essential characteristics of informational texts with regard to comprehension
intervention in an online format for students in grades three and four who were at-risk of reading
difficulties or already struggling in reading comprehension. More precisely, the researcher
examined those characteristics of informational texts utilized in an online comprehension
intervention that lead to greater student engagement with the texts, and can also lead to
comprehension success.
This design-based implementation and mixed-methods study explored reading
comprehension and the role that texts and new literacies play in answering the central research
question:
What are the essential characteristics of informational texts that can be used for training
reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading comprehension skills of
diverse third and fourth grade students?
This research study was further guided by the following two sub-questions:
93
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
This chapter introduces and reviews the methodology used for the study. The description
and discussion of the methodology used in the study will center on each research question. This
review of the methodology includes the rationale for the study; the research paradigm; the
research setting and participants; overview of the data, including the methods and tools used for
data collection; methods for analysis of the data; and ethical considerations, biases and
limitations.
Rationale for the Study
While public education has had many goals since its inception in the early nineteenth
century, one goal has remained consistent throughout the tenure of public education: to provide
access to and equality of opportunity (Spring, 2018). To that end, early success in reading is a
strong predictor of success in high school (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Sparks et al, 2014).
The goal of reading is to understand what is read - in other words, comprehending the text - in
order to create new knowledge. Yet many students in the United States, and in Minnesota, where
this study took place, continue to struggle in reading. Their struggle is evidenced by stagnant
94
reading proficiency scores over the last decade. Only one-third of fourth graders have met
grade-level proficiency on assessments designed specifically to analyze trends and growth
(National Center for Education Statistics, 2015; 2019), despite increased time dedicated to
standardized assessment of students’ ability to comprehend texts. Additionally, a significant gap
persists for fourth grade students based on race and socioeconomic status (National Center for
Education Statistics, 2019). Further, the results on state and national reading assessments indicate
that students in the United States, and in Minnesota, continue to struggle to comprehend
expository texts, which are used in content-specific instruction as students move up grade levels
(National Center for Education Statistics, 2019; Minnesota Department of Education, 2020).
Much instruction at these grade levels continues to focus on more generic and low-level
strategies to promote learning from texts, including recall of information or inferring word
meanings from surrounding content (Goldman, et al., 2016). Students often do not have the
requisite prior knowledge needed to comprehend expository texts fully, yet prior knowledge is an
integral component of reading comprehension (Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016). Given these factors,
and the results currently realized in reading achievement as evidenced by state and national
reading assessments (National Center for Education Statistics, 2019; Minnesota Department of
Education, 2020), there is a great need for explicit and effective reading comprehension
instruction utilizing texts of all genres at the elementary level, beginning in the early grades; and
for a strong system of comprehension intervention for students who are struggling with this skill.
Furthermore, expository texts are necessary for comprehension and new learning in content areas
such as social studies and science; therefore, identification of essential characteristics of
95
expository texts will further inform instructional practice as teachers continue to make decisions
on what texts to place in front of their students.
Because of this need, there is currently a large national research study funded through the
Institute of Educational Sciences (IES) that is developing a comprehension intervention tool,
called the Interactive Strategy Training for Active Reading and Thinking for younger readers
(iSTART-Early). The tool will be a web-based reading comprehension strategy tutor that will
provide both instruction and opportunity for student practice. Once created, the tool will target
students in grades 3 and 4, two years in a student’s academic career that are critical for reading
development.
Because this small research study is one leg of the larger national study focusing on
grades 3 and 4, the target group of students for this study were third and fourth grade students
who were identified, by the district through normed and standardized testing scores, as reading
below grade level, and struggle to comprehend expository texts from which they are to acquire
new knowledge (Duke, 2000; Jeong, et al., 2010, Pearson & Billman, 2016). Although these
students struggle, they are maturing and many are becoming developmentally ready for
introduction of higher-order cognitive processes which have the potential to improve reading
comprehension (Del Giudice, 2014; Del Giudice, 2018). Additionally, there is an increase of
expository texts in the curriculum beginning at this age and grade level, texts from which
students are expected to gain new knowledge. However, as Duke (2000) and Jeong, Gaffney, and
Choi (2010) examined, the majority of classroom print prior to third grade is narrative and the
proportion of informational text is small, especially in schools with higher numbers of
low-income students. The time spent on reading instruction specifically focusing on
96
informational text, including time spent on informational text structure, is but a miniscule portion
of the student’s school day (Beerwinkle, et al., 2018; Duke, 2000; Strong, 2020).
Compounding this issue is that the time students spend engaging in informational text,
and the time spent instructing using informational text, may be miniscule compared to the school
day itself (Beerwinkle, et al., 2018; Duke, 2000; Ness, 2011; Strong, 2020). These texts are used
by students to acquire new meaning. However, the texts present challenges for which some
students are unprepared, including increasing text complexity, density of language, increased
length, lack of cohesion across text, and abstract concepts that require knowledge of the world
that students may not have (Chall, 1983; Jeong, Gaffney, & Choi, 2010). This lack of exposure
and preparation results in many fourth graders experiencing a decline in reading achievement,
beginning during the transition from primary to intermediate grades in elementary school, and
which continues into middle school (Best, Floyd & McNamara, 2008; Chall, 1983; Chall &
Jacobs, 2003; Chall, Jacobs & Baldwin, 1990; McNamara, et al., 2011; Wharton-McDonald &
Erickson, 2017). Further, this lack of exposure and preparation may also impede students’
learning of the content in areas such as science and social studies.
This researcher made the decision to focus the research in this study specifically on
science texts, and did so for two reasons. First, Minnesota adopted a new set of K-12 science
standards in 2019 which included changes that have significant implications on instructional
pedagogy, and for districts, initiates review of curriculum used in classrooms. Therefore, this
work is timely with the shift in the standards. Second, the district historically placed less
emphasis on science instruction in the elementary grades than instruction in literacy and math,
because the state requires annual assessment in those two content areas beginning in third grade.
97
However, less science instruction means less exposure to topics of interest for a variety of
students, especially students of color (Lee, 2020), and less exposure to content-specific
vocabulary needed to successfully navigate science texts. As was discussed in Chapter Two, the
reading of various texts provides students with the ability to gain knowledge of different topics,
which aids the students in comprehending more complex texts (Goodman, et al., 2017; Kendeou
& O’Brien, 2016). Since there was less science instruction throughout the day and week, there
was less use of science texts for instructional purposes, and therefore less understanding by the
students of science concepts, which contributes to comprehension difficulties. The use of science
texts would provide participating students with another opportunity to read texts that focused on
science concepts.
Additionally, a large achievement gap persists in the reading proficiency between White
students and students of color (SoC), many of whom are multilingual learners and many of
limited socioeconomic means (National Center for Education Statistics, 2019). These factors,
through a review of the literature, contribute to reading difficulties, whether through language,
vocabulary, or prior knowledge. Finally, review of the literature uncovered a need for teachers to
better understand the comprehension skills needed to successfully read online texts and how
those skills may be different than those needed to successfully read printed texts. This
understanding will better equip teachers to help their students navigate through new literacies.
Additionally, this understanding will help teachers realize what assets struggling readers do have
and what skills they are still lacking, in order to make strong instructional decisions that lead to
greater reading comprehension in both printed and digital text formats.
98
Because students are becoming increasingly exposed to digital text, this researcher,
herself a former English language arts teacher and a current district administrator, built a strong
partnership between the school district used for this study, and a major research university
located in the same metropolitan area; this was a partnership that initially focused on the role that
inference has to reading comprehension in the younger grades. The major research university has
created online tools for improving inference-making in reading, which is a strategy known to
increase reading comprehension (Kendeou, McMaster, & Christ, 2016; Wanzek, et al., 2018).
The strong partnership between the district and the research university provided the opportunity
to design a study that would help to inform the creation of a new web-based comprehension
intervention tool titled Interactive Strategy Training for Active Reading and Thinking for young
developing readers: iSTART-Early, designed specifically for students during a very critical
period in reading development: grades 3 and 4. The iSTART-Early tool is currently being
developed and, once created, will focus on the comprehension strategies of comprehension
monitoring, paraphrasing, inference (prediction/bridging/elaboration), question asking,
explanation, and summarization. The researcher, who was a district administrator overseeing the
work of literacy in the district, spent the last several years disaggregating reading achievement
data in the district, and knew that these years were critical for student success in reading.
Therefore, she made the decision to focus this study on better understanding the role that the
texts themselves play in students’ reading success to help inform the types of texts that could be
utilized in iSTART-Early.
The iSTART-Early tool has a module sequence that was designed similarly to its
predecessor, iSTART, which was built for older students through funding from a national,
99
multi-year Institute of Educational Sciences (IES) grant. In iSTART-Early, there are five
modules planned for the tool. They incorporate six reading comprehension strategies
(paraphrasing, comprehension monitoring, question asking, elaboration, bridging, and
summarizing). The five modules are as follows: Ask It, which is an overview of comprehension
monitoring and question asking; Reword It, which incorporates the paraphrasing strategy; Find
It, which also works with paraphrasing; Explain It, which works on elaboration, bridging,
question asking, and paraphrasing, and Summarize It, which helps students with retelling and
explanation, and also includes work on main idea. The modules are presented in sequential
design, with the strategies building off one another. Paraphrasing is the first module in which
students generate actual responses to text (the first module requires only identification tasks).
This study will help to inform the larger research study and the eventual creation of
iSTART-Early by identifying essential text characteristics for texts that will be developed for the
iSTART-Early tool, especially in schools and districts that serve a higher proportion of
disadvantaged students, including students of color and English Language learner (EL) students.
Research Paradigm
In schools, most teachers use texts to teach reading and to teach content. Students
increasingly use texts to gain new understanding, beginning in earnest in third grade. Thus, it is
important to gather information from both users to ascertain the essential characteristics of an
informational text to help improve reading comprehension. Teachers, who plan lessons and units
using reading materials, must understand why the characteristics are important. Further, it is
100
imperative that researchers understand how students traverse through informational text. As
McMillian and Schumacher (2010) illustrated, relying on just qualitative or quantitative methods
can be insufficient to provide a complete picture for which the purpose of a study requires. Their
statement rings true for the purposes of this research study; both qualitative and quantitative data
was needed to help answer the research questions. This study analyzed teachers’ input on the
texts utilized in this study with regard to length, readability, structure, cohesion, interests and
topics (qualitative data), and analyzed students’ understanding of the texts through the strategy of
paraphrasing, which is the first module in which students would engage in the final
iSTART-Early intervention tool. Additionally, the students did so in an online format
(quantitative data).
The theoretical framework for this study was guided by the construction-integration (C-I;
Kinstch & van dijK, 1978; Kintsch, 1983; Snow, 2002) model of comprehension that provides a
more individualized understanding of reading comprehension, such as variance by age, skill,
format, and life experience (Fox & Alexander, 2017; Israel & Reutzel, 2017; Kintsch, 1998;
Paris & Hamilton, 2005). The C-I model provides a more balanced view of reader and text, and
provides an extremely important focus on context, which is a filter of sorts that people use to
view the world and make meaning (Kintsch, 1988). Construction-integration brings together the
reader, text, context and situation; the research design for this study provided the data needed to
ascertain a holistic view of the type of texts needed for a diverse group of third and fourth
graders who struggle with reading comprehension.
Because this study focused on the interaction of students with texts, this research was
viewed through the lens of pragmatism, which as Cresswell and Cresswell (2018) explain, arises
101
out of “actions, situations, and consequences rather than antecedent conditions” (p. 10). This
epistemological paradigm fits well in the theoretical framework of construction-integration with
regard to reading comprehension. Readers interact with a text in a certain situation; they bring
with them their lived experiences and other sources of knowledge that have helped to form their
knowledge base, which may or may not be accurate (Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Kendeou, et al.,
2016). Reading comprehension is the result of that interaction, those life experiences, prior
knowledge and knowledge bases, and the situation in which the reader is engaged.
To account for these complex interactions, this study used both Design-based
implementation research (DBIR; Fishman et al., 2013; Penuel, Fishman, Cheng & Sabelli., 2011)
and Mixed Methods research. Design-based research (Cobb et al., 2003) is a research design
with which Mills (2010) recommended increased use with regard to literacy. It fosters flexible
conceptions of literacy, especially digital literacy, and provides formative data that is
contextually situated. DBIR is an extension of design-based research that includes the active
involvement of those who will ultimately be implementing the strategy intervention – for this
study, it was teachers and students. This research approach involves iterative, collaborative
design of solutions targeting multiple levels of the system: design that is informed by ongoing
and systematic inquiry into implementation and outcomes. The four key principles of DBIR align
well with education research, especially to the persistent low achievement in literacy, because
DBIR includes the development and testing of ideas and innovations that can improve teaching
and learning (Penuel, et al., 2011). Paraphrasing Fishman, et al., (2013, pp. 142-143), these four
principles are:
102
Focusing in persistent problems of practice, especially from multiple
stakeholders’ perspectives (in this case, teachers, students, researchers);
Committing to a collaborative design that is iterative in nature (trying something
out, analyzing data, revising, implementing again);
Committing to developing both theory and knowledge of students’ learning in the
classroom, and the implementation of strong instructional practice through
systemic inquiry;
Committing to creating capacity for sustainable change in systems.
With DBIR, there is a common commitment to building theory and knowledge within the
research community (Fishman, et al., 2013). It is not enough to know that an innovative
intervention strategy is effective – it is also important to understand the key active ingredients
and the underlying mechanisms that are related to promise and sustainable change; this
ultimately informs practice in the classroom. For this study, there were two iterative cycles, a
number which was unknown at the beginning of the research as the cycles were continually
determined based on previous findings throughout the research phase of the study.
Mixed methods research (MMR, Johnson & Christensen, 2017) is the type of research in
which the researcher combines elements of qualitative and quantitative research approaches. This
research design can be very helpful in providing information in the way of themes, issues, and
other information that will be the focus of quantitative research (McMillian & Schumacher,
2010). A mixed methods approach is congruent with the principles of DBIR, in that the two
types of data obtained (qualitative and quantitative) can come from multiple stakeholders in the
study. In this study, the researcher used qualitative research to obtain data from practitioners
103
(teachers and literacy experts) regarding types and characteristics of texts that they would use in
literacy instruction with third and fourth grade students. That data was then used to create a set of
texts used in a research environment where students in grades 3 and 4 interacted with the texts
(reading and engaging in paraphrasing in an online environment, and answering questions
focusing on how they felt about the texts they read) whereby quantitative data could be obtained.
The researcher also gathered data on student participants’ reading attitudes and their basic
understanding of grade level science concepts, which data could be quantified and compared
with the paraphrasing results. Finally, the researcher interviewed each participant individually at
the end of the observation cycle to obtain information on what they remembered about the
strategy used, and also abou their own efficacy as a reader.
For the purposes of this study, the researcher utilized exploratory sequential mixed
methods design (Cresswell & Cresswell, 2018). She began with qualitative research to explore
the ideas, opinions, and views of practitioners. Analysis of that data informed the creation of
tools used in the quantitative phases of the study. This process involved the creation of the texts
used in the research, and also involved the students.
In the quantitative phase involving the students, this study used a single-group,
quasi-experimental design, utilizing a single group of students in a pre-/post design which,
according to McMillian and Schumacher (2010), is helpful in education research because it is
often impossible to create random assignment of students. Therefore, in this study, the students
were not randomly assigned to groups, and the original setting - the classroom and the summer
targeted services context - was preserved as much as possible, given the limitations that the
104
COVID-19 pandemic placed on the original plans outlined for the district summer targeted
services program.
By combining qualitative and quantitative data, the researcher augmented the quantitative
data obtained from the students by including the perspectives of the practitioners. This aided in
the development of a more thorough understanding of the relationship between the texts chosen
for a reading comprehension intervention and how the students actually engaged with them. This
fuller understanding could not be obtained through the use of only one method or the other. For
the purpose of this study, the researcher leaned on Johnson, Onwuegbuzie, & Turners (2007)
definition of mixed methods:
research . . . in which a researcher . . . combines elements of qualitative and quantitative
research approaches (e.g. use of qualitative and quantitative viewpoints, data collection,
analysis, inference techniques) for the broad purposes of breadth and depth of
understanding and collaboration (as cited in McMillan & Schumacher, 2010, p. 396).
A deeper discussion of the methodology for this study takes place by discussing the qualitative
and quantitative phases separately. To better understand the methodology, however, a discussion
of the participants and research setting was warranted.
Research Setting and Participants
Research Setting
The setting for this research study was a small, first-tier suburban district in Minnesota,
with a student population of approximately 4,100. The district was chosen for three reasons.
105
First, it is the district in which this researcher works as a district administrator. Secondly, this
district has a partnership with the large research university which is developing iSTART-Early,
the web-based reading strategy tutor. This research study helped to inform research on that tool.
Finally, because this study was also focused on essential text characteristics appropriate for
ethnically, linguistically and economically-challenged students, this district provided the
population necessary to carry out the research.
While small, the district is extremely diverse, with no one federal race category that
encompasses a wide majority of students. There are four elementary schools in this district that
serve a very ethnically, culturally, racially, and socio-economically diverse student body. At the
time this study took place, approximately 70% of the district’s elementary students were students
of color. The demographics follow in Table 3.1.
Table 3.1. District Demographics as of June, 2020
Federal Race Category
Percentage of District Students
American Indian
1.05%
Asian
5.45%
Black, Non-Hispanic
13.02%
Hawaiian/Pacific Islander
0.22%
Hispanic/Latino
39.71%
Two or More Races
10.37%
White
30.17%
106
Of the elementary students in the district, 60.62% qualify for free- or reduced-price lunch (FRP),
with the number qualifying for free lunch approximately four times the number qualifying for
reduced-price lunch. English Learners (EL) comprise 33.1% of the elementary students in this
district, and 15.33% of the student body qualify for special education services.
The demographics for the district’s third and fourth grade cohorts are quite similar to the
overall district demographics. Table 3.2 below outlines the demographics for the district’s third
and fourth grade students.
Table 3.2. Demographics of Third and Fourth Grade Students as of June, 2020
Federal Race Category
Percentage of District Students
American Indian
0.88%
Asian
5.96%
Black, Non-Hispanic
14.91%
Hawaiian/Pacific Islander
0.35%
Hispanic/Latino
39.30%
Two or More Races
11.05%
White
37.54%
63.86% of the students in this grade band qualify for free- or reduced-priced lunch; 35.96% are
English Learners (EL), and 17.02% of the students in this grade band qualify for special
education services.
The large research university that is involved with developing the online comprehension
intervention tutoring tool, iSTART-Early, has partnered with this district in the past, largely
107
because of the demographics of the students and the opportunity to conduct research that
contributes to the larger body of research on reading achievement of various demographic
groups. As discussed in Chapter Two, reading achievement continues to be largely predicted
along the lines of race and socioeconomic status, and thus research that focuses on achievement
of different demographic groups continues to be warranted.
The district’s reading achievement continues to be a persistent issue and, like the state
and the nation in general, continues to be largely predicted along the lines of race and
socioeconomic status. In 2019, elementary reading proficiency on the state standardized test at
grades 3 and 4 in the district was 40.7%, with a 30.5% proficiency gap between White students
and students of color. English language learners had the largest gap at 45.6 percentage points:
only 10.2% of the EL students were proficient, compared with 55.8% of the non-EL students.
With regard to socioeconomic status, 30.2% of the students qualifying for free- or reduced-price
lunch were proficient, compared with 61.9% of the non-FRP students, showing a 31.5 percentage
point gap. The smallest gap was realized in the area of special education, with a 29.1%
proficiency gap between students qualifying for special education services compared with their
general education peers: 15.6% of students on IEPs were proficient in reading compared with
44.7% proficiency for those students not identified for special education services (Minnesota
Report Card, 2020).
The research for this study took place during summer targeted services, known more
commonly as summer school. In this district, students are identified for summer targeted services
based upon achievement data in both math and reading, and are invited to participate in the
targeted services summer program. Summer targeted services are not mandatory in this district;
108
therefore, parent consent for participation is required. Parents can opt their child(ren) out of
summer targeted services.
As a result of this “opt-out” system, the number of students participating in the summer
targeted services program from any one school are not necessarily a direct proportion of the total
number of students in that building, by race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, or identified service
needs (e.g. special education or English Language [EL] services). A summer targeted services
coordinator, who is an in-district elementary teacher, was hired to work through the logistics of
the elementary summer programming and, in partnership with the classroom teachers,
communicated with families of those identified students in the spring, inviting their child(ren) to
participate in summer targeted services. This individual communication was especially important
in 2020, given the upheaval this district experienced as a result of a highly contagious virus.
Interruption to the Research Setting
The COVID-19 pandemic caused schools in this state to cease all face-to-face learning in
March of 2020, and turn to distance learning for the remainder of the school year. This was an
immense shift to educational practices in the district, as most elementary teachers had very little
experience in teaching from a distance. Prior to distance learning, the ratio of devices to students
was not one to one, and a large number of families in the district did not have devices suitable for
student learning in the home. Because the district did not have enough devices for every
elementary student, the district made plans to get a device to every household that needed one,
which meant that some students within one household shared a device for learning. Further, the
district identified households for which reliable access to the Internet, or any access to the
Internet, was an issue, providing hotspots it had on hand. Additionally, the district ordered as
109
many additional hotspots as was available from vendors. This type of technology was in great
demand across the state and country, and much of the technology needed for remote learning was
backordered. Many households in the district had limited or no connectivity to the Internet.
Prior to the COVID-19 pandemic and the shift to distance learning, the district had 532
students in grades K-7 registered for the 2020 summer targeted services program. However,
because of the pandemic, this district, like all districts in the state, had to revise their traditional
summer targeted services programs. The traditional elementary summer targeted services
program for the participating district for this study was five weeks in length, four days a week,
6.5 hours a day (which included breakfast, lunch, and recess). This district revised its traditional
model, creating a choice of elementary summer programming. The first choice offered was a
fully-online elementary program where teachers would be instructing and students would be
learning in a synchronous setting, meaning that the students would be online interacting with the
teacher and each other live through video conferencing (in this case, Google Meet) as a group.
This type of learning is the opposite of an asynchronous learning environment, where students
are engaging in online learning at different times with no instructor present, sometimes watching
pre-recorded videos of the teacher facilitating the lesson, interacting with students through
discussion boards or contributing their comments in others’ documents.
The second choice offered was an in-person option which could accommodate 14
classrooms of nine students each in grades K-4, to comply with mandated social distancing. The
district prioritized certain students for the in-person model of summer targeted services; the
coordinator of the program looked at those students for whom life circumstances, such as limited
internet connectivity, or lack of engagement in the distance learning from March through early
110
June, caused a greater interruption of their learning. The coordinator also obtained lists of those
students who did not engage in any online learning and just received physical learning materials
during the distance learning period, lists that were updated weekly by the elementary school
staff. She contacted each family individually to ascertain their preference.
A further change was in the length of the summer program. The program in both formats
was shortened to three weeks, rather than the typical duration in this district of five weeks. The
school day itself was shortened as well, from the typical 6.5 hour day to a four-hour day, which
included breakfast, a take-home lunch, recess, and a strong focus on socio-emotional learning for
the first hour of the day. However, the number of days a week remained consistent: a Monday
through Thursday format. Once these plans were finalized, the district summer targeted services
coordinator contacted parents and notified them of the changes in order to gauge their interest in
an online targeted services program versus an in-person summer program and to finalize
registrations.
Logistics and instruction for the two programs differed in several ways. Online
instruction was synchronous, meaning students and teachers were online at the same time for the
duration of each day. Students saw each other virtually; there were breaks built in, but no
breakfast was served. For the in-person program, there were up to nine students to a teacher, and
there were no support staff in the classrooms. Students were able to move about the room,
interacting with physical materials such as books and math manipulatives, and they were able to
interact with each other. In-person students received backpacks and their own school supplies,
which mitigated the need to share materials. In-person classrooms had instructional technology
at their fingertips, such as Smartboards, and teachers played calming background music during
111
periods of independent practice. Students had a scheduled recess each day with one class outside
at a time.
The modified summer targeted services environment was not something students were
accustomed to experiencing in school; furthermore, the students had not been in school
physically for three months. Stickers on hallway floors helped students maintain proper distance
needed for social distancing when outside of the classroom. Desks in classrooms were placed at
least six feet apart. Staff were given face shields to wear. A mask mandate was declared in the
state during the second week of the program, and the district required staff to shift to wearing
masks indoors at all times, and masks worn by students were strongly encouraged, but not
required. However, students were asked to wear masks when in less than a six-foot proximity to
another person, and adults in the buildings worked with students to help them wear the masks
properly. The state mask mandate declared by the governor presented a new challenge for
in-person instruction, as it was impossible for students to see teachers’ mouths as they talked.
Additionally, voices were more muffled, many students had a hard time keeping masks on, and
the masks presented a distraction when students were wearing them.
The change to in-person and virtual summer targeted services forced the researcher to
choose which group of students were to be studied, as the two environments would provide
vastly different learning experiences for the students. Therefore, for purposes of this study, the
researcher chose to utilize the in-person summer targeted services program in order to physically
observe the students interacting with the texts. The demographic breakdown of the 2020
in-person summer targeted services program participants is described below.
112
Overall and In-Person Student Participation in Summer Targeted Services
The district elementary summer targeted services program registered 255 students to
participate in both elementary summer programs (distance-learning and in-person). Students
identified for inclusion in this study were students participating in the district summer targeted
services program who had been identified, through district standardized assessment data, as
reading below grade level. Data for identification included a student’s overall Rasch Unit (RIT
score) on the fall Northwest Education Association (NWEA) Measures of Academic Progress
(MAP) reading assessment. Students whose RIT scores were below grade level mean on the fall
assessment were identified for inclusion in the summer targeted services program. Grade 4
students were identified through the use of results from both the MAP reading assessment and
preliminary results of the Minnesota Comprehensive Assessment (MCA-III) reading assessment
that is required beginning in spring of grade 3, but for which official results are not available
until late summer of the year in which it is given. Students who scored in the ranges of partially
meets or does not meet on the MCA-III were identified for inclusion in the summer targeted
services program.
Because of the COVID-19 pandemic, it was determined by the district that further
prioritization of students had to take place, due to limited space for in-person learning. First
priority for in-person summer targeted services went to English learners, students who were
designated as free- and reduced-price lunch students, and students who had been flagged by
teachers, administrators, and/or families as having struggled in distance learning. Participating in
summer targeted services programs in the participating district is not mandatory, meaning that
113
families may choose to have their children participate or not participate. Because of the
COVID-19 pandemic, the district offered a choice of in-person participation or distance-learning.
47.06% of the enrolled summer targeted services students participated in in-person learning.
Table 3.3 breaks down the enrollment by federal ethnic/racial designation, Limited English
Proficiency, and Free- and Reduced-Price status:
Table 3.3. Demographics of Student Participants in Summer Targeted Services
Federal Category
# of Participants
Percent of Total
American Indian or Alaska Native
1
.39
Asian
18
7.06
Black/African American
34
13.33
Hispanic/Latino
152
59.61
Native Hawaiian or Pacific Islander
2
.78
Two or More Races
13
5.10
White
35
13.72
Limited English Proficiency (LEP)
153
60.0%
Free- and Reduced-Price Status (FRP)
198
77.6%
Because this research study was part of larger, national research that will lead to the
creation of an online comprehension intervention tutoring tool for third and fourth grade
students, this study focused solely on students in grades 3 and 4; therefore additional analysis of
the data was warranted for those two grades specifically. Of the 255 total participants in the
summer targeted services program, 55 students were in grade 3 and 33 students were in grade 4.
Summer targeted services enrollment data specific to grade 3 and 4 was as follows:
114
Grade 3: of the 33 total students registered for both in-person and
distance-learning, four students dropped, and 14 of 29 remaining participated in
the on-site program, for a 48.3% on-site participation
Grade 4: of the 55 total students registered, 11 students dropped, and 18 of 44
remaining students participated in the on-site program, for a 40.9% on-site
participation
As a result of these numbers, there were three classrooms of students in grade 3 (one was a
combined grades 2 and 3 classroom due to space constraints) and two classrooms of students in
grade 4. Table 3.4 below delineates the participation in summer targeted services for these two
grades.
Table 3.4. Grade 3 and 4 Students’ Participation in On-Site Programming
Grade
# of Participants in
Person
# of Total Enrollment
3
18
44
4
14
29
* Four on-site drops constituted 10.68% of total enrollment
The research for this study made use of in-person participation only, as the researcher
wanted to physically observe the students interacting with texts, and wanted to interview the
students in person at the end of the quantitative phase of the study. Further, the researcher had
more control over the environment in which the student would be interacting with the online text.
Finally, the in-person environment provided the only opportunity for students to engage in
science texts that had been written based on the qualitative data provided by the teacher focus
groups with regard to essential characteristics of informational texts - in this case, texts written
115
about science topics aligned to third and fourth grade state science standards. Because the
students were attending school in person, the district technology department was able to batch
upload the Qualtrics tool created for this study onto each computer assigned to the participating
student.
In order to conduct this study, several steps were taken in preparation. First, this
researcher contacted the summer targeted services coordinator. Through conversation, she
explained the study and what effect, if any, it would have on teachers’ plans; she also obtained
the names of the teachers who were going to be teaching students in grades 3 and 4. Next, she
contacted those teachers and obtained permission to conduct the study in their classrooms,
explaining her methodology and timeline. All teachers eagerly granted their permission. The
third step included contacting district technology staff to ensure that the Qualtrics tool could be
loaded on to student devices, and that students would be using the same device every day (which,
of course, they were because of the limited sharing of materials due to COVID-19), and obtained
confirmation that the district technology staff would load the texts onto the specific
Chromebooks to which individual students were assigned and that students were using in the
in-person environment. The final step was to communicate with the teachers and finalize a
schedule whereby the research could take place with no disruption to the teacher or classroom
schedule. This research would take place within two of the three weeks of summer targeted
services, after the teacher established a community of learners and students settled into in-person
learning, something they had not engaged in since mid-March.
116
Demographics of In-Person Student Participation in Summer Targeted Services
There were 255 elementary students registered for summer targeted services in the
district in 2020. Of those 255 students, 120 of them, or 47.06%, opted for in-person learning,
which is less than half of the total elementary students enrolled. From that pool of 120 students,
17 were dropped; either they never attended or were dropped from the roster for various reasons
once summer targeted services began.
Table 3.5 below shows the demographic breakdown of the total number of students
participating in the district in-person elementary summer targeted services program.
Table 3.5. Demographics of In-Person Student Participants in Summer Targeted Services, June,
2020
Federal Category
# of Participants
In Person
% of Total in
Person Enrollment
% of Total
Enrollment
(both Distance
and In-Person)
American Indian or Alaska
Native
0
0%
0
Asian
7
5.83%
2.75
Black/African American
16
13.33%
6.27
Hispanic/Latino
77
64.17%
30.20
Native Hawaiian or Pacific
Islander
0
0%
0
Two or More Races
2
1.67%
.78
White
18
15.0
7.06
Limited English Proficiency
(LEP)
76
63.3%
29.8%
117
Free- and Reduced-Price
Status
103
85.8%
40.4%
Human Subject Review
Institutional Review Board
For the purposes of this study, three Institutional Review Board (IRB) approvals were
required. The first IRB was with a large research university that will utilize the data and results
from this study in their federally-funded research project to develop the final comprehension
intervention for use with third and fourth grade readers. The district in which this study took
place had an existing IRB with the large research university. However, because the data and
results of this study will be used in the larger research project, this researcher was required to
obtain IRB approval with the large research university, which was obtained after the required
training with that institution. This IRB dovetailed with the approved IRB obtained by the large
research institution for future creation of the final online comprehension tutoring tool. The
second IRB approval was with the school district itself. The third IRB approval was through the
university in which this researcher was earning her doctorate in education.
Because this research study was an iterative mixed-methods study, it is best to discuss the
methodology used for data collection in two phases. The first phase, helping to inform the first
research question, concentrated on text creation.
Study 1: Data Collection with Teachers To Inform Text Creation
Stated previously, this was a mixed-methods study utilizing design-based implementation
research (DBIR). Therefore, data collection followed iterative cycles, beginning simultaneously
118
with the gathering of qualitative data from the teacher participants and the creation of an initial
corpus of texts that would be used to guide the discussion. This discussion, and the creation of
the texts themselves, contributed to the exploration of not only the first research question of this
study, but also of the first sub-question for this study:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion, topics) for
the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching reading comprehension
strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation, summarization, and question asking, in
a digital format?
It was imperative to the researcher that she begin this research with a strong understanding of
what teachers and literacy experts believe are important characteristics of texts used in literacy
instruction. Qualitative research, according to Cresswell and Cresswell (2018), “is an approach
for exploring and understanding the meaning individuals or groups ascribe to a social or human
problem” (p. 4).
To assist in answering the first research question, this mixed-methods approach provided
evidence, utilizing quantitative data from the creation of texts to gather qualitative focus group
data from the practitioners themselves - the teachers and literacy experts in the district. Their
work relies on high-quality intervention and their work will be more deeply informed by
understanding how those interventions work with students in order to create sustainable change
in practice and, ultimately, in student reading achievement. The voice and expertise of this cadre
of teachers were needed to guide the creation of the texts that would ultimately be used with the
students who participated in this study. The set of short texts that a team of researchers created
119
for the focus to read prior to meeting was necessary to ground the discussion in common texts
that all could discuss.
Teacher Participants in the Research Study
Design-based implementation (DBIR) research includes involvement of all those who
will be involved in implementation. Thus, it was imperative to gather qualitative data from the
teacher practitioner perspective on informational texts to be used with students. This researcher,
therefore, sought to bring a group of teachers together from the district in which this study was
conducted to form a focus group.
Specific criteria for participation in this study required any participating teacher to have a
background and school role related to literacy and instruction, to be between the ages of 18 and
75, to be an employee of the district in which the study took place, and to currently be providing
classroom instruction or reading intervention to children in grades three or four, or providing
literacy coaching to teachers. The researcher, through email communication, invited all
qualifying current grades 3 and 4 teachers, elementary instructional coaches, and elementary
reading interventionists in the district to be part of the focus group. This researcher was looking
to recruit approximately 8-12 members. Participation in the focus group was voluntary, and was
communicated very clearly as such. The problem that would be discussed during the focus group
meeting was the low reading achievement of the students in the district, and the particular focus
was on comprehension of informational text and characteristics of informational text that can
enhance or impede comprehension, given the review of the literature outlining the need for
explicit reading instruction using informational text.
120
A total of eight staff volunteered to be part of this focus group: seven staff identified as
female and one staff member identified as male. The eight volunteers were then sent a Google
Form with questions that were created to ascertain demographic information, level of education
obtained, and number of years of service, as well as their role in the district. To reiterate, the
adult participants for the teacher focus group consisted of teachers, reading interventionists, and
literacy experts in elementary schools in the district.
One teacher focus group participant identified as a person of color and Hispanic. The
other seven participants identified as White. All participants had been teaching in the district for
at least three years. Three of the focus group participants were fluent in the Spanish language,
with one focus group participant being a native Spanish speaker. The teachers were verified as
teachers of record by the researcher through contact with the district’s Human Resources
Department. Further, all participants were teachers the researcher knew well and with whom she
interacted on a regular basis.
Prior to the first virtual meeting, the researcher sent, via email, initial questions with
regard to demographics, years in education, and educational attainment of those who were
participating in the teacher focus group. The results of this questionnaire are found in Appendix
A. The use of email was appropriate for this part of data gathering, because the use of email is
commonplace in the district for communication that is important, but not urgent. All
communication to the focus group members prior to the first meeting was done via email. The
work of the teacher focus group would concentrate on characteristics of texts, helping to answer
the first research question, What are the essential characteristics of informational texts that can
be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
121
comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? The group provided data which
was used to create informational texts that were ultimately used by the student participants in this
study. In order to gather that focus group data, however, an initial corpus of texts were created to
be read and used in the focus group discussion.
Focus Group Materials
In preparation for the work of the focus group, a set of short texts were created by a
group of writers hired by the large research university which is developing iSTART-Early. For
the purposes of this study, the texts created were informational texts focusing on science
concepts taught at grades 3 and 4. These science concepts were identified and chosen by the
researcher, who is a district curriculum administrator with 25 years of experience working with
state and local standards. These concepts also aligned with the 2019 state academic standards in
Science. An example of such a standard in third grade is the following:
3L.4.2.1.1 Obtain information from various types of media to support an argument that
plants and animals have internal and external structures that function to support survival,
growth, behavior, and reproduction . . . Examples of structures may include thorns, stems,
roots, colored petals, heart, stomach, lungs, brain and skin. Examples of media may
include electronic sources (Minnesota Department of Education, 2019, p. 15).
Once the standards-aligned concepts were identified, the commissioned writers then began
creating an initial corpus of expository texts based upon what was deemed essential in terms of
content, themes, structures, and interest. There were six general, standards-aligned scientific
122
themes outlined, with approximately 9-12 texts written under each theme, for a total of 67 texts.
All 67 texts written were analyzed through the use of a Natural Language Processor (NLP) tool.
Natural Language Processing (NLP) Analysis of Texts
To further explore both the first research question, What are the essential characteristics
of informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students and to
continue to explore the first sub-question, “What are essential text characteristics (length,
readability, structure, cohesion, topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used
for teaching reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?” a set of texts with different levels that
were appropriate for this study were created. To do this, a Natural Language Processing (NLP)
tool with pre-populated indices was used. The tool used was the Coh-Metrix Common Core Text
Ease and Readability Assessor, or T.E.R.A. (Jackson, Allen, & McNamara, 2016), a
computational tool that uses natural language processing to produce indices of the linguistic and
connected discourse representations of a text. Written text is considered discourse because its
ideas are unified, it has purpose, and is connected logically (van Dijk & Kintsch, 1983).
The Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. analyzes a text on five components: narrativity, syntactic
simplicity, referential cohesion, deep cohesion, and word concreteness (Jackson, Allen, &
McNamara, 2016). The Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. tool informs writers (and teachers) of which
components of a text are less cohesive, so that writers can revise the texts to make the reading
easier, and teachers can work with students to help them recognize parts of texts that are less
123
cohesive and to build skills to overcome the obstacles that are naturally found in less cohesive
texts. In addition, T.E.R.A. utilizes Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level Readability formula to identify a
Flesch-Kincaid score (FK), which is a single dimension of text difficulty relative to grade level
or the reading level of the student. This score can be very useful when assigning texts to students
with varying reading levels (Graesser, McNamara, & Kulikowich, 2011; Jackson, Allen, &
McNamara, 2016). The Flesch-Kincaid Reading Ease score is determined by sentence length and
word length (numbers of letters in the words).
The Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. calculates a number of linguistic indices related to various
aspects of language that can be used to determine the quality, readability, or other specific
properties of a written or spoken text. The system analyzes multiple levels and factors of texts in
order to provide a multi-dimensional perspective of the text. The Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. selects a
set of those indices relevant for classroom texts. To illustrate, Coh-Metrix measures simple
indices such as word frequency and sentence length as well as more complex indices, such as
cohesion, and syntactic simplicity and complexity. Cohesion refers to the way the words within a
sentence, and sentences themselves tie together (Jackson, et al., 2016). Syntactic simplicity and
complexity refers to numbers of clauses, numbers of words in sentences, and the number of
words used before the main verb in the sentence. The lower these numbers, the higher the
Coh-Metrix score, and the easier the text is to read (Jackson, et al., 2016).
Coh-Metrix has been used to analyze texts for multiple educational tutoring systems and
for a variety of purposes (McNamara et al., 2014). Though there are a multitude of indices
produced by this tool, not all were relevant to this study. All indices relevant to reading
comprehension are listed in Appendix B, but for the purposes of this study, six were determined
124
by the Principal Investigator of the iSTART-Early project and the university researchers to be
appropriate for use. They include narrativity, syntactic simplicity, referential cohesion, deep
cohesion, word concreteness, and the Flesch-Kincaid reading ease score (RDFRE), to allow for
the eventual adaptability of the web-based comprehension intervention tool. The data on the
complexity of each text used for this study is outlined in Appendix C.
The Coh-Metrix tool provided information on all of the texts in order to determine which
of the corpus of texts were appropriate for this study. Of the 67 texts written, 23 were ultimately
chosen to bring to the focus group, based upon having a Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level Readability
score of <6.0, meaning that all texts were at or below a fifth grade reading level. Those 23 texts
were sent to the focus group ahead of the focus group meeting. The participants of the focus
group were asked to read through the short texts in preparation for the meeting.
Focus Group Discussion
Prior to the group meeting, the researcher prepared questions (see Appendix D) to help
facilitate the discussion. Those questions formed the foundation of the focus group conversation,
with additional questions being asked based on the context of the discussion. The group
members had widely varying educational experiences and backgrounds; therefore, it was
important to the researcher that the conversation felt organic and that the members could add to
and build upon one anothers thoughts. The discussion was focused on the texts with which
students typically engage in the classroom.
Because of the COVID-19 pandemic, the focus group took place virtually, using Google
Meets. The meeting lasted approximately 65 minutes and was recorded. Additionally the
125
researcher took observational notes as the focus group conversation unfolded. The researcher
acted as facilitator.
Members of this group worked together on identifying the core parameters for
informational text development and selection. Creating and using a set of guiding questions for
discussion, and keeping questions open-ended, invited the group members to build off of each
others’ thoughts. Leaving room for spontaneous follow-up questions based on answers given
provided a better understanding as to how the teachers and literacy experts viewed texts and
instruction, and what they valued in the texts themselves. The focus group helped to formulate
the parameters that encapsulated the essential characteristics of informational texts: content,
themes, text structures with regard syntactic structure (e.g. sentence length and placement of
words within a sentence), the interests of the students, and the developmental appropriateness of
such texts with regard to third and fourth graders. Scientific themes for the texts themselves were
determined in tandem with text characteristics.
Knowing the curriculum and the diversity of the population of students in the district,
including their ethnicity, socioeconomic status and native language, the focus group confirmed
the texts’ alignment to grade level standards that had previously been chosen by the researcher,
and also whether the topics of the texts were engaging to students in the third and fourth grade
age group, based on the group’s prior experiences with the variety of students in that age group.
After the meeting ended, the recording was transcribed and coded for broad categories
and themes. The focus group meeting was recorded on Google Meet, a transcription was created
from that recording, and notes were taken during the discussion. The qualitative data collected as
126
a result of the group discussion was coded authentically for themes. No themes were identified
by the researcher prior to coding.
The researcher identified overarching categories of the focus group responses specifically
with regard to feedback on the created texts. This information was sent to the team ultimately
responsible for creating the comprehension intervention tool, and was also sent to the team of
writers responsible for creating the texts for this study. The cadre of writers utilized the feedback
and made ample revisions to the texts. hence the iterative nature of this phase of qualitative
research.
Once revision was done, the texts were again scored using the Coh-Metrix Common Core
Text Ease and Readability Assessor (T.E.R.A., Jackson, Allen, & McNamara, 2016). The
decision was made by the larger research university to choose the topics of animals, outer space,
human body, and geological catastrophes. Once the topics were identified, the two texts with the
lowest Flesch-Kincaid scores per topic were chosen to be tested by the student population, for a
total of eight texts. The range of readability of those eight texts was 2.7 (second grade, seventh
month) to 5.5 (fifth grade, fifth month). See Appendix C for a detailed analysis of the eight
texts.
The eight texts were loaded into Qualtrics, which is an online tool used for many
purposes, including surveys, evaluations, and higher education research. They were presented to
the students in order from easiest to most difficult. However, before the students interacted with
the texts, it was paramount to collect data from the students to better understand the students'
attitude toward reading in general, and their basic understanding of grade level science concepts.
127
This data would be triangulated with the data collected from the interaction with the texts in
order to help answer not only the main research question but the sub-questions as well.
Study 2: Data Collection with Students To Inform Text Development
The main objective of this research study was to identify essential characteristics of texts
that can be used to train reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
comprehension of a diverse population of third and fourth grade students. To guide that research,
a third sub-question was created:
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
The term diverse, like many words in the English language, has multiple meanings. For the
purposes of this study, the researcher used the Cambridge Dictionary’s first definition of the
term: “including many different types of people or things” (diverse, n.d.). This research study
focused on students of color (SoC) and English Language Learners (ELs) who were below grade
level in reading, as their reading achievement is continually lower than those of their White,
native English-speaking peers (National Center for Education Statistics, 2019; Minnesota
Department of Education, 2020). The students who participated in this research study are
discussed in the next section.
128
Student Participants in the Research Study
The iSTART-Early intervention tool, once created, will focus on comprehension
intervention for students in grades 3 and 4. Because this study’s results contributed to the corpus
of research that informed the ultimate creation of iSTART-Early, the students participating in this
study were required to be in grades 3 or 4. There were two classes of grade 3 students, two
classes of grade 4 students, and one class of grades 2 and 3 students combined, which was due to
the limits to class size. Children with significant cognitive disabilities, sensory impairments
(blind/visually impaired or deaf/hard of hearing), or children who did not have sufficient English
language skills (below WIDA Level 3) to be able to engage with English texts and respond to
instructions given in English were excluded from the study. All student participants were
identified by the district as achieving below grade level proficiency in reading at grades 3 and 4.
This researcher visited every classroom, introduced herself and the purpose of the study,
and handed out the Informed Consent to Participate forms, which were written in English and
translated into Spanish. Of those 33 students, 19 obtained parental consent to participate in this
study, for an initial 57.6% participation rate. The demographic breakdown of students is shown
in Table 3.6 below.
Table 3.6. Research Study Student Participant Demographics
Federal Category
Grade 3
Grade 4
Gender
Male
3
4
Female
6
6
Ethnicity
Asian
1
0
Black/
0
1
129
African American
Hispanic
8
9
EL Status
Y
8
8
N
1
2
Free/
Reduced-
Price Lunch Status
Free
7
8
Reduced
0
2
None
2
0
Throughout the duration of the on-site research, student absenteeism was an issue. While
the number of students originally providing consent to participate was 19, that number slowly
dwindled throughout the week. Data collected for the purposes of this study was not consistent
across the entire population of students, with only 11 students attending the final day of the data
collection to interview on their experiences in the study. As this was a mixed-methods study
utilizing design-based implementation research (DBIR), data collection followed iterative cycles,
and both teacher data and student data was needed. It was important to understand the role
student efficacy as it relates to reading, as research continues to show that reading self-efficacy is
both a contributor to and a predictor of successful reading comprehension (Guthrie, et al., 2007;
Ortlieb & Schotz, 2020; Solheim, 2011). Therefore, this study used two tools to gain a deeper
understanding of what the participating students believed of themselves when it came to reading.
The first tool used was a reading attitude survey.
130
Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Data Collection
Students’ attitude toward reading has an effect on reading achievement (McKenna,
Conradi, Lawrence, Jang, & Meyer, 2012; McKenna, et al., 1995; McKenna & Kear, 1990;
Petscher, 2010; Smith, 1990, Varuzza, et al., 2014; Wade, 2012; Walberg & Tsai, 1985). Further,
a review of the literature showed that there is a difference in reading attitude based on gender and
language, two important areas of interest for this study (Akbari, et al., 2017; Logan & Johnston,
2009; Martínez, et al., 2008; McKenna, et al., 2012; Mohd-Asraf & Abdullah, 2016). Therefore,
it was prudent to collect data on the participating students’ attitudes toward reading.
To collect reading attitude data, the researcher administered to each participating student
the Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS, McKenna & Kear, 1990; 1999), which is a tool
that measures both recreational and academic reading attitudes. This instrument was chosen for
this study for several reasons. First, it has a large normative frame of reference; it is valid and
reliable, with reliability coefficients ranging from .74 to .89; of the 18 coefficients computed, 16
were at least .80 (Martinez, Aricak & Jewell, 2008; McKenna, Kear & Ellsworth, 1995;
McKenna & Kear, 1990). The ERAS is a 20-item pictorial rating scale which uses a Likert-type
scale of four possible responses, called nodes. The pictorial format uses the image of the cartoon
character Garfield the Cat, appealing to younger students, for the nodes. All items have a short,
simply-worded question about reading, which is followed by four pictures of Garfield ranging
from very happy to very angry, which pictorially represent feelings in the range from very
positive to very negative. Each mode is then assigned 1, 2, 3 or 4 points, with a 1 indicating the
most negative and a 4 indicating the most positive. The use of an even number of nodes
131
eliminates the possibility of a neutral, or non-committal response (MeKenna, Kear & Ellsworth,
1995). The survey takes approximately ten minutes to administer.
The 20 items are broken into two 10-item subscales measuring recreational reading and
academic reading attitudes. Therefore, each participating student received three scores: one for
recreational reading, one for academic reading, and one for total reading attitude. The assessment
provides general characterizations of reading attitude on those two dimensions (recreational and
academic) as well as a total reading attitude score. The scores were also reported as percentages.
The data was analyzed to find mean and standard deviation, in order to gain
understanding of the significance between the students’ attitudes toward the two types of
reading. As the review of the literature shows that girls have a more positive attitude toward
reading than do boys (Logan & Johnston, 2009; Martínez, et al., 2008; McKenna, et al., 2012;
Mohd-Asrat & Abdullah, 2016), this data was further disaggregated by gender. Once the
researcher administered the Elementary Reading Attitude Survey, she came back to the
classrooms on a different day to gather data on general science knowledge.
General Science Knowledge Data Collection
The researcher determined that a base understanding of student grade level science
knowledge was warranted. Due to time constraints with the students because of the altered
summer targeted services program day, and the suspension of the state standardized testing in the
spring, the decision was made to assess the students on their knowledge of grade level science
standards using a modified yet appropriate version of the fourth grade NAEP Science
Assessment (National Assessment for Educational Progress, 2009, see Appendix E), which was
132
created by the research team at the large research university overseeing the iSTART-Early
research. Questions on this pre-assessment correlated with the themes of the texts students read.
There were eight questions on the assessment, and students earned one point for each correct
response. The scores were reported as percentages, and analyzed to find mean and standard
deviation.
Again, the assessment took little time to administer. However, some students struggled
with reading the assessment. Since the goal of the assessment was an overall understanding of
students’ science knowledge, and not an assessment of reading skill, both teachers and the
researcher helped students who were struggling to read the assessment, which is not uncommon.
Of the 17 students who took the science assessment, four needed help reading some of the items.
This totaled 23.5% of the students taking the assessment. As is discussed in Chapter 5, this
finding has implications for future research and practice in the classroom.
The disaggregated data from both the ERAS and the science assessment were used
alongside the analysis of the students’ paraphrasing results, to better understand the students’
comprehension of the texts. To gather the paraphrasing data, it was next necessary to have the
students read the texts that were created for this study and use a strategy to paraphrase sentences
that would help the researcher better understand the students’ comprehension.
Data Collection on Text Reading and Paraphrasing
The central research question for this study focused on essential characteristics of
informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension of diverse third and fourth grade students. That question is
133
further guided by a sub-question focusing on specific text characteristics, including length,
readability, structure, cohesion, and topics that can be used for teaching reading strategies. To
gain a deeper understanding of the role of text characteristics in successful reading
comprehension, students engaged in reading and paraphrasing the texts created for this study.
The eight revised texts from the original corpus of 23 were loaded into Qualtrics, which
is an online tool used for many purposes, including surveys, evaluations, and higher education
research. This tool in particular was used because it could somewhat simulate the experience that
the ultimate web-based tutoring tool, once developed, will provide. As mentioned previously the
texts were loaded from easiest to most difficult; this is an important point to emphasize, as no
student got through all eight texts. The third and fourth grade student participants read these texts
during the summer targeted services day and paraphrased predetermined target sentences
throughout each text. The students typed their paraphrases into the Qualtrics tool at
predetermined times throughout each text. The sentences to be paraphrased were chosen by the
Principal Investigator for the iSTART-Early project, who is a nationally recognized expert in
reading comprehension, cognition, linguistics, and learning.
Prior to the students actually reading and paraphrasing the texts, the students needed to
learn a paraphrasing strategy. Because of the COVID-19 pandemic and resulting modification of
the summer targeted services program, this researcher had limited time with the students to teach
them a paraphrasing strategy. Therefore, the researchers at the large university created a short,
student-friendly instructional video that students individually watched on their computer and
could access at will throughout this portion of the research study.
134
The video was four minutes, eight seconds in length and used what Roehler and Duffy
(1984) explained as direct explanations of comprehension processes. The video explained, in
student-friendly language, what paraphrasing was and listed four strategies that one can use to
paraphrase. Further, the video provided modeling of each of the strategies and provided wait
time so students could think about the sentences and practice. This wait time provided the
students time to think about words they could use, prior to the narrator continuing the
explanation of the strategy. This researcher, along with the staff at the large university, deemed
that the length and the pace of the instructional video was appropriate for students this age.
After watching the video, the students then began to read subsets of the texts developed
and engaged in paraphrasing specific target sentences throughout each text. Understanding the
students’ engagement in the tasks was warranted, as the Construction-Integration (C-I) model of
reading illustrates that the act reading is a relationship between the reader, text, context and
situation (C-I; Kintsch & van Dijk, 1978; Kintsch, 1983; Snow, 2002). Therefore, observation of
the students actually reading and paraphrasing was critical to this study.
The researcher, using a modified observation form (Kendeou & McMaster, 2016, see
Appendix F), informally observed students watching the instructional video and reading and
paraphrasing the texts. This observation form provided quantitative data on the number of
observations made regarding four tasks in which the students engaged. The tasks identified for
this study included reading, paraphrasing, engaging in a vocabulary task (such as trying to figure
out the meaning of a word), or “other” - an act that did not have anything to do with the texts
themselves, including watching the instructional video. This quantitative data was analyzed to
135
find mean and standard deviation, and to gain understanding of the significance between
different tasks in which students were engaged.
Additionally, qualitative observational notes were taken on each student during each
session. The researcher took narrative notes on the behaviors of the students, including
frustration and how the student worked through that frustration, engagement, or off-task
behaviors. The quantitative data provided the researcher data on the students’ time spent on
various tasks; the qualitative observational data collected provided the researcher with real-time
narrative data on student behaviors, perceived attitudes, struggles, and engagement with the
tasks.
Observation of students engaged in reading and paraphrasing the texts took place over the
course of a week, spending approximately one hour at a time in each classroom at a time during
the students’ literacy block. During this time, those students not participating in the study were
engaged in other reading tasks, most often independent reading - either online or with a printed
book. This schedule provided the most cohesive environment for all students - on the surface, all
students looked engaged in a similar activity, which this researcher deemed to be important to the
overall experiences of the students. At the end of the study, participating students received their
choice of a pencil and eraser as a reward for their efforts.
During the reading and paraphrasing of the texts themselves, students were asked to rate
each text after they read it, as students typically like some texts more than others. To rate the
text, a short, student-friendly survey tool was created and placed at the end of each text. This tool
included a short phrase and an accompanying emoticon. This tool provided information on what
students thought about each text they read, and what they thought about paraphrasing. Students
136
were asked to rate the text for ease of reading and interest level (if they liked reading the text),
and ease of paraphrasing and interest level (whether they liked paraphrasing the sentences). To
do this, the following questions were asked:
Did you like reading the text passage?
How easy was it to read the text passage?
Did you like paraphrasing the sentences?
How easy was it to paraphrase the sentences?
Ratings were displayed on the screen both in written form and pictorially: for text ratings,
a green happy face stood for “I liked it!”; a yellow emotionless face stood for “It was okay.”; and
a red sad face stood for “I didn’t like it.” For paraphrase ratings, a green happy face stood for “It
was easy!”; a yellow emotionless face stood for “It was okay.”; and a red sad face stood for “It
was hard.” Students chose the icon that best matched their opinion. An analysis of the answers to
these questions provided additional information with regard to students’ interests, as ultimately
the web-based comprehension intervention tool, iSTART-Early, will need to be populated with
informational texts that match students interests, thus providing the opportunity for greater
motivation to read.
Next, the paraphrases themselves were analyzed in order to better understand the
students’ comprehension of the texts. The paraphrases produced by the students were scored
using a modified version of a rubric developed by McNamara and colleagues (2007a, see
Appendix G). This approach correlated with the first generative strategy used in the first module
of iSTART-Early, which is paraphrasing. This strategy provided information on how well the
137
students comprehended the texts. A rubric was used to score the paraphrases, and the raters were
trained on the rubrics.
The paraphrases themselves were first filtered for any responses that were irrelevant, too
short, or direct copies or clause reversals of the target sentences. Remaining paraphrases were
dichotomously coded for the presence of eight different factors: paraphrase presence, lexical
similarity, syntactic similarity, semantic similarity, elaboration, inaccuracy, and other content.
Finally, paraphrases were scored on a scale of 0-2 for overall quality: 0 indicating a poor
paraphrase, 1 indicating the presence of paraphrase strategies but a need for improvement, and a
2 indicating a high quality paraphrase. Two researchers coded all responses separately, and then
met to create inter-rater reliability, coming to consensus on each code in order to determine final
scores.
Finally, every student who participated in the study was individually interviewed by the
researcher, in person, at the end of the quantitative phase of the study. The researcher asked the
same three questions of each student participant, and recorded their answers on a form. The three
questions are as follows (asked in order):
Do you remember what paraphrasing is?
How did you figure out how to paraphrase the sentences?
Do you think paraphrasing helped you to read better? Why?
The length of each interview depended upon how well the student elaborated on each answer.
There were no follow-up questions. This researcher analyzed the qualitative data of each
response to provide additional information with regard to the impact of the study on each
student’s attitude toward their comprehension, their experience paraphrasing with an online
138
science text, and their reading self-efficacy. Their explanations were analyzed to determine
overall themes in the use and effectiveness of paraphrasing in this setting, and its impact on their
identity as a reader in general.
Data Analysis
This study was designed to study text characteristics that were essential for use in reading
strategy instruction, specifically strategy training that will involve the creation of a strong
comprehension tutoring tool for third and fourth grade students struggling in reading. This
research study aimed to answer the question, What are the essential characteristics of
informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? This
question is further informed by asking the following sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking in a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
To understand the role of informational texts used in comprehension intervention,
specifically with regard to the paraphrasing strategy used in this mixed-methods, iterative study,
it was important to gather and analyze multiple points of qualitative and quantitative data at
different points throughout the study, beginning with the texts themselves. Through the use of
139
nationally-recognized and research-based readability tools, texts were created and underwent an
iterative process of quantitative analysis to ensure proper readability for students.
Qualitative data was obtained from practitioner (teacher) focus group conversations and
analyzed for broad themes and categories that were brought back to the text writers in order to
revise the student texts and to create the corpus of texts that were included in this study and used
by students to read and paraphrase. More qualitative data was collected from the participating
students at the end of the study and is discussed below.
A broad understanding of the students themselves required obtaining quantitative data
on the students prior to their engagement in the texts and the paraphrasing activity. A short
science pre-assessment, using a modified but appropriate version of the fourth grade NAEP
Science Assessment (National Assessment for Educational Progress, 2009, see Appendix E)
provided data on students’ basic science knowledge. It was scored and analyzed to find mean and
standard deviation. The questions on this pre-assessment correlated with the themes of the texts
students read and paraphrased. The Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS, McKenna &
Kear, 1990; 1999) was administered to students that provided data on reading attitude, which
research shows has an effect on reading achievement (McKenna, et al., 1995; Petscher, 2010;
Smith, 1990, Varuzza, et al., 2014; Wade, 2012; Walberg & Tsai, 1985). The survey measured
recreational and academic reading attitude separately, and also provided a total reading score.
This data was analyzed for reliability coefficient, and was also analyzed for mean and standard
deviation.
Observational data was gathered while students read the short texts and engaged in a
paraphrasing strategy in order to gauge their engagement in the reading and paraphrasing tasks.
140
The observational data collected was both quantitative and qualitative. First, the system was
loaded onto each student computer, and students read the texts online, stopping to paraphrase
sentences. After each text was read, students answered a short set of questions about their
attitude toward the text and the act of paraphrasing. Using a modified observation form
(Kendeou & McMaster, 2016, see Appendix F), the researcher quantitatively observed the
students in four areas: reading, paraphrasing, engaging in some sort of a vocabulary activity
(such as trying to figure out the meaning of a word), or some other activity. This quantitative
data was analyzed to find mean and standard deviation, and to gain understanding of the
significance between the different tasks in which students were engaged. The researcher also
took narrative notes on the behaviors of the students, including frustration, engagement, or
off-task behaviors. This qualitative data was analyzed alongside the quantitative data from the
observation form to glean a deeper understanding of the student behavior in the four areas, such
as how a student worked through frustration when trying to figure out the meaning of a word.
The paraphrases themselves were scored using a coding rubric (see Appendix G).
Paraphrases were scored based on five criteria: whether or not the paraphrase was too short; a
copy/paste; a clause reversal; garbage, meaning it was gibberish; or irrelevant, meaning it had no
connection to the text. This methodology provided quantitative data from the results of students
engaged in paraphrasing short science texts and was analyzed for the length and quality of each
paraphrase, which correlated to the students’ understanding of and comprehension of the text.
Data was also obtained on how many texts students read and paraphrased.
The last data obtained was through a short, three-question interview with the final
participating students remaining in the summer targeted services program on their experience
141
with paraphrasing, which provided qualitative data on each student’s experience and
understanding of paraphrasing, and how they felt as a reader as a result of the exercise. While
short, the responses were still coded for themes. No codes were identified prior to the analysis;
rather, they were identified authentically as a result of student answers and analyzed student by
student.
All of this data were triangulated in order to identify emerging patterns and themes, such
as the relationship between the qualitative data (e.g. whether or not the student liked the texts and
paraphrasing, and whether or not they thought paraphrasing made them a better reader) and the
quantitative data (e.g. paraphrasing scores, science assessment scores, and reading attitude
survey) in order to understand if the texts were appropriate for the web-based comprehension
intervention tool. The researcher used the data not only to help answer the research questions,
but also to understand the relationship between the results of this study and the review of the
literature to determine recommendations for future practice and research. Data obtained through
this study, including paraphrasing scores, science assessment scores, and reading attitude survey
results, were also analyzed and used within the larger study protocol.
For all data collection involving students, each student was given a numerical
identification, to which all data, both qualitative and quantitative, was assigned. This provided
the researcher with the ability to analyze the data at both a group and individual level, taking into
account student demographics, such as gender, race/ethnicity, language, grade level, and
socioeconomic status.
142
Conclusion
The purpose of this chapter was to review and provide the rationale for the methods used
in this study, which was a mixed-methods research study to explore the role of informational
texts and specific components of reading comprehension while reading digitally. The
methodology chosen for this study delved into the impact of informational texts on reading
comprehension skills, and their essential characteristics with specific regard to comprehension
intervention in an online format for students in grades 3 and 4 who are at risk of reading
difficulties or already struggling in reading comprehension. This researcher examined
characteristics of informational texts that can lead to greater engagement in and comprehension
success with the reading content created for an online comprehension intervention, and taught
and used the paraphrasing strategy to determine that comprehension success.
A review of the literature detailed the fact that these two grades are a critical time in a
student’s literacy development, especially as demands on learning new content through reading
increase. Further, a detailed review of the literature confirmed that there is a continued,
pervasive, and large achievement gap between students of color, students of limited
socioeconomic means, and students for whom English is not their dominant language and their
White, middle class peers.
This study was part of a larger, federally-funded study to develop an online, automated
comprehension strategy tutor for students in grades 3 and 4, called the Interactive Strategy
Training for Active Reading and Thinking for Young Developing Readers, or iSTART-Early.
This tutor will ultimately support both teachers and students in the classroom, as teachers will be
143
able to assign texts and monitor students’ comprehension progress. Therefore, this study focused
on the text characteristics of informative texts that will ultimately populate this tool, and how
students interfaced with the informative texts in an online format.
A detailed description of the participants and setting of this study, and a detailed
description of the creation of the texts used in the study was warranted for this chapter, as this
study was conducted in a very diverse school district with a large population of students of color,
students living in poverty, and English Language learners; additionally, the district had a large
percentage of students scoring partially proficient on the standards or not meeting standards, and
the district has a large achievement gap between their students of color and English Language
learners and their White, middle-class peers.
Finally, and unexpectedly, this chapter outlined what the effects that the COVID-19
pandemic, an unparalleled event in American education, and an unanticipated event with regard
to this research, had on the study itself. A thorough analysis of demographic data, teacher and
literacy expert focus group data, and student reading data was undertaken in order to identify
emerging themes. Chapter Four provides an in-depth analysis of the data as it relates to results.
144
CHAPTER FOUR: ANALYSIS AND RESULTS
Our understanding of the human brain can be dramatically accelerated if we collect and share
research data on an exponentially wider scale.
-Tan Le
Introduction
The majority of students in grades 3 and 4, not only in the United States but in the district
in which this study took place, are not proficient readers as measured by national and state
standardized assessments (National Center for Education Statistics, 2019; Minnesota Department
of Education, 2020). These statistics are alarming, as this grade level band has been identified as
a critically important time period in young readers’ lives. They are being asked to learn content
from increasingly complex informational and expository texts, yet their exposure to and
instruction in these non-fiction texts has lagged (Beerwinkle, et al., 2018; Castles, Rastle, &
Nation, 2018; Chall, et al., 1990; Duke, 2000; Jeong, et. al., 2010; Strong, 2020). Further, as was
established previously through a review of the literature, there is an elusive and persistent gap in
reading achievement between White, middle-class students and students of color, students from
low socioeconomic families, and students for whom English is not their first language. The
researcher, herself being a former English language arts teacher and K-12 literacy director with
over 25 years in education, has witnessed firsthand students struggling to understand what they
are reading and has dedicated her work as an educator to eliminate reading achievement
disparities which are predictable across federal categories.
145
One of the ways this researcher has tried to increase reading achievement for students has
been to seek and foster relationships with research institutions. She had the opportunity to
collaborate with a large research university to provide data that would aid in the eventual
creation of a web-based comprehension intervention, called iSTART-Early. This study was
designed to answer the following question: What are the essential characteristics of
informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students?
Further, the main research question was guided by the following sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
This chapter presents the findings of the Design-based implementation research (DBIR)
and mixed methods research study designed to answer the research questions. Design-based
implementation research includes the active involvement of those who will ultimately be
implementing the strategy intervention which, for the purpose of this study, are third and fourth
grade teachers and their students. Data were collected in iterative phases. First, data collected
from the teachers were analyzed, and informed the creation of expository science texts that were
146
read by students. The data collected from the students were analyzed to answer the research
questions.
The central question, What are the essential characteristics of informational texts that
can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to improve the reading
comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? was answered through multiple
stages of research in this study. The first phase began with the initial creation of a corpus of texts
prepared for the teacher focus group discussion. This work was done through collaboration with
this researcher and researchers at the large research university. The data collected from the focus
group were used to revise the initial corpus of texts in order to create a final set of texts for use
by the students in this study. Below is a discussion of the findings of this phase of the research.
Study 1: Data Analysis To Inform Text Creation
In the first phase of the study, the researcher obtained qualitative data from a focus group
made up of teachers and literacy experts on their experience with and opinions regarding
informational texts appropriate for use in grades three and four for strategy training. The focus
group members worked on identifying the core parameters for informational text development
and selection. This group read informational texts created specifically for this study that aligned
with state grade-level science standards. Members of the focus group provided feedback on the
texts, including their appropriate use with third and fourth grade students. Data from a
quantitative analysis of the initial set of texts were used to create the final corpus of eight texts
used with students in this study.
147
Natural Language Processing (NLP) Analysis of Texts
The first stage of this study required that texts be created to populate a tool students
would use to read and show comprehension of the texts. These texts were created through an
iterative process. First, this researcher and several university researchers came together to
identify broad science themes that could be used to create texts aligned to state standards for
third and fourth grade. This researcher had strong knowledge of the state science standards and
was able to guide the university researchers through the standards to build understanding. Once
the topics were identified, writers created 67 initial texts based upon the identified science
themes. These writers were hired based upon their previous experience in writing, their previous
experience in a school setting with elementary students, or a combination of both experiences.
The initial corpus of texts written were analyzed by work count, Flesch-Kinkaid (FK)
scores, and Lexile range, as the developers of those texts knew that the texts would change as a
result of feedback. Those with FK scores greater than a score 6.0 were removed, as they would
have been too difficult for the students at these grade levels to read. This left a corpus of 23 texts,
which were presented to a focus group with eight members consisting of teachers, reading
interventionists, and literacy specialists. The group was asked to read the texts prior to the
convening of a virtual meeting to discuss the texts, and were given a week to read them. All of
the texts were short - approximately 400-650 words each. The virtual meeting took place via
video conferencing, in this case Google Meet, which was the district’s approved method of video
conferencing. Additional analysis of the 23 texts occurred after the focus group feedback. The
text titles and data on the 23 texts presented to the focus group are found in Table 4.1 below.
148
Table 4.1. Text Titles and Characteristics
Text Title
Word Count
Lexile Range
Flesch-Kincaid Score
Zoos & Humane
Treatment
363
610-800
5.9
Bioluminescence
310
410-600
5.3
Ostriches
358
410-600
3.7
Sloths
387
610-800
5.4
Starfish
397
410-600
3.8
Platypus
329
410-600
3.5
Avalanches
359
610-800
5.6
Quicksand
317
610-800
5.3
Wildfires
237
610-800
5.1
Flooding
384
810-1000
5.0
Using 10% of Brain
312
610-800
5.5
Eating Turkey
208
610-800
5.2
Eating Healthy
399
410-600
3.8
Blood
388
410-600
3.9
GroupThink
237
410-600
3.5
Venus
352
810-1000
6.0
Pluto
403
610-800
5.6
Meteors
372
610-800
4.8
The Universe
285
410-600
4.9
A Visit to Mars
361
410-600
2.8
How a Star is Born
243
410-600
2.8
149
Lightning
344
610-800
5.2
Magnets
231
610-800
5.3
Focus Group Qualitative Data Analysis
Because meeting as a group was not possible due to the COVID-19 pandemic, the
researcher convened the focus group via Google Meet. This video conferencing tool offers a
service whereby the meeting can be recorded; a copy of that recording is sent to the organizer of
the meet within a few hours after the meeting has ended. All teachers who volunteered to
participate were present in the meeting, they agreed to be recorded, and the meeting lasted
slightly over one hour.
During that meeting, participants discussed what they noticed in and about the texts. The
researcher had pre-planned questions (see Appendix D) to be used as conversation starters, but
additional questions were asked as a follow-up depending on the conversation that ensued, to
continue the engagement in the topic about grade level texts. This researcher recorded the
Google Meet and obtained a recording of it that she then transcribed. The transcription of the
meeting was analyzed for the emergence of overarching themes and categories, which were then
categorized. Table 4.2 below outlines the thematic categories, with the number of mentions for
each theme and percentage of total mentions for each theme, in descending order.
Table 4.2. Teacher Focus Group Themes
Category
# of Mentions
% of Total Mentions
Text Structures
67
30.7
Instructional Pedagogy
42
19.3
150
Engagement
31
14.2
Vocabulary
29
13.3
Connections
14
6.4
Self-Efficacy
14
6.4
Motivation
13
6.0
Prior Knowledge
8
3.7
The analysis of the focus group data determined broad themes. The importance of those
themes, as evidenced by the number of mentions, was valuable information which this researcher
brought back to the text writers. They used this information to revise the texts which would be
used in this study. It was obvious that text structure was important to the teachers and literacy
experts. Vocabulary was also identified as a strong theme. However, the original literature review
had little emphasis on vocabulary. Because of its importance in the focus group discussion, it
was imperative that vocabulary be included as a focus in the review of literature for this study.
Vocabulary, as will be discussed later, became a central issue with regard to student
comprehension of the texts.
This analysis of the focus group data was provided to the original team of literacy experts
who wrote the texts, and aided in their revision of the texts. During this time, the district’s
learning environment was hugely disrupted due to the COVID-19 pandemic, and there was much
interruption to the original parameters of this study. The end of the school year was altered
significantly and moved to distance learning. As a result, this researcher found it impossible to
reconvene the teacher focus group in its entirety, even virtually, as the teachers who participated
151
expressed feelings of stress and exhaustion after the events of the spring, and stated that they
needed to separate from their professional life as an educator. This researcher, in collaboration
with the team writing and revising the texts, determined that there was enough information
gleaned from the focus group discussion to move ahead with revision and finalization of the 23
Chapter three texts to create the cadre of texts that would be used by the students in this study.
Analysis of the Texts Created and Used
The writers revised the initial set of 23 texts based upon the themes gleaned from the
focus group data, especially with regard to text structure and vocabulary. Next, the texts were
further analyzed using reading indices. All reading indices related to reading comprehension are
found in Appendix B. For the purposes of this study, six components of the Coh-Metrix
Common Core Text Ease and Readability Assessor (T.E.R.A., Jackson, Allen, & McNamara,
2016) were used, five of which are described below. The sixth, the Flesch-Kincaid Readability
Score, has been previously described in Chapter Three. No text was included in the set presented
to students that had a Flesch-Kincaid Readability Score (FK) of >6.0. For each of the remaining
five T.E.R.A. components, generally speaking, the higher the score, the easier a text is to read
(Jackson, Allen, & McNamara, 2016). These five components are briefly described in Table 4.3
below.
Table 4.3. Components of the Coh-Metrix Text Ease and Readability Assessor (T.E.R.A.)
T.E.R.A. Component
Brief Description
Narrativity
Genre: how story-like the text is
Syntactic Simplicity
Complexity of sentences: use of clauses, number of words in a
sentence, number of words before the verb
152
Word Concreteness
Use of words for things one can see, hear, taste, smell, touch
Referential Cohesion
The overlap of words or concepts from one sentence or section of
a text to another
Deep Cohesion
How well the events, ideas, and information from the whole text is
tied together - the use of connectives, e.g. “after” or “because”
Although a corpus of 23 texts was created for this study, not all were used with the
student sample. Due to the COVID-19 pandemic, the summer targeted services program was
altered, and the length of time reduced. Therefore, the use of 23 texts was not feasible. Once
revision of the 23 texts was completed, the decision was made by the staff at the large research
university, and confirmed by this researcher, to choose four science topics and to include two
texts per topic. Those four topics were animals, outer space, the human body, and geological
catastrophes. They were chosen because of their alignment with the state science standards for
grades 3 and 4, and an understanding of general student interest at those two grade levels.
The two texts selected per topic were those with the lowest Flesch-Kincaid scores and the
highest Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. scores. The range of Flesch-Kincaid readability scores on the eight
selected texts was 2.7 (second grade, seventh month) to 5.5 (fifth grade, fifth month). The word
count for the texts ranged from 275-398; the number of paragraphs for each text ranged from
three to 16; and the sentence count for each text ranged from 20 to 39. Data on the eight texts
used with students in this study are found in Appendix C.
The Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. tool scored each text on the five previously mentioned
components: narrativity, syntactic simplicity, word concreteness, referential cohesion, and deep
cohesion. Each component earned a score out of 100 and is represented as a percentile; the
153
higher the score in each area, the easier the text was to read. Of the 40 scores earned for the texts
(five scores for each of the eight texts), there were 24 scores of 60 and above, and 16 scores of
59 and below. The mean score and standard deviation for each of the T.E.R.A. components are
found in Table 4.4 below.
Table 4.4. Mean Scores and Standard Deviation for Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. Components
T.E.R.A. Component
Mean
Standard Deviation
Narrativity
53.49
10.63
Syntactic Simplicity
80.14
13.22
Word Concreteness
70.71
21.18
Referential Cohesion
65.29
22.57
Deep Cohesion
54.43
28.64
Because all texts used for this study were expository science texts, the T.E.R.A. scores for
narrativity were predictably lower, with no text earning a score above 67. Therefore, the
narrativity component had the lowest mean score, and also the lowest standard deviation.
Of particular interest was the finding on the component of deep cohesion, which
measures how well the information of the whole text is tied together. This component had the
second lowest mean score, meaning that the texts used for this study had a lower number of
connectives and/or they did not incorporate other components that helped students make
inferences. However, this component also had the highest standard deviation, which meant there
was considerable variation on the deep cohesion of each text. Text 4 had a deep cohesion score
of 24, compared to Text 3, which had a deep cohesion score of 99. According to Jackson, et al.
154
(2016), in the case of informational text, “the lack of deep cohesion may pose challenges,
particularly for low knowledge readers” (p. 55). Further, according to Jackson, et al., a score for
deep cohesion is extremely informative for expository texts that may need more explicit cues to
help the reader make sense of the text. Further analysis of the texts coupling the T.E.R.A.
components with paraphrase data will be discussed later in this chapter.
In order to fully answer the first research question, What are the essential characteristics
of informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? the
students needed to interact with the texts by reading them and paraphrasing sentences to gain an
understanding of their comprehension. Therefore, once the analysis of all texts was completed,
they were populated into Qualtrics, and that tool was loaded onto each participating student’s
computer. The tool was created in Qualtrics in such a way that the participating students had to
complete reading, paraphrasing, and rating each text before they were able to move on to the
next text. Because of this particular format of the Qualtrics tool, the texts were ultimately
presented to the students in order from easiest to most difficult. In the next phase of the study,
qualitative and quantitative data were collected by the researcher on the students’ interactions
with the text.
Study 2: Student Data Analysis
In this second phase of the study, qualitative research focused on observation of students
reading and paraphrasing the texts, and a short interview was conducted with all remaining
students who participated in the research. As was discussed in previous chapters, and will be
155
discussed more thoroughly in the Limitations section of this chapter, there was an unforeseen yet
sizable interruption to the research setting due to the COVID-19 pandemic. This researcher had
fewer students with whom to work, and less time with the students. Therefore, the decision was
made to focus on one reading strategy from which to gather data - the paraphrasing strategy.
Paraphrasing is an effective comprehension strategy (Hagaman, et al., 2016; Hagaman, et
al., 2012; Kletzien, 2009), and can be taught effectively to younger students to help them monitor
and increase their comprehension (Kletzien, 2009, Pearson & Billman, 2016). Paraphrasing is
sometimes considered a form of summarization (Kletzien, 2009). However, it is a skill that can
be learned before a student learns the more complex and formal skill of summarizing, because
paraphrasing means putting content into one’s own words. This researcher taught her students to
remember the short adage, “Say it the way you’d say it!” Paraphrasing, like any comprehension
strategy or skill, needs to be explicitly taught to students. To do this, a short, student-friendly
instructional video on the paraphrasing strategy was created and loaded onto each participating
student’s computer. The students watched the video and could access it at will throughout the
paraphrasing process.
The quantitative research in this second phase was multifaceted, and included analysis of
the following:
pre-assessment data on the students’ reading attitudes
student results on a short grade level science assessment to ascertain base level science
understanding
observational data while students engaged in watching the instructional video, reading,
and paraphrasing texts
156
paraphrase data, which included the number and quality of students’ paraphrases
After students watched the paraphrasing strategy instructional video and read and
paraphrased the texts, data were analyzed to understand the effects of the paraphrasing strategy
instruction on the reading comprehension of the participating third and fourth grade students.
The next step in analysis was informed by the two sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion, topics) for
the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching reading comprehension
strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation, summarization, and question asking, in
a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a racially and
ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or (b) students who
read below grade level?
While the analysis of the text themselves, especially with regard to the six reading indices used
for this study, helped to inform the first sub-question, data were gathered from students while
they read and paraphrased the texts, which aided in answering the second sub-question.
In this phase of this study, students watched the instructional video on the paraphrase
strategy, and worked with the texts that were created in Qualtrics and loaded onto their
computers. They were asked to read short science texts, paraphrase specific, pre-determined
sentences, and rate each text and paraphrase activity before going on to the next text. Prior to
analyzing the paraphrase data, it was important to this researcher to better understand the
students in two particular areas: their attitudes toward reading, and their general understanding of
157
grade level science knowledge aligned to the texts created for this study. The next section
discusses the students who participated in this study.
Student Participant Data
There were a total of 33 students in grades 3 and 4 who initially registered to participate
in the in-person summer programming for this district. The researcher visited all third and fourth
grade classrooms, introduced herself and explained the research process and explained to each
class that they would need to obtain permission from their family in order to participate in the
research study. Providing the students with Consent to Participate forms, which were written in
both English and Spanish, the researcher explained that their parents or guardians would need to
sign the form, and they would need to bring the signed form back in order to participate.
Of the group of 33 total students, 19 students obtained permission to participate in this
study. There were 12 female participants; six were in grade three and six were in grade four.
There were seven male participants; three were in grade three and four were in grade four. All
students participating in this study were BIPOC (Black/Indigineous/Students Of Color). Three
federal race categories were represented in this population of participants. Asian (one
participant) and Black/African American (one participant) comprised 10.5% of the participants;
Hispanic (17 participants) comprised 89.5% of the group participating in this research study. Of
the participating students, 84.2% were identified by the district as English Language Learners,
and 89.5% qualified for free- or reduced-price lunch. Grade, gender, ethnicity, language, and
socioeconomic data on each student are presented in Table 4.5.
Table 4.5. De-Identified Student Data
158
De-Identifier
Grade
Gender
Ethnicity
EL
FRP
10001
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10002
4
M
Hispanic
Y
Reduced
10003
3
F
Hispanic
Y
None
10004
4
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
10005
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10006
3
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
10007
4
F
Hispanic
N
Free
10008
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10009
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10010
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10011
4
M
Black/ Af. Am
N
Free
10012
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10013
3
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
10014
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10015
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10016
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
10017
4
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
10018
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Reduced
10019
3
M
Asian
Y
None
159
Student Pre-Assessment Data.
Data were obtained from each student prior to students reading the science texts and
paraphrasing sentences. Of particular interest to this researcher was the students’ attitude toward
reading and their base knowledge of grade level science concepts, as this data align with the
review of the literature emphasizing the positive effect that engagement (DeNaeghel, et al., 2012;
Guthrie & Cox, 2001; Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; Reschly & Christenson, 2012; Skinner &
Pitzer, 2012, Unrau & Quirk, 2014; Varuzza, et al., 2014; Wigfield, et al., 2008) and prior
knowledge (Coiro, 2011a; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Kendeou & van den Broek, 2007; Lipson,
1982; McCullough, 2013; McNamara, et al., 2011) have on successful reading comprehension.
The data points from both pre-assessments were used in the analysis of the students’
paraphrasing results. All de-identified student pre-assessment data can be found in Appendix H.
This section analyzes the data from both pre-assessments, beginning with the participating
students’ attitudes towards reading.
Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS).
The review of the literature confirmed that attitude impacts learning (McKenna, et al.,
1995; Petscher, 2010; Smith, 1990, Varuzza, et al., 2014; Wade, 2012; Walberg & Tsai, 1985).
Because reading is such a fundamental component of school success, it is important to
understand all of the components that lead to reading success. A review of the literature has
shown that reading attitude can affect reading performance (McKenna & Kear, 1990; Petscher,
2010; Walberg & Tsai, 1985). Therefore, it was important to understand the reading attitudes of
the students participating in this research study.
160
To gauge a general understanding of the students’ attitudes towards reading, this
researcher administered the Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS, McKenna & Kear,
1990; 1999) on the second day she was with students. The Elementary Reading Attitude Survey
is a tool that measures both recreational and academic reading attitudes. This tool contains 20
items, each having four possible responses, called nodes, in a pictorial format that is appealing to
younger students. The use of an even number of nodes eliminates the possibility of a neutral, or
non-committal response (MeKenna, Kear & Ellsworth, 1995). Each mode is then assigned 1, 2, 3
or 4 points, with a 1 indicating the most negative attitude and a 4 indicating the most positive.
The survey takes little time to administer.
The 20-item assessment consists of two subsets: one 10-item subset focused on
recreational reading attitudes and one 10-item subset focused on academic reading attitudes.
Students are asked to score every item in the subset; therefore, they earn a score between 10-40
on each subset, and can earn a score between 20-80 for the overall reading score. The three
scores are each converted to percentiles, which are based on national norms and are
grade-specific. The assessment provides general characterizations on reading attitude on those
two dimensions as well as a total reading attitude. The survey is widely understood to be very
reliable, with reliability coefficients ranging from .74 to .89; of the 18 coefficients computed, 16
were at least .80 (McKenna, Kear & Ellsworth, 1995). Participating students took this survey in
one sitting. The reliability coefficient for combined ERAS reading scores for this small group
was .67, which falls within a low but acceptable range. De-identified reading attitude scores can
be found in Appendix I; however, a detailed analysis of the data is below.
161
Scores for the recreational reading component of the Elementary Reading Attitude
Survey for the students participating in this study ranged from 17-37, with percentiles ranging
from the 1st percentile to the 90th percentile. The mean score in recreational reading was 29.47,
which was 61.2% of the total score possible, showing that overall, the group of students enjoyed
recreational reading more than the average student. This is an interesting finding, considering
that these students are all struggling readers. Standard deviation in recreational reading attitude
scores was 5.004, showing that the scores were somewhat dispersed, although the sample size
was small. Of the 19 participating students who took this assessment, 15 were within one
standard deviation of the recreational mean score. That number represents 78.9% of the
participating students.
Next, the data were analyzed by gender and grade level. A review of the literature
showed that grade level and gender have effects on reading attitudes. First, there is a difference
in reading attitude based on gender and language, two important areas of interest for this study
(Akbari, et al., 2017; Logan & Johnston, 2009; Martínez, et al., 2008; McKenna, et al., 2012;
Mohd-Asraf & Abdullah, 2016). Secondly, positive reading attitudes can decline as students
matriculate through the grades (Martínez, Aricak, & Jewell, 2008; McKenna, et al., 1995; Smith,
1990; Varuzza, et al., 2014).
Breaking the data down by gender, there was a relatively significant difference in
recreational scores at both grade three and grade four. Females in both grades had higher
recreational reading attitude scores than males in this study. The difference was six points at
grade 3 and 7.08 points at grade 4, which corroborates with the review of the literature. Further,
scores for the girls in grade 4 had little variability, with a standard deviation of 1.972.
162
When looking at the data by grade level, however, the mean recreational reading scores
for both males and females increased from grade three to grade four by approximately 10%, and
the standard deviation decreased from grade three to grade four. This finding was somewhat
surprising, given that a review of the literature showed that reading attitudes decline as students
move through grade levels. Table 4.6 below shows the mean recreational reading attitude data
and the standard deviation for the participating students delineated by gender and grade level.
Table 4.6. Mean Recreational Reading Attitude Score Data by Gender and Grade Level
Grade 3
Male
Female
Mean: 24.33
SD: 5.44
N= 3
Mean: 30.33
SD: 3.14
N= 6
Grade 4
Mean: 26.25
SD: 4.02
N= 4
Mean: 33.33
SD: 1.97
N= 6
The data were disaggregated by gender and grade level only, because all students participating
were students of color, only three of the 19 students (15.8%) were not English Language
Learners (ELs), and only one student was not under free/reduced lunch (FRP) status. Therefore,
this researcher did not compare the differences in reading difficulties between these groups.
Scores in academic reading ranged from 21-37, with percentiles ranging from the 20th
percentile to the 95th percentile. The mean score in academic reading was 29.11, which was
61.2% of the total score possible. Similar to the recreational reading scores, students in this study
had a more positive attitude toward academic reading than the average student. Standard
163
deviation in academic reading attitude scores was 4.677. Of the 19 student participants, 12 were
within one standard deviation of the academic mean score. That number represents 63.2% of the
participating students. Table 4.7 below shows the mean academic reading attitude data and the
standard deviation for the participating students delineated by gender and grade level.
Table 4.7. Mean Academic Reading Attitude Score Data By Gender and Grade Level
Grade 3
Male
Female
Mean: 28.67
SD: 4.99
N= 3
Mean: 30.0
SD: 4.12
N= 6
Grade 4
Mean: 25.75
SD: 5.78
N= 4
Mean: 30.67
SD: 4.59
N= 6
Breaking the scores down by gender, while there still was a difference in academic
reading attitude scores between males and females, there was less of a difference in academic
reading attitude scores between males and females in grade three (difference of 1.33) than in
grade four (difference of 4.92), and less overall difference in academic reading attitude scores at
both grade levels than differences in recreational reading attitude scores. The academic reading
attitude of males in this study declined from grade three to grade four by over 10%, which is in
keeping with the review of the literature. However, there was no decline in female academic
reading attitudes; the scores from grade three to grade four stayed relatively stable. Again, this
researcher disaggregated the data by gender alone, as all students participating were students of
color, only three of the 19 students (15.8%) were not English Language Learners (ELs), and only
one student was not under free-/reduced lunch (FRP) status.
164
Overall reading attitude scores (combining both recreational and academic reading
scores) ranged from 39-72, with percentiles ranging from the 9th percentile to the 91st percentile.
The mean total score was 58.59, which was 62.12% of the total score possible. Standard
deviation in total reading attitude scores was 8.853. Of the 19 students participating, 14 were
within one standard deviation of the total reading score. That number represents 73.68% of the
participating students. Table 4.8 below shows the mean total reading attitude data and standard
deviation for participating students delineated by gender and grade level.
Table 4.8. Mean Total Reading Attitude Score Data By Gender and Grade Level
Grade 3
Male
Female
Mean: 53.0
SD: 9.89
N= 3
Mean: 60.33
SD: 6.52
N= 6
Grade 4
Mean: 52.0
SD: 6.67
N= 4
Mean: 63.5
SD: 5.94
N= 6
When analyzing the total reading attitude scores by gender, there was a relatively
significant difference in mean scores between males and females at both grade levels, although
the sample size was small. The girls participating in this study indicated a more positive overall
attitude toward reading as evidenced by a mean score difference of 7.33 points at grade three and
a mean score difference of 11.5 points at grade four. This finding is in agreement with the review
of the literature.
Raw scores for recreational and academic are approximately the same (recreational mean
= 29.47; academic mean = 29.10), but the percentiles slightly favored a preference for academic
165
reading (recreational mean percentile = 50.8; academic mean percentile = 59.7). The fourth
grade students participating in this study slightly preferred reading in both recreational and
academic categories, although the sample is too small to say the difference is significant. Overall,
the students’ mean percentile for reading attitudes is in the 55th percentile, showing that this was
a fairly balanced group.
Science Pre-Assessment.
Prior knowledge plays a pivotal role in reading comprehension and in content learning,
because people learn things from what they read, and they apply that knowledge to new texts in
order to comprehend them (Afflerbach, 1990; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017). The
greater the background knowledge, the more a student is able to draw on it as they read, and the
more profound the effects on reading comprehension (Afflerbach, 1990; Castles, Rastle &
Nation, 2018; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Oakhill, Cain, & Bryant, 2003; Pearson & Billman,
2016; Wharton-Mcdonald & Erickson, 2017; Willingham, 2017; Willingham, 2006). The
research has shown that when a person is reading and can make connections to the text from
existing knowledge they have on a subject, comprehension is enhanced (Afflerbach, 1990;
Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Kendeou, McMaster, & Christ, 2016; Kendeou & O’Brien,
2016, Spilich, Vesonder, Chiesi & Voss, 1979). Thus, it was essential to gain an understanding of
the participating students’ general knowledge of grade level science concepts, especially those
concepts that aligned to the themes of the texts they would be reading for the purposes of this
research study.
166
To assess the students’ base knowledge of science concepts, an eight-item science
assessment was created by the research team at the large research university overseeing the
iSTART-Early research. The team used a modified yet appropriate version of the fourth grade
NAEP Science Assessment (National Assessment for Educational Progress, 2009, see Appendix
E). Questions on this pre-assessment correlated with the topics chosen for the texts students read
and paraphrased. Students earned one point for each correct answer on this modified assessment,
for a total possible of eight points. The students’ science pre-assessment data are found in Table
4.9 below.
Table 4.9. Science Pre-Assessment Data
Student
Gender
Grade Level
Score (out of 8 total
possible)
Percentage:
10001
F
4
6
75%
10002
M
4
5
62.5%
10003
F
3
2
25%
10004
M
4
7
87.5%
10005
F
3
4
50%
10006
M
3
ABSENT
10007
F
4
5
62.5%
10008
F
3
1
12.5%
10009
F
3
ABSENT
10010
F
3
2
25%
10011
M
4
1
12.5%
10012
F
3
3
37.5%
167
10013
M
3
3
37.5%
10014
F
4
6
75%
10015
F
4
6
75%
10016
F
4
2
25%
10017
M
4
4
50%
10018
F
4
6
75%
10019
M
3
4
50%
Mean Score: 3.94117647
SD: 7.6773157
Mean Percentage:
49.26%
Students scored between 1-7 on this assessment, with two students absent during this part
of the research. Two students scored a 1 out of 8, and one student scored a 7 out of 8. The overall
mean score earned on this assessment was 3.9412, a number just below 50% of the total points
possible. Only three students scored a 4 out of 8, which was just over the mean. The standard
deviation was 7.6773, which meant that every student was within one standard deviation of the
mean. The number of items on this assessment was small, hence the large variability between
scores. While the mean percentage score on the science assessment was 49.3%, fourth grade
students earned, on average, a higher mean score (60%) than the third grade students earned
(33.9%), which is not surprising, given the fact that they are one year older.
Breaking down the data further by grade level and gender, some interesting findings were
made. Table 4.10 below shows the mean score breakdown by grade level and gender.
Table 4.10. Mean Science Pre-Assessment Data by Gender and Grade Level
Grade 3
Male
Female
168
Mean: 3
N= 2
Mean: 2.2
N= 5
Grade 4
Mean: 4.25
N= 4
Mean: 5.17
N= 6
Females in grade 3 earned a higher score than males at that grade level. Interestingly,
girls at grade 4 outscored boys in both grades three and four with a mean score of 5.17, which
represents 64.25% of the total score possible. Overall, however, the result of this assessment
revealed that knowledge of grade level science concepts was limited. The purpose of this
assessment was to show science knowledge, not reading ability; therefore, support from the
classroom teacher or researcher was warranted if students were struggling to read the
assessment.
The results of both pre-assessments were used when analyzing the paraphrasing results
for each student and the group as a whole. The next step in the research process entailed students
learning about the paraphrasing strategy, and reading and paraphrasing texts. While the students
engaged with these tasks, the researcher gathered both quantitative and qualitative observational
data. Once done, the paraphrases themselves were analyzed. The next section will analyze both
the qualitative and quantitative data gathered from the act of paraphrasing.
Student Paraphrase Data.
Comprehension, as evidenced by the review of the literature, is an integral part of
academic success, as it is a required skill in schools in order to learn new content. However,
comprehension is not a singular skill; rather it is a conglomeration of many component skills and
169
activities (Kendeou, van den Broek, White, & Lynch, 2007; Rapp & van den Broek, 2005), and
are developed early in a child’s life, outside of reading (Kendeou, van den Broek, White, &
Lynch, 2007). As part of this study, students were asked to read subsets of the texts developed
and paraphrase selected sentences. Paraphrasing is a strategy that has shown to be an effective
comprehension strategy (Hagaman & Casey, 2016; Hagaman, et al, 2016; Hagaman, et al., 2012;
Kletzien, 2009).
The decision to focus only on paraphrasing was one made as a result of the significant
alterations to the traditional summer targeted services program in the district. As has been
previously established, there were unforeseen disruptions to the traditional summer targeted
services programming caused by the COVID-19 pandemic. As a result, original plans for the
summer targeted services program were altered significantly, and the researcher had limited time
with the students. Therefore, the decision was made by the researcher, along with the team of
researchers at the large research university, to focus on the paraphrasing strategy to help
understand how students were comprehending the text, as this is the first strategy in the ultimate
iSTART-Early web-based comprehension intervention tutoring tool where students generate
responses. Further, the paraphrasing strategy, along with comprehension monitoring and
inference-making, are strategies that are needed for higher order cognitive processing in order to
comprehend texts, such as utilizing prior knowledge to integrate ideas (Kintsch, 1988, 1998) and
students in this age range and grade level band are developmentally ready for interventions that
will help increase their use of comprehension strategies (Del Giudice, 2014; Pearson & Billman,
2016). To ensure that students had an understanding of the paraphrasing strategy, an instructional
170
video explaining the paraphrasing strategy was created for students to watch prior to reading the
texts.
The researcher gathered qualitative data on students’ engagement with and concentration
on the instructional video, and also while they worked on reading and paraphrasing texts. This
work was done individually on each student’s computer; they watched the instructional video,
read portions of a text and were asked to paraphrase sentences by typing them on their computer.
The qualitative data were analyzed for overarching themes, which will be discussed in the next
section. Additionally, the researcher gathered quantitative data on both the acts of watching the
instructional video, and of reading and paraphrasing by the students (see Student Observation
Form Text Reading and Responding, Appendix F). This student observation form was used to
gather quantifiable data on what activities the students engaged in during a set observation time,
and included reading, watching the short instructional video, paraphrasing, and engaging in
vocabulary activities. The quantitative observational data will be discussed in the next section.
At the end of reading and paraphrasing each text, a set of questions were shown on the
screen. Students were asked to rate each text and the act of paraphrasing sentences in each text.
This provided data on students’ perceptions of the texts. Results of the students’ text and
paraphrase perceptions can be found in Appendix K. Next, the paraphrases were scored to gather
data on the quality of each paraphrase (see Coding Table for Student Paraphrasing Responses,
Appendix G). In order to answer the research questions, the researcher analyzed and triangulated
all data. This researcher collected both qualitative and quantitative data on students’ engagement
with the work required of them in this study: watching the instructional video, reading the texts,
and paraphrasing the selected sentences. These data are discussed in the next two sections.
171
Student Engagement in Reading and Paraphrasing.
Engagement is a critical factor in successful reading for many students. More engaged
readers are more motivated, have higher reading self-efficacy, and tend to utilize strategies to
continue comprehending the text (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Massey & Miller, 2017;
Wigfield, et al., 2008; Willingham, 2017). The third research question focuses on a diverse
population of students: What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for
(a) a racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or (b)
students who read below grade level? Therefore, it was important to understand the interactions
and experiences a diverse body of students have with texts, in order to provide the best texts
possible in a reading comprehension intervention.
All students participating in this study were students of color, most of whom were
English Language Learners (ELs). The role of engagement and motivation is found to be a
greater problem for students of color, for whom dropout rates are the highest (Fredricks,
Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Rumberger, 1987; Rumberger, 1995). Thus, it was imperative to
observe all students in the act of reading and paraphrasing to gauge their engagement in the
various tasks that comprised this research. Observational data was collected both qualitatively
and quantitatively. That data will be discussed in the next two sections.
Qualitative Observational Data Analysis.
Observational data was gathered in each classroom over two non-consecutive days.
Sixteen students in five classrooms were observed for 240 minutes over that two-day period. The
researcher gathered data on six activities: watching the instructional video, ease and/or difficulty
172
of passage reading, activities related to vocabulary, activities related to paraphrase creation,
alignment of the paraphrase content, and engagement in the task, which the researcher gauged
through observations of the students’ concentration. Once the data was collected, the researcher
authentically coded the qualitative data for themes; no codes were created prior to the data
collection. Four themes emerged as having an observable impact on the work in which the
students were engaged. They are found in Table 4.11 below. A complete table of qualitative
observational data can be found in Appendix J.
Table 4.11. Themes from Student Observations
Theme
Brief Description
Engagement
Looked at concentration level of student while working on
the task at hand
Stamina
Looked at how long student persisted with the task at hand
Text
Difficulty of text
Difficulty of vocabulary
Alignment to paraphrase task
Paraphrase Creation
Thoughtfulness of the paraphrase
Ability to type
On the days scheduled for observation, there were four students absent, three male and
one female. Because of the absences, there were 15 students over the course of two days for
whom observational data were taken. Of those 15 students, 11 were female and four were male.
Every student who was observed identified using the federal category of Hispanic. The
researcher took careful notes with regard to students’ actions as they watched the instructional
video, read the passages, and typed their paraphrases of the predetermined sentences.
173
All students watched the instructional video, with 14/15 completely immersed in the task.
One female student in grade 3 became distracted with the actions of other students while
watching the video, and was reminded by the researcher to keep watching. Of the 15 observed
students, three watched the video more than one time. Two students needed help with the
directions. One third grade male student needed support from the researcher in reading the
directions. While this student began to read the first text, he began to play with the buttons on the
computer and, after 20 minutes, asked the researcher, “Do I have to continue?” Another third
grade female began watching the video intently right away. However, she began to be distracted
by noises nearby, and was redirected by the researcher. Once this student began reading the text,
she needed to be reminded of the directions; after 12 minutes working on the activity she
claimed, “I’m stuck.”
All students were observed to have an initially positive attitude toward the activities
involved in this study. Once the students began to read and paraphrase, however, the researcher
noted behaviors that indicated some students were struggling to read the texts. One female third
grader moved her face closer to the screen when reading, furrowed her brow and began to point
to the screen. One student needed to have the directions read to him by the researcher. One third
grade student struggled so much that, on the second day of observing her, the researcher helped
her to read many words in the passage, yet she still struggled to understand the meaning of the
passage. What is interesting to note about this student, however, is that when generating a
paraphrase for a sentence, she wanted to use the word equipment and asked for help spelling it.
From observing the students interacting with the texts, the researcher found that the
vocabulary in the texts was difficult for students in both grades 3 and 4. Several students asked
174
for help with vocabulary words. One student asked, “What does ‘clump’ mean?” Another
student could not read the word nutrient. When the researcher read the word to the student, they
asked, “What is a nutrient?” Several students needed help with the word Fahrenheit. One student
in fourth grade, rather than ask the researcher for help, used an online search engine to try to
determine the meaning of words. This was especially noteworthy, as the students were
demonstrating persistence and problem-solving skills to help them through the text.
The researcher noticed, as students were reading the texts, that some difficult words were
not defined within the context of the text (e.g. satellite); rather, it was assumed by the text
creators that the students would know the word. Other words were defined within the tool after
the sentence needed paraphrasing, yet understanding of informational text, especially science
texts that use content-specific words, requires knowledge of a vast array of content-specific
words (Santoro, et al., 2016). One keenly interesting observation was of a third grade student
who exhibited a larger oral vocabulary, using the term research when trying to explain to the
researcher the ideas of satellite and telescopes, but the student could not transfer that idea in
written form. A further notable observation by the researcher was that students were struggling
with spelling and were intent on spelling the words correctly. This need to spell every word
correctly slowed the generation of their responses considerably.
The researcher noted that two girls in one third grade classroom struggled to read the
very first text, which was the easiest of the eight used in this study. Both girls, however, were
interested in the task and asked for help. One of the two girls tried hard to sound out words,
pointing to the screen. Eventually they both became distracted, with one talking about school and
175
one playing with the computer cord. During this observation, both female students abandoned
the text after 27 minutes.
The act of reading and paraphrasing sentences on a computer was important for the
researcher to observe, as these behaviors help to inform the second research question, What are
essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion, topics) for the selection of
informational texts that can be used for teaching reading comprehension strategies, such as
paraphrasing, self-explanation, summarization, and question asking, in a digital format? The
researcher was interested to see how students interacted with the activities in a digital format.
Several very important observations were made.
On the first day of observation, one student’s computer did not have the Qualtrics link
loaded. This issue was quickly resolved by a district technology staff member; however, the
student was unable to begin the task at the same time as the other classmates. Once the link was
loaded, the student began the task in earnest. However, during the first day of observation, this
student was unable to get through one passage.
Tools available on the computer were both a help and a hindrance. Some students used
Spellcheck to help with their spelling. Others used a search engine to try to spell words correctly.
Because the instructional video was available on demand, some students took advantage of that
availability and were observed watching the instructional video more than once during this study,
with one student watching it twice in a row. One student, on the second day of observation,
clicked out of the Qualtrics tool and ended up on a different site. The student stopped, looked at
another student, and began to laugh. When she realized she was being observed by the
researcher, she went back into the Qualtrics tool and began to type. During the second day of
176
observation in a different classroom, another student inadvertently clicked out of the Qualtrics
tool but immediately got back in again and continued the work. One student was observed
simply looking at the keyboard while playing with the computer cord.
The physical act of typing the sentences was troublesome for all but one student, slowing
them down considerably when creating their paraphrases. The researcher repeatedly recorded
students struggling with typing and struggling to find the correct key on their computer. Four
students were observed using one finger to type words. The other students used their two index
fingers to type, but it was obvious to the researcher that all but one of the students were still
learning the location of letters on the keyboard. One female student began typing her paraphrase
using two fingers, but after struggling to find the letters on the keyboard switched to one-finger
typing.
Another very interesting observation with regard to paraphrase generation is that it
seemed to be more difficult for students to paraphrase shorter sentences. The Qualtrics tool was
created in such a way that the passage would break after the target sentence was presented. The
student would paraphrase the target sentence before continuing with the passage. The researcher
observed several students staring at the screen displaying a short sentence that needed to be
paraphrased; one student asked, “How can you change that?” On the first day of observations,
the researcher noticed that it took approximately 30 minutes for one student in one fourth grade
classroom to read and paraphrase one text. Two students were unable to complete even one text.
They abandoned the exercise after the first paraphrase; therefore, no Qualtrics data was available
for them.
177
The behaviors the students were displaying as they watched the instructional video and
read and paraphrased the texts was of keen interest to the researcher, as behaviors can change if
frustration levels rise (Halladay, 2012; Ilter, 2017). All students engaged in the video for the
entire four minutes and eight seconds, although one third grade female became briefly distracted
by nearby people. However, she re-engaged quickly after being encouraged by the researcher.
Further, all students exhibited a willingness to try hard and concentrate at the beginning. When
students began to struggle, however, is when the researcher observed engagement waning and
their behaviors shifting.
The students began to exhibit struggle with the task by looking away from the screen.
Further, difficulty in typing led to disengagement in the task. Students were observed simply
stopping their typing and getting up and stretching, playing with the computer cord, or looking
out the window. Those students also asked how many passages they needed to complete.
However, when the researcher provided some help to the student, such as providing the
definition to a word, the student exhibited engagement once again. Surprisingly to the researcher,
some students were quite intent on spelling words correctly before moving on in the typing of
their paraphrase. They concentrated for some time on this task as they tried to figure out how to
correctly spell a word, even using a search engine to attempt to spell the word correctly.
The researcher noted several times during the observations that reading was somewhat
easier for students in grade 4 than those in grade 3. Stamina, in the form of lost concentration,
was an impediment to the work in both third and fourth grade. This can be illustrated by the
question that one third grade female asked the researcher, “How many do we have to do?”
Another third grade student, after completing paraphrases on two texts, asked the researcher,
178
“Can I be done?” One female student in grade four stood up, fixed her hair, sat back down and
began playing with a pencil. After one minute, this student went back to reading the passage.
However, stamina was not an issue for three students in fourth grade. They kept engaged with
the reading and paraphrasing and were observed completing at least three texts.
The qualitative notes that the researcher took while observing the students provided a rich
narrative to the activities through which the students worked. However, the researcher also
wanted to gather quantitative data while observing the students. The analysis of the data that was
amassed quantitatively is presented in the next section, with comparisons made to the qualitative
data gathered.
Quantitative Observational Data Analysis.
The researcher, using a modified observation form (Kendeou & McMaster, 2016, see
Appendix F), informally observed students engaging in reading and paraphrasing of the texts.
She spent time in each classroom observing students while using this form. This observation
form provided quantitative data on the number of observations made regarding tasks in which the
students engaged. There were four engagement indicators for observation. These indicators are
briefly described in Table 4.12 below.
Table 4.12. Engagement Indicators
Indicator
Brief Description
Reading
The student is engaged with the screen (other than video). Could include
student whispering/saying words or phrases
Paraphrasing
The student appears to think about the sentence that requires paraphrasing
or is actively typing a paraphrase response
Vocabulary
The student seems to be rereading to determine meaning or is actively
179
trying to determine the meaning of the word through other means
Other
The student displays appropriate behavior and engagement for a task not
previously described (e.g. watching instructional video).
The engagement indicator titled “Other” included activities such as watching the
instructional video, figuring out how to spell a word or checking the spelling of a word. If the
student’s computer showed the text, the researcher coded the observation either in the “Reading”
or “Vocabulary” engagement indicator, depending upon what task the student was observed
doing. If the student’s computer showed that a paraphrase was needed, or if the student was
active in creating a paraphrase, the researcher coded the observation in that engagement
indicator. If the student was active in that task, they scored a 1 for that observation in that
category. All disengagement displayed by the students scored a 0.
The researcher spent 10 seconds observing a student before moving to the next student.
The researcher used a timer on her phone when observing the students in all classrooms. Using a
timer ensured consistency. The students in each classroom were observed in order. For four of
the five classrooms, there were a total of 120 10-second observation intervals, but each
classroom had a different number of students being observed; therefore, the number of times a
student was observed differed from class to class. One classroom had only one student being
observed, and in that classroom, the researcher made an observation mark every 30 seconds.
This student was struggling and wanted to end early. Therefore, there were 31 30-second
observation interval notations for this student.
180
The number of scores (either 1 or 0) in an engagement indicator was then counted, and a
percentage of the total number of observations (120 for four classrooms, 31 for one classroom)
was calculated. The initial data provided the researcher with an understanding of how much time
was spent by the students on an observable task at hand and, conversely, how much
disengagement they showed for an engagement indicator. The researcher then calculated the
number of 1s and 0s in each engagement indicator to better understand the overall student
engagement/disengagement in the paraphrasing activity. This set of data is broken down by
classroom in Table 4.13 below.
Table 4.13. Engagement Observed in Paraphrasing Activity
Classroom
Reading
Observations
Mean %: 24.08
Paraphrasing
Observations
Mean %: 25.86
Vocabulary
Observations
Mean %:
5.48
Other
Observations
Mean %: 39.56
Disengagement
Observations
Mean %:
12.4
1 (120
Observations)
5 Students
28/120
23.3%
41/120
34.1%
13/120
10.9%
38/120
31.7%
9/120
7.5%
2 (120
Observations)
3 Students
53/120
41.2%
31/120
25.8%
9/120
7.5%
27/120
22.5%
5/120
4.2%
3 (120
Observations)
2 Students
46/120
38/3%
43/120
35.8%
2/120
1.6%
29/120
24.2%
32/120
26.7%
4 (120
Observations)
4 Students**
32/120
26.7%
21/120
17/5%
5/120
4/2%
44/120
51.7%
5/120
4.2%
5 (31
Observations)
1 Student
4/31
12.9%
5/31
16.1%
1/31
3.2%
21/31
67.7%
6/31
19.4%
**In this classroom, there was a technical issue with one student’s computer. The Qualtrics tool
was not loaded on the computer, so the student had to wait until the tool was loaded. The
181
observer coded the student waiting for the technician to load the tool in “other,” as the student
simply waited patiently to begin and seemed eager to begin the task.
Of the five classrooms of students being observed, two were fourth grade, two were third
grade, and one was a second/third multi-grade classroom. The means of each engagement
indicator were calculated using percentages, because the number of observations differed
between Classroom 1-4 and Classroom 5. Looking at the data, there are some interesting
observations to be made. First, the engagement indicator of “Other” had the highest single mean
percentage of all engagement indicators, with the mean percentage at 39.56%. This observation
is not surprising, as it took students some time to watch the instructional video and some students
watched it more than one time. Additionally, other activities, in particular using a search engine
to check the spelling of a word, was categorized under “Other.”
Next, the vocabulary engagement indicator had the lowest number of observations; the
mean percentage for this indicator was 5.48%. This observation was somewhat surprising. Many
of the students struggled to read the passages, yet they were not actively trying to determine
meanings of individual words. Some did through asking what a word meant or even looking up a
word in a search engine. The mean percentages of both the “Reading” engagement indicator and
“Paraphrasing” engagement indicator were quite close, being 24.08% and 25.86%, respectively;
the two categories of engagement indicators together accounted for 49.94% of all observations.
Classroom 4 had the highest number of students who watched the instructional video
more than once, which accounts for the high number in the engagement indicator of “Other.”
Two classrooms of third grade students had the highest number of observations of
disengagement. Interestingly, Classroom 1 and Classroom 3, one a fourth grade classroom and
182
one a third grade classroom, had the highest number of observations in the paraphrasing
engagement indicator.
Classroom 5 was the only multi-age classroom, with students in grade two and grade
three in the classroom. This class had just one student whose family provided consent to
participate in this study. The student, a third grader, struggled with this activity. The student was
easily distracted; they watched the video more than one time to try to better understand, which
led to a higher percentage of observations in the engagement indicator of “Other.” The student
typed with only one finger, so it took them longer to finish a paraphrase. This accounted for a
greater number of observations in the paraphrasing engagement indicator than in the reading
indicator; 16.1% of the observations for this one student was in the paraphrasing indicator.
However, the student’s percentage of observations in the “Other” engagement indicator was
67.7%, by far the highest percentage of all five classrooms and all four engagement indicators.
The student needed to watch the video more than once and also was very distracted. This student
was able to complete only two paraphrases of the first text.
Analyzing the observational data by gender rendered some interesting observations. First,
the only students overtly concerned with correct spelling were male, across all classrooms.
Disengaged observations were gender-agnostic; that is, struggles with a task were displayed by
both genders. However, more frequent and lasting distracted behavior was displayed by female
students; male students displayed distraction or struggle for shorter periods of time before
reengaging in the task. Female students asked the researcher for help more than did male
students. The quantitative observational data were not broken down by ethnicity or language
183
status, as all students were students of color and all but one student participating in the study
qualified for free- or reduced-price lunch.
Observational data gathered provided the researcher with the behaviors of the students
while working through the tasks of reading and typing paraphrases of short science texts.
Qualtrics captured the paraphrases themselves and the number of paraphrases completed by each
student. It is important to note that no student completed all eight texts, but the students
completed 187 paraphrases. Prior to discussing the quality of the paraphrases themselves, it is
important to discuss how the paraphrases were coded.
Student Paraphrase Coding.
The paraphrases produced by the students were scored using a modified version of a
rubric developed by McNamara and colleagues (2007a, see Appendix G). The use of a
paraphrase activity correlates with the first generative strategy which will be used in the first
module of iSTART-Early, which is paraphrasing. This strategy provided information on how well
the students comprehended the texts. The modified rubric was used to score the paraphrases, and
the scoring of the paraphrases was three-phased.
Paraphrase Filter.
The first phase involved the use of a filter. The participating students generated 187
paraphrases for this study. Prior to coding the paraphrases for quality, the paraphrases were first
filtered for any responses that were irrelevant, too short, or direct copies or clause reversals of
the target sentences. First, if the paraphrase could not be read or if it was irrelevant or it was
difficult to derive any meaning from it, scoring for that paraphrase ended, and the paraphrase
184
earned a score of 9, meaning that the paraphrase was not gauged for any quality. If a paraphrase
earned a score of 0 for length, it was looked at for the next filters, which were copy/paste and
clause reversal; a clause reversal is a modified copy/paste, in that the clauses are reversed. If a
paraphrase earned a 1 in any of those categories, scoring ended for the paraphrase and it earned a
9, meaning it was not further gauged for any quality. A score of 9 indicated that the paraphrase
was filtered out. Of the 187 paraphrases, 69 were filtered out, which accounted for over one-third
of the total paraphrases that the students generated. Table 4.14 below illustrates the paraphrase
filter data.
Table 4.14. Paraphrase Filter Data
Filter
Number Filtered
Percent of Total
Paraphrases (n=187)
Percent of Filtered
Paraphrases
Clause Reversals
2
1.069
2.899
Copy/Paste
39
20.856
56.522
Too Short
14
7.487
20.289
Garbage (Nonsense)
8
4.278
11.594
Irrelevant (Not
related to topic)
6
3.209
8.696
Total:
69
36.898
100
Paraphrase Numerical Coding.
The second phase took place once the filtering was completed. The remaining
paraphrases were dichotomously coded (using 0 or 1) for the presence of eight different factors:
paraphrase presence, lexical similarity, syntactic similarity, semantic similarity, elaboration,
185
inaccuracy, or other content. For the categories of paraphrase presence, lexical similarity,
syntactic similarity, semantic similarity, and elaboration, a score of 1 meant that the particular
factor was present in the paraphrase, and a 0 meant that the particular factor was not present. For
inaccuracy and other content, a score of 0 meant a higher quality paraphrase, in that there was
either no inaccuracy or other content. Once the individual factors were scored, the paraphrases
were given an overall score on a scale of 0-2 for quality: 0 indicating a poor paraphrase, 1
indicating the presence of paraphrase strategies but a need for improvement, and a 2 indicating a
high quality paraphrase.
The third phase of scoring involved the creation of inter-rater reliability. Two researchers
from two different research institutions coded all paraphrases separately. They then met to
discuss their results in order to establish inter-rater reliability. They did so by resolving all
disagreements and differences and coming to consensus on each code in order to determine final
scores for each paraphrase generated by students. The paraphrase quality data are discussed in
the next section.
Paraphrase Quality Data.
The two researchers scored each paraphrase separately and came to consensus on each of
the paraphrase quality scores. This paraphrase quality data set is shown in Tables 4.15 below.
Table 4.15. Paraphrase Quality Score Data
Paraphrase Quality Score
Number of Paraphrases
Percentage of Paraphrases
0
24
12.834%
1
63
33.690%
186
2
31
16.578%
9 (paraphrases filtered out)
69
36.898%
Total
187
100%
This researcher disaggregated the paraphrase quality data by individual text, as the texts
were populated into Qualtrics in order of least to most difficult. This data set is shown in Table
4.16 below.
Table 4.16. Paraphrase Quality Data by Text
Text
Number and
Title
# of
Paraphrases
Scoring 2
# of
Paraphrases
Scoring 1
# of
Paraphrases
Scoring 0
# of
Paraphrases
Filtered
Total
Paraphrases
1
How A Star
Is Born
12
16
7
17
52
2
Eating
Healthy
6
15
6
18
45
3
A Visit to
Mars
3
13
9
10
35
4
Wildfires
8
6
1
10
25
5
Blood
1
4
1
4
10
6
Ostriches
0
4
0
6
10
7
Starfish
2
4
0
4
10
187
Because the texts were presented to students in order of easiest to most difficult, this
researcher predicted that the students would have the most success on the first text. As predicted,
Text 1, “How a Star is Born,” had the highest number of paraphrases generated (N=52), and the
highest number paraphrases earning a score of 2, 1, or 0 (N=35). Total paraphrases generated for
each text declined with each subsequent text, which is also predicted, given that the texts
increased in difficulty and many students stopped paraphrasing after the third text. An additional
five students stopped paraphrasing after the fourth text, and two students paraphrased seven
texts.
However, when looking at the percentages of paraphrases earning a score of 2, 1, or 0, “A
Visit to Mars,” which was the third text presented to students, had 71% of the total paraphrases
earning a score of 2, 1, 0, which was the highest percentage of the seven texts paraphrased by
students. “How a Star is Born,” the first text in the series, had 67% of its total paraphrases
earning a score of 2, 1, or 0. When analyzing the paraphrase quality by score (not filtered out),
“Wildfires,” which was Text 4, had the highest percentage of paraphrases earning a quality score
of 2, at 53%.
Interestingly, the two most difficult texts that students paraphrased, which were Texts 6
(“Ostriches”), and 7 (“Starfish”), generated 20 paraphrases total. However, half of those
paraphrases earned a score of 2 or 1. Additionally, two earned a paraphrase quality score of 2,
which is 20% of the total paraphrases for those texts, and eight earned a score of 1, which is 40%
of the total paraphrases generated for those two texts.
As the researcher continued to disaggregate data, it was important to analyze the student
behaviors with regard to paraphrase generation. Several interesting observations with regard to
188
student paraphrase responses were noticed. First, as time went on, many students stopped trying
to create paraphrases. Some just started typing in nonsense, but many would just retype the
sentence verbatim (or change only a word or two). There were two students in particular that got
farther than their peers in paraphrasing; one completed 19 paraphrases and one completed 34 -
but for the later half of their responses, they were just retyping the sentences. They were able to
produce more answers because their typing did not get in the way, as it did for others.
There were two other students, however, who also completed 19 paraphrases and 34
paraphrases, and their paraphrase data was of higher quality than the other two students who
completed just as many. Utilizing the qualitative observational data gathered while students were
watching the instructional video, reading the texts, and paraphrasing the predetermined
sentences, the researcher observed that one of these two students watched the video more than
two times. The other student whose paraphrases were of higher quality worked through four texts
rather quickly and wanted to stop for the day on the first day of paraphrasing; however on the
next day that student began the task again.
There were two student participants in this study who did not submit even the first set of
paraphrases into Qualtrics. Therefore, Qualtrics was unable to capture data on those two
students. Each of the remaining 13 students’ paraphrase quality data are illustrated in Table 4.17
below.
Table 4.17. Paraphrase Quality Data By Student
Student
ID
Grade
Gender
Total
Paraphrases
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
10001
4
F
4
2
0
0
2
189
10002
4
M
19
9
10
0
0
10003
3
F
4
1
2
1
0
10004
4
M
14
4
2
7
1
10005
3
F
9
1
4
0
4
10010
3
F
19
0
1
0
18
10012
3
F
19
0
1
9
9
10013
3
M
9
0
2
0
7
10014
4
F
34
4
25
1
4
10015
4
F
34
5
11
4
14
10016
4
F
14
0
5
0
9
10017
4
M
4
2
1
0
1
10018
4
F
4
3
0
1
0
Totals
187
31
64
23
69
The paraphrase quality data were further analyzed by grade level and gender. Because of
dwindling numbers in the in-person summer school targeted services program, the remaining
student participants were 100% English language learners (ELs), and all but one qualified for
free- or reduced-price lunch; there was no need for the data to be analyzed by those particular
strands. Therefore, the researcher next analyzed the data by gender first. This data set is
presented in Table 4.18 below.
Table 4.18. Paraphrase Quality Data by Gender
Gender
(% of
Total)
Total
Paraphrases
(%)
# Scoring 2
(%)
# Scoring
1(%)
# Scoring 0
(%)
# Filtered
(%)
Female
141 (75.4%)
16 (51.6%)
49 (76.6%)
16 (69.6%)
60 (87.0%)
190
(69.2%)
Male
(30.8%)
46 (24.6%)
15 (48.4%)
15 (23.4%)
7 (30.4%)
9 (13.0%)
Mean:
Female: 23.5
Male: 11.5
Female: 1.78
Male: 2.5
Female: 5.44
Male: 3.75
Female: 1.78
Male: 1.75
Female: 6.67
Male: 2.25
Female student participants made up 69% of the students on which the researcher was
able to gather paraphrase quality data. It would be logical, then, to assume that the girls would
generate more paraphrases, which they did. However, they outperformed the boys’ generation of
paraphrases slightly based on their numbers. Girls generated 75.4% of all paraphrases; the mean
paraphrase generation number for female participants was 23.5. Male student participants made
up 30.8% of the population, but they generated only 24.6% of all paraphrases. Mean paraphrase
generation number for male student participants was 11.5.
When looking at the paraphrase quality data, however, interesting observations were
made. Girls did not generate paraphrase quality scores of 2 commensurate with their numbers of
paraphrases generated. The female participants in this study generated 16 paraphrases that earned
a quality score of 2, which is 51.6% of all paraphrases earning that score. However, that number
constituted only 11.3% of the female paraphrases overall. The male participants generated 15
paraphrases earning a 2, or 48.4% of the total paraphrases earning that score. However, that
number constitutes 32.6%, or nearly one-third of the total paraphrases the male students
produced. They generated less paraphrases, which was predicted, given the fact that they only
made up approximately one-third of the student participants; however, they generated nearly half
of the paraphrases earning a quality score of 2.
191
The female student participants earned a paraphrase quality score of 1 on 49 paraphrases,
which was 76.6% of the total paraphrases earning that score. Additionally, that number
constitutes 34.8% of all paraphrases generated by the girls. Their percentage of paraphrases
generated that earned a score of 1 (76.6%) is just slightly more than the percentage of total
paraphrases generated by the girls (75.4%). However, females made up only slightly more than
two-thirds of the total student participants in this study. Male student participants scored a 1 on
15 paraphrases, which was 23.4% of the total paraphrases earning this score; however, that
number constituted 32.6% of all paraphrases generated by the boys. Their percentage of
paraphrases generated that earned this score (23.4%) is nearly even with the percentage of total
paraphrases generated by the boys (24.6%). Girls outperformed the boys in paraphrases earning a
quality score of 1.
The female participants earned a paraphrase quality score of 0 on 16 paraphrases. This
number constitutes 69.6% of the 23 total paraphrases earning a score of 0, which is only 0.4% off
from the total percentage of female participants generating paraphrases in this study (69.2%).
Further, that number constitutes 11.3% of all paraphrases they generated. In this category, the
girls earned a score of 0 which is commensurate with their percentage of participation. The same
can be said about the performance of the male student participants earning a paraphrase quality
score of 0. The boys earned a quality score of 0 on 7 paraphrases, which is 30.4% of the total
paraphrases earning that score. The boys earned a score of 0, which is commensurate with their
percentage of participation. This number constituted 15.2% of the total paraphrases they
generated.
192
With regard to the final category, the number of paraphrases that had been previously
filtered and therefore unscorable, the male student participants generated far less paraphrases that
were filtered than their female counterparts. The total number of paraphrases generated by the
male student participants was 9, or 13.0% of the total number of paraphrases filtered. This
percentage is over 17 percentage points lower than the percentage of male student participants
generating paraphrases in this study. Further, the number of paraphrases generated by males that
were filtered out contstituted 19.6% of all paraphrases they generated. Female student
participants, on the other hand, generated 60 paraphrases that were previously filtered out. This
number represents 87.0% of all paraphrases filtered prior to scoring, and is nearly18 percentage
points higher than the percentage of girls who generated paraphrases for this study. Further, that
number is 42.6% of all paraphrases the girls generated.
Overall, while females generated over three times as many paraphrases than males in this
study, 65.2% of the paraphrases that the males produced scored a 2 or a 1 for paraphrase quality,
compared to 46.1% of the paraphrases generated by the females earning those scores.
Conversely, females had a much higher number of paraphrases earning a score of 0 (69.6%) or
being filtered out prior to scoring (87%), than did the males’ paraphrases that earned those scores
(30.4% and 13%, respectively). While the number of paraphrases the males generated was lower
than that of the females, their paraphrases overall were stronger.
The paraphrase quality data was also disaggregated by grade level. That data set is found
in Table 4.19 below.
Table 4.19. Paraphrase Quality Data by Grade Level
193
Grade
Level
(% of
Total)
Total
Paraphrases
(%)
# Scoring 2
(%)
# Scoring
1(%)
# Scoring 0
(%)
# Filtered
(%)
Grade 3
(38.5%)
60 (32.1%)
2 (6.5%)
10 (15.6%)
10 (43.5%)
38 (55.1%)
Grade 4
(61.5%)
127 (67.9%)
29 (93.5%)
54 (84.4%)
13 (56.5%)
31 (44.9%)
Mean:
Gr. 3: 12.0
Gr. 4: 15.9
Gr. 3: 0.4
Gr. 4: 3.6
Gr. 3: 2.0
Gr. 4: 6.7
Gr. 3: 2.0
Gr. 4: 1.6
Gr. 3: 7.6
Gr. 5: 3.9
When disaggregated data by grade level, the paraphrase quality data uncovered few surprises.
Students in third grade struggled more with the work. Grade 3 students wrote fewer paraphrases,
and had more paraphrases filtered out. The percentage of paraphrases falling in each group (e.g.
total paraphrases, total paraphrases filtered out) was not commensurate with the participation of
grade 3 students, which was 38.5% of the total student participation. The closest came in the
number of paraphrases generated, with the grade 3 students generating 32.1% of the paraphrases.
The third grade students had fewer paraphrases that scored a 2 or a 1; however, they did have
more paraphrases that scored a 0.
Overall, while nearly 13% of the paraphrases scored a 0, and nearly 37% of the
paraphrases had been previously filtered out, over 50% of the paraphrases earned a score of 1 or
2. This data point meant that the paraphrase strategy was present in over half of the paraphrases
generated by the students. Conversely, this data point also reveals the struggle that students had
in reading and paraphrasing the texts. Considering the fact that these students had previously
194
been identified as below grade-level readers, and pre-assessment data revealed gaps in students’
basic science knowledge, this paraphrase quality data revealed both problem and promise.
Comparison of Paraphrase Data to Text Indices Data
The researcher next compared the paraphrase data to the data gathered on the reading
indices on the texts used. Doing this analysis provided additional information on the nuances of
each text with regard to the Text Ease and Readability (T.E.R.A.) components of narrativity,
syntactic similarity, word concreteness, referential cohesion, and deep cohesion (for an
explanation of these components, see Table 4.3 presented previously in this chapter). As would
be expected, Text 1, “How a Star is Born,” generated the most paraphrases (N=57), as it was the
easiest text presented to students. It had the second-highest narrativity score (63), the
second-highest deep cohesion score (90), a high syntactic simplicity score (91), and strong word
concreteness and referential cohesion scores (75 and 74, respectively). However, while it had the
highest number of paraphrases earning a score of 2, it did not have the highest percentage of
paraphrases earning a score of 2. The text with the highest percentage of paraphrases earning a
score of 2 was “A Visit to Mars,” which was the third text presented to students.
An interesting finding was revealed with regard to Texts 3 (“A Visit to Mars”) and 4
“Wildfires”) when analyzing individual paraphrase scores for these two texts. Text 3, “A Visit to
Mars” had a deep cohesion score of 24, which was the lowest of the eight texts. Text 4,
“Wildfires” had a deep cohesion score of 99, which was highest of the eight texts. The two texts
combined had a total of 60 paraphrases generated - 35 for “A Visit to Mars” and 25 for
“Wildfires,” a difference of 10. Of the 35 paraphrases completed for “A Visit to Mars,” 25
195
earned a paraphrase score of 2, 1, or 0, which comprised 71.4% of the corpus of paraphrases
generated for this text. Text 4, “Wildfires,” on the other hand, had 25 total paraphrases, 15 of
which earned a score of 2, 1, or 0. That number accounts for 60% of the total number of
paraphrases generated for “Wildfires.” The paraphrase quality data for these two texts are found
in Table 4.20 below.
Table 4.20. Paraphrase Quality Data for “A Visit to Mars” and “Wildfires”
Text
# of Paraphrases
Scoring 2
# of Paraphrases
Scoring 1
# of Paraphrases
Scoring 0
Paraphrases
Filtered Out
A Visit to
Mars
(N=35)
3
13
9
10
Wildfires
(N=25)
8
6
1
10
The deep cohesion scores of the two texts differed by 75, which is the largest difference
between scores in any component category. However, the overall paraphrase quality data did not
fully reflect that wide difference, which means that other differences in the components
(narrativity, syntactic simplicity, word concreteness, and referential cohesion) were factors. The
T.E.R.A. component scores for these two texts are found in Table 4.21 below.
Table 4.21. T.E.R.A. Component Scores for “A Visit to Mars” and Wildfires”
Text
Flesch-
Kincaid
Score
Narrativity
Syntactic
Similarity
Word
Concreteness
Referential
Cohesion
Deep
Cohesion
Mars
3.0
60
79
30
88
24
196
Wildfires
5.1
47
90
74
44
99
Text 4, “Wildfires,” had three T.E.R.A. component scores which were higher than those for
“Mars.” Further, “Mars” had a higher narrativity score than “Wildfires,” and the narrative genre
is generally easier to read than expository text (Beck & Kucan, 2002; Jackson, et al., 2016);
Perfetti, 2007). Yet, while “Mars” had more paraphrases, “Wildfires” had a higher number of
paraphrases earning the highest quality score. “Mars” had three paraphrases that earned a quality
score of 2, which is 12% of the paraphrases for that text earning a score and not filtered out,
which was the second lowest percentage of the seven texts paraphrased by students. After
completing the comparative analysis of the paraphrase quality data to the text component data,
the researcher then did an analysis of paraphrase data to the students’ pre-assessment data.
Comparison of Paraphrase Data to Pre-Assessment Data
Paraphrase Data and Science Pre-Assessment Data.
To further guide an understanding of the essential characteristics of texts needed for a
reading comprehension strategy intervention tool, the researcher compared the paraphrase data
with each student’s pre-assessment data. First, it was important to this researcher to compare the
paraphrase quality data of each student to their science pre-assessment scores to determine any
correlation or predictable patterns. Table 4.22 below outlines the students’ paraphrase scores of
0, 1, and 2, and their science pre-assessment scores.
Table 4.22. Comparison of Students’ Paraphrase Quality Scores to Science Pre-Assessment
Student
Number
Total
Paraphrases
% of
Paraphrases
% of
Paraphrases
% of
Paraphrases
# of
Paraphrases
Science
Pre-Assessment
197
Scoring 2 (#)
Scoring 1 (#)
Scoring 0 (#)
Previously
Filtered Out
Percentage
10001
4
50% (2)
0
0
9
75%
10002
19
47.4% (9)
52.6% (10)
0
0
62.5%
10003
4
25% (1)
50% (2)
25% (1)
0
25%
10004
14
28.6% (4)
14.3% (2)
50% (7)
1
87.5%
10005
9
11.1% (1)
44.4% (4)
0
4
50%
10010
19
0
5.3% (1)
0
18
25%
10012
19
0
5.3% (1)
47/4% (9)
9
37.5%
10013
9
0
22.2% (2)
0
7
37.5%
10014
34
11.8% (4)
73.5% (25)
2.9% (1)
4
75%
10015
34
14.7% (5)
32.4% (11)
11.8% (4)
14
75%
10016
14
0
35.8% (5)
0
9
25%
10017
4
50% (2)
25% (1)
0
1
50%
10018
4
74% (3)
0
25% (1)
0
75%
Next, the researcher grouped the paraphrase data by science pre-assessment score. That set of
data is found in Table 4.23 below.
Table 4.23. Paraphrase Data by Science Pre-Assessment Score Percentage
Science
Pre-
Assessment
Percentage
Student
Number
(Gender)
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scored
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scoring a 2
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scoring a 1
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scoring a 0
# of
Paraphrases
Previously
Filtered Out
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scored (Not
Filtered)
Below 50%
10003
(F)
4
1
3
0
0
4
10010
(F)
19
0
1
0
18
1
198
10012
(F)
19
0
1
9
9
10
10013
(M)
9
0
2
0
7
2
10016
(F)
14
0
5
0
9
5
Total Below
50%
65
1
12
9
43
22
50% - 74%
10002
(M)
19
9
10
0
0
19
10005
(F)
9
1
4
0
4
5
10017
(M)
4
2
1
0
1
3
Total
50%-74%
32
12
15
0
5
27
75%-100%
10001
(F)
4
2
0
0
2
2
10004
(M)
14
4
2
7
1
13
10014
(F)
34
4
25
1
4
30
10015
(F)
34
5
11
4
14
20
10018
(F)
4
3
0
1
0
4
Total
75%-100%
90
18
38
13
21
69
Only one student with a science pre-assessment score of less than 50% had any paraphrase
quality score of 2, but those students with a science pre-assessment score of less than 50%
collectively scored a paraphrase quality score of 1 on 12 paraphrases. Students scoring 75% and
199
higher on the science pre-assessment collectively earned a quality score of 2 on 18 paraphrases,
and a quality score of 1 on 40 paraphrases. Interestingly, the students could be grouped based on
the number of paraphrases completed; there were five groups of students who had the same
number of paraphrases (19, 34, 14, 4, and 9, respectively). The researcher deemed that a further
analysis was warranted on their scores within the grouping, both individually and collectively by
score.
Three students completed 19 paraphrases: Students 10002, 10010, and 10012. Student
10002, however, had quite different scores on the paraphrase data and the science pre-assessment
data than the other two students. With regard to the paraphrase data, Student 10002 had nine
paraphrases that scored a 2, 10 paraphrases that scored a 1, and no paraphrases that scored a 0 or
were filtered out.
Conversely, neither Student 10010 nor Student 10012 had any paraphrases that earned a
quality score of 2. Those two students collectively had five of their paraphrases which scored a 1,
and seven paraphrases which scored a 0. Student 10010 had 18 paraphrases that were filtered
out, and one paraphrase that earned a score of 1. Student 10012 had nine paraphrases that were
filtered out, one paraphrase that earned a score of 1, and nine paraphrases that earned a score of
0.
With regard to the science pre-assessment data, Student 10002 scored 5 out of 8, or
62.5% on the science pre-assessment, whereas student 10010 scored 2 out of 8, or 25%; and
10012 scored 3 out of 8, or 37.5%, on the science pre-assessment. Student 10010 was one of
three students who scored the lowest on the science pre-assessment, and Student 10012 was one
of two students who had the second lowest scores. It is interesting to note that, although student
200
10002 earned a lower science pre-assessment score than five other of their classmates, this
student had the highest number of paraphrases earning a quality score of 2, had no paraphrase
score of 0, or had any paraphrases filtered out.
Two students, 10014 and 10015, completed 34 paraphrases. They earned a paraphrase
quality score of 2 on a similar number of paraphrases (4 and 5, respectively), but their other
paraphrase quality scores varied rather considerably. Student 10014 earned a paraphrase score of
1 on 25 of their paraphrases, compared to 11 for Student 10015, although it must be noted that
these two students had the highest number of paraphrases of all the students that earned a quality
score of 1. Further, Student 10015 had 14 paraphrases filtered out, compared to just four for
Student 10014. They collectively earned a score of 0 on five paraphrases; Student 10014 earned
that score on only one of their paraphrases, whereas student 10015 earned that score on four of
their paraphrases. Their science pre-assessment scores, on the other hand, were exactly the same
at 75%.
Two students, 10004 and 10016, completed 14 paraphrases, yet their science
pre-assessment scores had the largest difference (87.5% and 25%, respectively). It is well worth
noting that their paraphrase quality scores also differed, although the differences did not fall
within a predictable pattern. Student 10004, who had a science pre-assessment score of 87.5%,
generated four paraphrases with a quality score of 2, compared to zero for Student 10016.
Further, Student 10004 had only one paraphrase filtered out, whereas Student 10016 had nine
paraphrases filtered out. However, Student 10004 had only one paraphrase that earned a quality
score of 1, and had seven paraphrases that earned a quality score of 0. Student 10016 had five
201
paraphrases that earned a quality score of 1, and had no paraphrases that earned a quality score of
0.
Four students, 10001, 10003, 10017 and 10018, completed four paraphrases, and again,
their science pre-assessment scores varied. However, their paraphrases did not fall within a
predictable pattern. Student 10003 earned the lowest science pre-assessment score, which was
25%. However, this student had one paraphrase that earned a quality score of 2, two that earned a
quality score of 1, one paraphrase that earned a quality score of 0, and none that were filtered.
Student 10017 earned a score of 50%. This student had two paraphrases earning a quality score
of 2, one paraphrase that earned a quality score of 1, and one that was filtered out. Two students,
10001 and 10018, scored 75% on their science pre-assessment. Student 10001 earned a quality
score of 2 on two of their paraphrases, and two paraphrases were filtered out. Student 10018 had
three paraphrases that earned a quality score of 2, and none were filtered out.
Two students completed nine paraphrases, Student 10005 and Student 10013, yet their
science pre-assessment scores varied by one question: Student 10005 earned a score of 50%, and
Student 10013 earned a score of 37.5%. Student 10005 had higher paraphrase quality scores than
did Student 10013: this student had one paraphrase that earned a quality score of 2, four that
earned a quality score of 1, and four that were filtered out. Student 10013 had seven paraphrases
filtered out, two paraphrases that earned a quality score of 1, and none that earned a quality score
of 2 or 0.
Following the analysis of paraphrase quality scores to science pre-assessment scores, the
researcher conducted a comparison of each student’s paraphrase data to their reading attitude
survey data.
202
Paraphrase Data and Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Data.
Each student’s paraphrase data were compared to their Elementary Reading Attitude Survey
(ERAS) scores. As previously established through a review of the literature, that reading attitude
can affect reading performance (McKenna & Kear, 1990; Petscher, 2010; Walberg & Tsai, 1985).
Therefore, the reading attitude survey data were compared with students’ paraphrase data to
identify emerging patterns or trends. The students’ paraphrase data, coupled with their reading
attitude scores, are found in Table 4.24 below.
Table 4.24. Comparison of Students’ Paraphrase Data to ERAS Scores
Student
Number
Total
Paraphrases
Scored
% of
Paraphrases
Scoring 2
(#)
% of
Paraphrases
Scoring 1 (#)
% of
Paraphrases
Scoring 0 (#)
# of
Paraphrases
Previously
Filtered Out
Rec.
Reading
Attitude
Score/%
Acad.
Reading
Attitude
Score/%
Total
Reading
Attitude
Score/%
10001
4
50% (2)
0
0
9
32/84th
32/79th
67/83rd
10002
19
47.4% (9)
52.6% (10)
0
0
28/41st
26/46th
43/41st
10003
4
25% (1)
50% (2)
25% (1)
0
28/38th
34/83rd
62/64th
10004
14
28.6% (4)
14.3% (2)
50% (7)
1
31/60th
28/58th
59/59th
10005
9
11.1% (1)
44.4% (4)
0
4
31/57th
32/74th
63/67th
10010
19
0
5.3% (1)
0
18
29/45th
28/52nd
57/48th
10012
19
0
5.3% (1)
47/4% (9)
9
37/90th
35/88th
72/91st
10013
9
0
22.2% (2)
0
7
17/1st
22/22nd
39/6th
10014
34
11.8% (4)
73.5% (25)
2.9% (1)
4
36/88th
34/87th
70/89th
10015
34
14.7% (5)
32.4% (11)
11.8% (4)
14
33/72nd
31/75th
64/75th
10016
14
0
35.8% (5)
0
9
34/78th
37/95th
71/91st
10017
4
50% (2)
25% (1)
0
1
26/26th
28/58th
54/41st
10018
4
74% (3)
0
25% (1)
0
32/66th
24/35th
56/48th
203
Next the researcher grouped the paraphrase data by ERAS total reading score percentile
groups. That set of data can be found in Table 4.25 below.
Table 4.25. Paraphrase Data by Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Total Reading Percentile
Percentile
Group
Student
Number
Total # of
Paraphrases
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scoring a 2
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scoring a 1
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scoring a 0
# of
Paraphrases
Previously
Filtered Out
Total # of
Paraphrases
Scored (Not
Filtered)
Below
50th%
10002
19
2
10
0
0
10010
19
0
1
0
18
10013
9
0
2
0
7
10017
4
2
1
0
1
10018
4
3
0
1
0
Total
Below
50th%
55
14
14
1
26
29
51st-
75th%
10003
4
1
3
0
0
10004
14
4
3
7
1
10005
9
1
4
0
4
10015
34
5
11
4
14
Total
51st-
75th%
61
11
19
12
19
42
76th-
99th%
10001
4
2
0
0
2
10012
19
0
1
9
9
10014
34
4
25
1
4
10016
14
0
5
0
9
204
Total
76th-
99th%
71
6
31
10
24
47
When disaggregating the data by the elementary reading attitude total score and grouping by
percentile group, the data fell into predictable patterns in two areas: total number of paraphrases
and total number of paraphrases scored (not previously filtered out). The group scoring at or
below the 50th percentile on their total reading attitude survey score had the least number of total
paraphrases and the least number of paraphrases scored and not filtered out. The group scoring
between the 76th - 99th percentile on their total reading attitude survey score had the highest
number of total paraphrases and the greatest number of paraphrases scored and not previously
filtered out. The data on the number of paraphrases filtered out, however, did not fall within a
predictable pattern. The students in the group with the highest ERAS percentiles (76th - 99th)
had 24 paraphrases filtered out, which number was the middle of the three groups.
The researcher noticed very interesting dichotomies when analyzing the paraphrase data
to the elementary reading attitude survey data. The first was with Student 10012. This student
had the second highest academic survey score and the highest total reading attitude survey score.
This student generated 19 paraphrases, which was in the top four of all students. However, this
student’s paraphrases were of low quality, with none scoring a 2, only one scoring a 1, nine
scoring a 9, and nine filtered out.
The second observation involved student 10016. This student had the highest academic
survey score and the second highest total reading attitude survey score. This student generated 14
205
paraphrases. However, while none of the student’s paraphrases scored a zero, none scored a 2,
five scored a 1, and nine were filtered out.
The analysis of student 10002 was also notable. The student’s academic survey score was
26, which is three points lower than the mean academic score of the group, and their total survey
score was 54, which is over four points lower than the mean total score of the group. But this
student outperformed all other students in paraphrase quality, with nine paraphrases scoring a 2,
ten paraphrases scoring a 1, and none that scored a zero or were filtered out.
The next data which were analyzed were the student text perception and paraphrase
perception data. These were the data points provided by the students at the end of each text,
when they were asked to rate the text itself and to rate the work of paraphrasing the
predetermined sentences. These data are explored in detail in the next section.
Student Perceptions of Text and Paraphrase Difficulty
Questions were populated in Qualtrics at the end of each text, requiring students to rate
the text itself and the act of paraphrasing the sentences. Ratings were displayed on the screen
both in written form and pictorially: for text ratings, a green happy face stood for “I liked it!”; a
yellow emotionless face stood for “It was okay”; and a red sad face stood for “I didn’t like it.”
For paraphrase ratings, a green happy face stood for “It was easy!”; a yellow emotionless face
stood for “It was okay”; and a red sad face stood for “It was hard.” Students chose the icon that
best matched their opinion. The Qualtrics tool was created in such a way that a student could not
move on to the next text until they rated both the text and the act of paraphrasing sentences. This
text and paraphrase perception data provided the researcher a better understanding of what the
206
students thought about the texts that increased in difficulty, and their thoughts as they
paraphrased increasingly difficult sentences. Students provided a total of 40 ratings each for text
perception and paraphrase perception. Overall text and paraphrase perception data are found in
Appendix K.
There were very few students who indicated that they disliked a passage, even the four
students who completed only one passage. For all eight texts combined, there were only three
ratings of “dislike”; interestingly both of the texts that received that rating were on the topic of
space: “How A Star Is Born” and “A Visit To Mars.” However, those two texts also received two
of the highest percentage of ratings indicating the students “really liked” them, 54% and 57%,
respectively. With regard to the difficulty of the passages, there were four students who indicated
that a text was “hard,” and three texts earned at least one of those ratings, including “How A
Star Is Born” and “A Visit To Mars.” However, those same two texts were rated overall the
easiest to read, with 54% and 57% of the responses, respectively.
Looking at the data by text and student, the rating of “dislike” with regard to the text did
not correlate with the rating of “hard” with regard to paraphrasing the sentences. Further, it is
important to remember that fewer students completed the latter texts as the texts increased in
difficulty. Text and paraphrase perception data by individual student and individual text are
found in Table 4. 26 below. No student read or paraphrased the last text in the set; therefore,
“n/a” was used in the individual tables for any student who did not read or paraphrase the
particular text.
Table 4.26. Text and Paraphrase Perception Data by Student and Individual Text
Text 1: A Star is Born
207
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
ReallyLIked
Easy
Really Like
Easy
10002 (gr 4, M)
Really liked
Easy
Really like
okay
10003 (gr 3, F)
Okay
Hard
Really liked
hard
10004 (gr. 4,M)
Really liked
easy
It was okay
easy
10005 (gr 3, F)
Okay
Okay
Really liked
okay
10010 (gr 3, F)
Didn’t like
Okay
It was okay
okay
10012 (gr 3, F)
Okay
easy
It was okay
okay
10013 (gr 3, M)
Didn’t like
hard
It was okay
okay
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10015 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
okay
Really liked
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
okay
Really liked
okay
10017 (gr 4, M)
Okay
easy
Really liked
easy
10018 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
It was okay
okay
Text 2: Eating Healthy
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10002 (gr 4, M)
Really liked
Okay
Really like
easy
10003 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10004 (gr. 4,M)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10005 (gr 3, F)
Okay
Okay
Really liked
okay
10010 (gr 3, F)
okay
hard
It was okay
okay
10012 (gr 3, F)
Okay
okay
It was okay
okay
10013 (gr 3, M)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
okay
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
208
10015 (gr 4, F)
okay
okay
Really liked
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
okay
Really liked
okay
10017 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10018 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
Text 3: A Visit to Mars
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10002 (gr 4, M)
Really liked
easy
Really like
okay
10003 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10004 (gr. 4, M)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10005 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10010 (gr 3, F)
Didn’t like
hard
It was okay
okay
10012 (gr 3, F)
Okay
okay
Didn’t like
okay
10013 (gr 3, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10015 (gr 4, F)
okay
easy
okay
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
okay
Really liked
okay
10017 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10018 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
Text 4: Wildfires
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10002 (gr 4, M)
Really liked
easy
okay
okay
10003 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
209
10004 (gr. 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10005 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10010 (gr 3, F)
okay
hard
It was okay
okay
10012 (gr 3, F)
Okay
okay
Really liked
okay
10013 (gr 3, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10015 (gr 4, F)
okay
okay
okay
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10017 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10018 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
Text 5: Blood
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10002 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10003 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10004 (gr. 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10005 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10010 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10012 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10013 (gr 3, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10015 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
okay
okay
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10017 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10018 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
210
Text 6: Ostriches
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10002 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10003 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10004 (gr. 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10005 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10010 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10012 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10013 (gr 3, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
10015 (gr 4, F)
okay
okay
okay
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10017 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10018 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
Text 7: Starfish
Student #
Liked text?
Easy to read?
Liked Paraphrasing?
Easy to Paraphrase?
10001 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10002 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10003 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10004 (gr. 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10005 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10010 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10012 (gr 3, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10013 (gr 3, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10014 (gr 4, F)
Really liked
easy
Really liked
easy
211
10015 (gr 4, F)
okay
okay
okay
okay
10016 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10017 (gr 4, M)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
10018 (gr 4, F)
n/a
n/a
n/a
n/a
There were some interesting findings when looking at the data by student. The researcher
compared students who completed a similar number of paraphrases. Four students completed
four paraphrases and only got through one text. All of the paraphrases created by Student 10003
earned a score - one earned a score of 2, two earned a score of 1, one earned a score of zero, and
none were filtered out. However, this student found the text hard to read and the paraphrases
hard to create. Further, although Student 10003 found the text and paraphrase creation hard, they
gave the highest ratings to both the text and the activity of paraphrasing. Student 10001 as well
gave the highest rating possible to both the text and the act of paraphrasing. It was very
interesting that this student completed only one text. Students 10017 and 10018 had nearly equal
yet opposite ratings of the text and paraphrasing; Student 10017 thought the text was just “okay”
but thought it was easy to read; they really liked paraphrasing and thought creating them was
easy. Student 10018 really liked the text and thought it was easy; however, this student gave
ratings of “okay” to the paraphrase activity. They thought that creating the paraphrases was just
okay, and thought that the difficulty of creating them was just okay as well.
Two students completed 19 paraphrases. Nine of the paraphrases created by 10002 earned
a score of 2, ten scored a 1, and none scored a 0 or were filtered out. This student gave the rating
of “really liked” to all of the texts they read, and gave a “really like” rating to the act of
212
paraphrasing for three of the four texts. However, the student rated the ease of paraphrasing as
only “okay” three out of four times. Student 10012 had no paraphrases that scored a 2, one
paraphrase that scored a 1, nine paraphrases that earned a score of 0, and nine were filtered out.
This student rated the act of paraphrasing as “really liked” for only one text. Every other rating
given by this student, whether it was for their liking of the text, the ease of the text, or the ease of
paraphrasing, was “okay.”
Students 10014 and 10015 completed the most texts and created the most paraphrases (34
paraphrases each). They had a similar number of paraphrases that scored a 2 (4 and 5,
respectively). However, Student 10014 had 25 paraphrases that scored a 1, and only four that
were filtered out. This student gave the highest ratings possible to every text, rating all texts and
the acts of paraphrasing as “really liked.” Student 10015, on the other hand, had 11 paraphrases
that scored a 1, and 14 paraphrases that were filtered out. This student rated their liking of the
text and the ease of the text “okay” 12 out of 14 times, equaling 87.5% of the text ratings. The
student also rated their liking of the paraphrase activity and the ease of paraphrasing “okay” 12
of 14 times, again equaling 87.5% of the paraphrase ratings.
Once the text perception ratings and paraphrase ratings were disaggregated by text, the
researcher further analyzed the data by gender. This data set is presented in Table 4.27 below.
Table 4.27. Text and Paraphrase Perception Data By Gender
Text Rating (N=40)
Gender
Number
Really
Liked (%)
Number
Okay (%)
Number
Disliked
(%)
Number
Easy
(%)
Number
Okay (%)
Number
Hard
(%)
Total
Ratings
Male
8 (80%)
1 (10%)
1 (10%)
8 (80%)
1 (10%)
1 (10%)
10
213
Female
14
(46.7%)
14 (46.7%)
2
(6.7%)
11 (36.7%)
15
(50%)
4 (13.3%)
30
Total
22 (55%)
15 (35%)
3 (10%)
19 (47.5%)
16 (40%)
5 (12.5%)
40
Total
Ratings
Really
Liked (%)
Total
Ratings
Okay
(%)
Total
Ratings
Disliked
(%)
Total
Ratings
Easy
(%)
Total
Ratings
Okay
(%)
Total
Ratings
Hard
( %)
Male
36.4
6.7
33.3
42.1
6.3
20
Female
63.6
93.3
66.7
57.9
93.7
80
Paraphrase Rating (N=40)
Gender
Number
Really
Liked (%)
Number
Okay (%)
Number
Disliked
(%)
Number
Easy
(%)
Number
Okay (%)
Number
Hard (%)
Total
Ratings
Male
7 (70%)
3 (30%)
0 (0%)
5 (50%)
5 (50%)
0 (0%)
10
Female
17
(56.7%)
12
(40%)
1
(3.3%)
9
(30%)
20 (66.7%)
1
(3.3%)
30
Total
24 (60%)
15 (37.5%)
1 (2.5%)
14 (35%)
25 (62.5%)
1 (2.5%)
40
Total
Ratings
Really
Liked (%)
Total
Ratings
Okay
(%)
Total
Ratings
Disliked
(% )
Total
Ratings
Easy
(%)
Total
Ratings
Okay
(%)
Total
Ratings
Hard
( %)
Male
29.2
20
0
35.7
20
0
Female
70.8
80
100
64.3
80
1
Female student participants provided more ratings because the two students who had the
most paraphrases were female. They completed more texts. When analyzing the ratings, it would
then make sense that the overall number of ratings would be higher for females. A deeper
analysis was warranted into each of the genders’ individual ratings. The male students as a group
liked both the texts and the paraphrase activity more than the female students did as a group.
214
Male students’ ratings of “really liked” was 80%, compared to only 46.7% of the female text
ratings. Additionally, female students had lower ratings for the ease of the texts: While 80% of
the male student ratings were “easy,” only 36.7% of the female students’ ratings were “easy.”
This is a significant difference; there was a difference of only three ratings between males and
females here, but females had three times the number of overall ratings than males. The findings
are somewhat surprising given that females at both grade levels had higher academic reading
attitude scores than did the males (see Appendix I). While the sample size is small, these findings
do corroborate the research favoring male academic reading attitude.
A similar finding was made for the ease of paraphrasing. Male students’ ratings of “really
liked” was 70%, compared to 56.7% of the female paraphrase ratings. Again, female students
had lower ratings for the ease of paraphrasing: 30% of the female student ratings on paraphrasing
were “easy,” compared with 50% of male students’ paraphrase ratings.
Female students gave a rating of “okay” four times as often as the male students for the
paraphrase activity. When rating the texts, there were 31 ratings of “okay” for whether the
students liked the text and the ease of the text; female students accounted for 29 of those 31
ratings, or 93.5%. Female students also had more ratings of “hard” for both the text and
paraphrase perceptions, and more ratings of “dislike” for the texts and the act of paraphrasing.
Overall, the text perceptions and paraphrase perceptions were positive. Of the 80 total
text perception ratings (40 for how the students liked the text and 40 for the ease of the text),
only 8, or 10%, were negative (that the student disliked the text or found it hard). Similarly with
the paraphrase perception ratings: of the 80 total ratings provided by the students, only two were
negative, or 0.25%. Conversely, 41 of the 80 ratings for text perception, or 51.2%, were very
215
positive (the students really liked the text and they found it easy). Paraphrase perception ratings
were similarly positive. 38 of the 80 ratings for paraphrase perception, or 47.5%, were very
positive.
Analysis of Text and Paraphrase Perception Data with Paraphrase Quality Data
When looking at the text and paraphrase perception data alongside the paraphrase quality
data, there were some predictable patterns. The text perception responses decreased with every
subsequent text until Text 5, “Blood.” At that point, there were only two students left who were
reading and paraphrasing the texts, and they both stopped with “Starfish,” so the last three texts
had the same number of ratings. This researcher wanted to see how the text and paraphrase
perception data compared to the paraphrase quality data. This correlation of data can be found in
Table 4.28 below.
Table 4.28. Comparison of Text and Paraphrase Perception Data to Paraphrase Quality Data
Text #: Text Name
Paraphrase Quality Score N=52 (%)
Text 1:
How A Star is
Born
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
12 (23.1%)
16 (30.8%)
7 (13.4%)
17 (32.7%)
Text Perception Data N=13 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
7 (53.8%)
4 (30.8%)
2 (15.4%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=13 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
8 (61.5%)
5 (38.5%)
0 (0%)
Text 2:
Eating Healthy
Paraphrase Quality Score N=45 (%)
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
216
6 (13.3%)
15( 33.3%)
6 (13.3%)
18 (40.0%)
Text Perception Data N=9 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
5 (55.6%)
4 (44.5%)
0 (0%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=9 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
7 (77.8%)
2 (22.2%)
0 (0%)
Text 3:
A Visit to Mars
Paraphrase Quality Score N=35 (%)
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
3 (8.6%)
13 (37.1%)
9 (25.7%)
20 (28.6%)
Text Perception Data N=7 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
4 (57.1%)
2 (28.6%)
1 (14.3%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=7 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
4 (57.1%)
2 (28.6%)
1 (14.3%)
Text 4:
Wildfires
Paraphrase Quality Score N=25 (%)
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
8 (32.0%)
6 (24.0%)
1 (4.0%)
10 (40.0%)
Text Perception Data N=5 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
2 (40.0%)
3 (60.0%)
0 (0%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=5 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
2 (40.0%)
3 (60.0%)
0 (0%)
Text 5:
Blood
Paraphrase Quality Score N=10 (%)
217
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
1 (10%)
4 (40.0%)
1 (10.0%)
4 (40.0%)
Text Perception Data N=2 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
2 (100%)
0 (0%)
0 (0%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=2 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
1 (50.0%)
1 (50.0%)
0 (0%)
Text 6:
Ostriches
Paraphrase Quality Score N=10 (%)
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
0 (0%)
4 (40.0)
0 (0%)
6 (60.0%)
Text Perception Data N=2 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
1 (50.0%)
1 (50.0%)
0 (0%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=2 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
1 (50.0%)
1 (50.0%)
0 (0%)
Text 7:
Starfish
Paraphrase Quality Score N=10 (%)
# Scoring 2
# Scoring 1
# Scoring 0
# Filtered
2 (20.0%)
4 (40.0%)
0 (0%)
4 (40.0%)
Text Perception Data N=2 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
1 (50.0%)
1 (50.0%)
0 (0%)
Paraphrase Perception Data N=2 (%)
# Really Liked
# Okay
# Disliked
1 (50.0%)
1 (50.0%)
0 (0%)
218
Given that only two students completed the last three texts, the researcher first focused on
the four other texts that were completed by more students. The first text, “How a Star is Born,”
had the highest number of paraphrases and the highest number of perception responses, which is
predictable given it was the first text. It was also the least difficult text, with a Flesch-Kincaid
level of 2.7 (grade 2, seventh month), and strong T.E.R.A. component scores, including 91 for
syntactic simplicity and 90 for deep cohesion. However, this text had neither the highest
percentage of paraphrases scored of the first four texts, nor the lowest percentage of paraphrases
filtered out. Interestingly, “How a Star is Born” also did not have the highest text perception
ratings or the highest paraphrase perception ratings of the first four texts.
Again, the interesting findings came with Text 3, “A Visit to Mars,” and Text 4,
“Wildfires.” Text 3, “A Visit to Mars,” had the lowest number of paraphrase quality scores
earning a 2 of this corpus of four texts, and it was the only text that earned a “dislike” rating
from a student for the paraphrase perception. Although it was the third text presented to students,
with a Flesch-Kincaid score of 3.0 (third grade, zero month), it was a hard text for students.
“Wildfires,” on the other hand, while a text less liked and harder to paraphrase according to the
perception data, had more paraphrase quality scores of 2 than “A Visit to Mars” (8 versus 3,
respectively). “Wildfires” had a high T.E.R.A. component score for syntactic simplicity (90), and
a word concreteness score of 74, which refers to the use of concrete nouns (those things one can
hear, see, taste, smell, touch). It also had the highest deep cohesion component score. This
analysis aligns with the review of literature on text structure and text features (Duke, 2000;
McNamara, et al., 2011; Santoro, et. al, 2016).
219
When comparing the text and paraphrase perception data with the paraphrase quality data
by gender, there were few surprises. Males had more favorable perceptions of the texts and the
paraphrasing, and their paraphrases, while fewer in number, were of higher quality than those
generated by the females. This finding aligns with the review of the literature in that texts that
students find interesting across different cultures and experiences, that reflects the students in the
classroom and draws upon their backgrounds and languages, and with which the students can
identify, has a positive impact on reading achievement and on readers’ self esteem (Barber, et al.,
2018; Ebe, 2010; Ladson-Billings, 1995; Kourea, Gibson, & Werunga, 2017; Tatum, 2006,
Troyer, et al., 2019). See Table 4.26 for a breakdown of text and paraphrase perception data by
text and gender.
It was important to this researcher to gather qualitative data from the students after they
had completed the tasks of reading and paraphrasing the texts presented to them in the Qualtrics
tool. Because the students participating in this study were struggling readers and all remaining
participants were English language learners, this researcher wanted to gain a better
understanding of what the students remembered about the paraphrase strategy and their reading
efficacy as a result of the activity. The data were gathered through a short, one-on-one guided
conversation with each student. The analysis of this qualitative data is provided below.
Qualitative Student Interview Data
After the students completed their text reading and paraphrasing, the researcher
interviewed each student who was still present in the summer targeted services program about
their experience with paraphrasing. Absenteeism continued to be an issue; 11 students were
220
present for this interview, 10 females and one male. Three questions were created: a what
question, a how question, and a why question. The what question would address the students’
understanding of paraphrasing; the how question addressed their mental processes while
paraphrasing, and the why question addressed their reading efficacy. The three questions are as
follows, in the order asked by the researcher:
Question 1: Do you remember what paraphrasing is?
Question 2: How did you figure out how to paraphrase the sentences?
Question 3: Do you think paraphrasing helped you to read better? Why?
Of the 11 students interviewed, four, or 36.4%, could not describe the paraphrasing
strategy. This was an interesting finding by the researcher, as the students had spent time over
two days working through the activity of reading and paraphrasing the texts in Qualtrics. One
student stated at first that they could not remember, but subsequently described the strategy.
Seven students, or 63.6%, could articulate how to paraphrase. One female student in particular
described paraphrasing quite well, utilizing one of the strategies taught in the instructional video,
which was substituting words: “if there’s like a word that you don’t know, you could … put it in
a new word, like if it was like ‘perfect’ you could say...is ‘perfect’ like ‘great’? It is, so instead of
saying ‘perfect’ you can say ‘great’.”
The most interesting finding came with the answers to the why question. Every single
student responded that paraphrasing helped them become a better reader. Three of the 11
students, or 27%, explained that it helped them make more sense of the text. Six of the 11
students, or 54.5%, explained that paraphrasing helped them with words, whether it was putting
221
others’ words into their own words, making the words smaller, or reading and understanding
words better. Student responses to the three interview questions can be found in Appendix L.
Conclusion
As was established at the beginning of this chapter, the majority of students in grades 3
and 4 in the district in which this study took place are not proficient grade level readers. This is
an issue that must be addressed and eradicated, because this grade level band is a critically
important time period in the life of a student. They are expected to learn content from
increasingly complex informational and expository texts that are much more complex in
structure, vocabulary, and content. Additionally, more and more reading is taking place digitally.
However, as was established from a review of the literature, students’ exposure to, and direct
reading instruction using informational texts, and reading in an online format, is not adequate to
ensure students’ success as readers and learners. Further, as was established previously through a
review of the literature, there is an elusive and persistent gap in reading achievement between
White, middle-class students and students of color, students from low socioeconomic families,
and students for whom English is not their first language. Chapter 5 synthesizes the results of
this study and provides recommendations for future research. The chapter outlines the many
limitations of this study that were unforeseen to the researcher when designing this study.
Finally, and for this researcher most importantly, Chapter 5 discusses implications and
possibilities for classroom instruction.
222
CHAPTER FIVE: DISCUSSION, RECOMMENDATIONS, AND IMPLICATIONS
If we teach children today as we taught yesterday, then we rob our children of tomorrow.
-John Dewey
Introduction
The research that took place during the course of this study was born from my desire to
better understand the role of the text and its characteristics in students’ comprehension. Further, I
wanted to better understand the struggle students from different backgrounds and lived
experiences face when “reading to learn.” Finally, I was keenly interested in how students
interact with text in an online format, as new literacies continue to evolve and technology is
changing the way we learn. While research on online and digital reading has been part of the
literature for the past several decades, it is still in its relative genesis because, as Baron (2017)
eloquently states, “digital technology is still in its relative infancy. We know it can be an
incredibly useful educational tool, but we need much more research before we can draw firm
conclusions” (para. 6). The more digital technology evolves, the more it finds its way into our
teaching and learning, and as a district administrator overseeing teaching and learning, I need to
bring the most effective programs, pedagogy, and resources to my district in order to help
students navigate and comprehend the texts they are reading.
As stated in Chapter One, comprehension is the ultimate goal of reading. It is being able
to both understand text and make new meaning from it. Therefore, scaffolding and support for
comprehension early on is critical to later success as a reader, as a communicator, and as a
223
critical thinker. Strong readers read more often, and the amount and the variety of reading in
which they engage directly contributes to their academic achievement skills and their ability to
obtain a good education (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Rapp, van den Broek, McMaster,
Kendeou, & Espin, 2007; Sparks, 2014). Unfortunately, as the reading struggle for many students
persists year after year, the capacity of some students to learn and deeply understand new and
more complex content and topics dwindles. Additionally, poor reading skills can lead to less
motivation to read, which may in turn lead to less time reading and less development of
comprehension skills (Cain & Oakhill, 2011; Guthrie, 2015; Troyer, Kim, Hale, Wantchekon, &
Armstrong, 2019). As a researcher, district leader and educator, I am plagued by the persistent
fact that a majority of students in the district in which this study took place cannot comprehend
grade level text. This trajectory must change, and at a pace not yet realized.
It is for all the reasons mentioned above that I conducted this mixed-methods study,
which was designed to answer the question, What are the essential characteristics of
informational texts that can be used for training reading comprehension strategies in order to
improve the reading comprehension skills of diverse third and fourth grade students? I wanted to
better understand how educators could ensure that the texts used for strategy instruction were
appropriate to the task, the environment, and the child. As the nature of texts continually change
in a digital world, and as we continue to serve an increasingly diverse population of students in
our public schools, the question was further guided by the following sub-questions:
What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion,
topics) for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching
224
reading comprehension strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation,
summarization, and question asking, in a digital format?
What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a
racially and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or
(b) students who read below grade level?
To answer the questions, I used a design-based implementation and mixed-methods
approach for research. Because of the iterative nature of this research study, I collected
qualitative and quantitative data from both practitioners and students. The data collection
methods of a focus group, student reading attitude inventory, observational notes, paraphrasing
quality data, and short student interviews were used to explore answers to the above questions.
The data shed light on basic background knowledge of the students with regard to grade level
science topics, their attitudes toward reading, and how well they were able to paraphrase short
texts that they read. It was important for me as a researcher to include the voice of the educators
and the experiences of the students. These are the voices of the practitioners and the ones for
whom the research will ultimately benefit. Design-based implementation research (DBIR) can be
tested and refined in the classroom where the real differences are found.
The results from this study provided interesting information for future research and
classroom implementation. This chapter presents a discussion of the findings and conclusions
from the research, focusing on each research question. It discusses overarching themes that
emerged from data collection and analysis, and connects those learnings to the extant literature.
Several delimitations and unforeseen limitations to the research design and process both existed
and presented themselves while conducting this research, and are discussed in detail. This
225
chapter further discusses how this study informed the larger research university study for the
creation of the iSTART-Early online comprehension intervention tutoring tool.
Recommendations for future research are offered, especially with regard to
comprehension and vocabulary instruction; gender, grade and informational text; teacher
preparation and the influence of gender; reading intervention; and the use of online text and
digital tools in teaching reading. Beyond recommendations for research, however, the current
research must also inform practice. As I am not only the researcher, but also a public school
district administrator overseeing curriculum and instruction, I offer implications for classroom
practice as they align to current and future research. Finally, I also offer my reflections and
myriad learnings that emerged throughout the process of conducting this study.
Discussion of the Findings
As was established firmly through the review of the literature for this research study,
reading comprehension is complex and is the interaction of a readers cognitive processes on the
text, such as background knowledge, goals and purposes for reading a text (such as for academic
or recreational purposes), and text structure (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018; Pearson &
Cervetti, 2015). Further, a focus on situation, which is an expansion of the idea of context, brings
into play a sociocultural element, which includes an implied or stated task for reading (Fox &
Alexander, 2017; Hartman, Morskin, & Zheng, 2010), and can include such elements as attitude
or motivation, self-efficacy, and content knowledge. The construction integration (C-I) model
(Kintsch & van Kijk, 1978; Kintsch, 1983; Snow, 2002) provided the structure for this research
study in that I was trying to gain a greater understanding of reading comprehension through a
226
more balanced view of identified struggling readers and informational texts they were being
asked to read in the context of summer targeted services. The C-I model provided me with the
framework of understanding when analyzing my findings, as it uses both bottom-up processes,
such as what foundational skills were needed to understand unfamiliar words, and top-down
processes, such as prior knowledge. Further, it helped me to better understand how the students
were showing their comprehension of the texts presented to them through a web-based format.
Essential Characteristics of Informational Texts
The main question guiding this research study focused on essential text characteristics of
informational texts needed in the selection of texts for use in an online reading comprehension
strategy tutoring tool for struggling students in grades three and four. Expository and
informational texts have structures that need to be taught while teaching reading; they have
increasingly complex structures and require more demanding processing skills in order to
comprehend them (McNamara,et al., 2011; Santoro, Baker, Fien, Smith, & Chard, 2016). They
have, as Duke (2000) pointed out, strong noun and verb constructions and text structures that are
unique, such as graphs and diagrams. These are all essential text components students can begin
to learn early on in school. If they do so, this knowledge will lead to strong reading
comprehension later because, as McNamara et al. (2011) emphasize, using knowledge to
comprehend depends in part on the text genre and text features.
For the purposes of this study, a focus group of current teachers and literacy experts was
gathered; they read texts created for this study to identify core parameters for informational text
development and selection. The results of the focus group discussion on essential characteristics
227
of texts revealed a number of themes that were used to create texts for this study. The theme of
text structure had the highest number of mentions, followed by instructional pedagogy,
engagement and vocabulary. Interestingly, the themes of self-efficacy, motivation, and prior
knowledge, while identified as themes throughout the discussion, had the lowest number of
explicit mentions by the focus group. While conducting the review of the literature, however, I
found that prior knowledge was a critical component of successful reading comprehension; a
solid knowledge base of content, vocabulary and text structure helps students navigate
increasingly complex texts that are used in schools to teach content and to build new knowledge
(Afflerback, 1990; Bailey & Heritage, 2008; Goodman, et al., 2017; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2017;
Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Nagy, et al, 2012; Pearson & Billman, 2016; Rupley, 1075;
Willingham, 2017; Willingham, 2006). The fact that these themes had the lowest number of
mentions tells me that we can and should provide more professional learning for teachers on the
importance of these themes in successful reading comprehension.
When creating the texts, structure was a central focus, once determination of the topics
was complete. The texts used for this study were short, with a mean word count of 351.7 and a
mean paragraph count of 6.3. The texts had low Flesch-Kincaid (FK) scores, with the mean FK
level at 4.3 (fourth grade, third month). No text had a Flesch-Kincaid level higher than a 5.5
(fifth grade, fifth month), and all students in the study stopped reading and paraphrasing prior to
reading that text. The first three texts had a mean FK level of 3.1 (third grade, first month). The
informational science texts used for this study were then analyzed using the Coh-Metrix Text
East and Readability Assessor (T.E.R.A.), and when analyzing both the paraphrase quality data
and text and paraphrase perception data, I found the components of the Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. for
228
each text to be invaluable. The texts themselves were presented to the students in order of easiest
to most difficult, based on five T.E.R.A. text dimensions: narrativity, syntactic similarity, word
concreteness, referential cohesion, deep cohesion. Additionally, the Flesch-Kincaid (FK)
readability score of each text was also used as a determinant of ease. Of these components,
generally, the most widely known by staff in the schools is the FK score. However, the other five
are extremely important for practitioners to understand, as informational text has structures that
differ from purely narrative texts, such as short stories and novels, and a student’s
comprehension of a text is influenced by the structure that is used to convey the information
(Kendeou & Van den Broek, 2007; McNamara, et al., 2011).
The Coh-Metrix analysis of the texts used in this study revealed that while the texts were
expository, which is a genre that is generally more difficult to process, comprehend and
remember, they were cohesive, syntactically simple, and had relatively high word concreteness.
Further, the texts had Flesch-Kincaid scores that were appropriate for the grade levels of students
participating in this study. Given this analysis, the texts were appropriate for use in an online
format by the students participating in this study. However, both qualitative and quantitative data
revealed that students struggled to read them.
When analyzing the paraphrase data and looking at the nuances of each text with regard
to Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. data analysis, no one component was found to be a prime factor in the
ability of students to paraphrase the texts. In fact, the component scores that may have predicted
higher paraphrase scores, such as a higher narrativity component score or a high deep cohesion
score, did not positively correlate to higher paraphrase scores. I analyzed the students’
paraphrase scores coupled with the component scores of the reading indices, and I found that a
229
less difficult or complex text structure itself was not enough to ensure student success. The
students needed exposure to many different text structures in order to build their capacity to
navigate through them. The students struggled with all of the texts presented to them, regardless
of the structure.
These findings were of great interest to me as both a teacher and administrator, as they
confirm the review of the literature with regard to the influence of text structure in
comprehension (see Duke, 2000; Kendeou & Van den Broek, 2007; McNamara, et al., 2011).
Successful student comprehension of the texts did not fall within the predictable pattern of
easiest to most difficult text. There were, not surprisingly, nuances in the students’ success with
the texts, which can be partly illustrated by the differences in the structural components of each
text. However, the findings of this study corroborate what the literature continually affirms: the
need for students to be exposed to, and explicitly taught, myriad informational text structures and
syntax as well as the content (Duke, 2000; Greenleaf & Valencia, 2016; Jeong, Gaffney & Choi,
2010; Kendeou & Van den Broek, 2007; Lipson, 1982; McNamara, et al., 2011; Ness, 2011;
Santoro, et al., 2016). Further, the literature affirms the need for students to learn new
information, including text structure and syntax, correctly at the outset in order to mitigate the
need to correct inaccurate information later on (Afflerbach, 1990; Goodman, et al., 2017;
Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Lipson, 1982).
When the text structure is more difficult for or less familiar to the reader, motivation and
engagement in the subject matter or content of the text play an important role in helping students
navigate more complex texts. Therefore, it was important to analyze the data collected on the
students’ perceptions of the texts they were reading and paraphrasing. This was particularly
230
important given the fact that the students had rather limited knowledge of grade level science
concepts, illustrated by a mean percentage score of 49.26% on the science pre-assessment.
Motivation and Engagement With Digital Text
Motivation and engagement play a large role in student success with reading
comprehension (Barber, et al., 2018; Christ & Sharma, 2018; Christenson, 2012; De Naeghel, et
al., 2012; Guthrie, 2015; Guthrie & Cox, 2001; Guthrie & Wigfield, 2000; Guthrie, et al., 2010;
Resley & Christenson, 2012; Skinner & Pitzer, 2012; Troyer, et al., 2019; Unrau & Quirk, 2014;
Varuzza, et al., 2014; Wigfield, et al., 2008). Further, technology can provide help or hindrance
for students, depending on how they have been taught to navigate the environment (Dalton &
Proctor, 2008; Leu, et al., 2013). The qualitative observational data I gathered aligned with the
review of the literature. When analyzing the observational data collected while students were
engaged in particular tasks, I was able to focus on the behaviors of the students. There were
several elements that the data analysis revealed that appeared to affect students’ motivation and
engagement.
Student Engagement in Watching, Reading, and Paraphrasing.
The first activity that the students engaged in was the short instructional video on
paraphrasing, and it appeared that video was an engaging medium for the students. It was noted
that all students but one were intently watching the video, and the one who was distracted was
able to be quickly redirected. Some students watched it more than one time before they began to
read. The qualitative observational data reflected their engagement; the indicator of “Other” on
the student observational form for text reading and responding (see Table 4.13) had the highest
231
single mean percentage of all engagement indicators. This finding was not surprising, as it took
students over four minutes to watch the instructional video one time and during this time it was
noted that only one student was distracted and redirected two times. The students seemed
comfortable and familiar with the format.
Second, engagement and motivation were affected by the act of reading and
paraphrasing. Initially, the students appeared eager to begin reading and paraphrasing, and began
this activity within a reasonable amount of time. However, some students began showing signs
of disengagement nearly right away. For the most part, students were easily redirected to the task
of reading and paraphrasing, as there were few minor distractions in the classrooms themselves.
Analysis of the observational data showed that students spent approximately half of their time
actively engaged in reading and paraphrasing. This finding was not surprising; watching the
instructional video on paraphrasing took time. Also, students also engaged in other activities
related to reading and paraphrasing, such as using a search engine to check the spelling of a
word.
There were other mitigating factors that affected the engagement and motivation of the
students. During the reading of the passages, many students struggled with vocabulary. While the
amount of text displayed at one time on the screen was relatively small, I observed that there
were, at times, more difficult words displayed on the screen that were not defined for the
students until after they had to paraphrase a sentence that included the more difficult vocabulary.
Those students who were more adept in technology skills utilized Spellcheck and a search engine
to both help them understand what a word meant or how to spell it correctly. Some students
simply asked me for help in defining a word. The qualitative observational data showed that
232
many students appeared to concentrate very intently on attempting to spell words correctly,
which was something I did not anticipate observing. It is presumed that the desire to spell words
correctly also slowed them down in their paraphrasing.
Another skill that led to a decrease in the rate of paraphrasing had nothing to do with the
reading of the passage, but affected the creation of paraphrases and again, it was something I did
not anticipate. Nearly all students were not adept in using a keyboard. They appeared to exert
considerable time and effort looking for the right key on the keyboard, which again slowed the
process of paraphrasing considerably. Students who struggled to type while they were reading
the texts appeared to become less engaged in the texts. Two students in the study could not
complete even one text; they simply gave up on the task and asked to be done. Their work was
not captured in the Qualtrics tool.
Texts as Factors in Engagement and Motivation
I analyzed the Coh-Metrix components of each text compared to the students’ perceptions
of the text to gauge a better understanding of engagement. The texts were presented to the
students in the Qualtrics tool in order from least to most difficult. However, the nuances in text
components, such as narrativity (how a text is more or less narrative in nature) and deep
cohesion, (how well the ideas and information of the whole text are connected) may have been a
factor in the unpredictability of paraphrase quality, other than the first text presented. “How a
Star is Born,” the first text presented to the students, had the highest number of paraphrases and
the highest number of text and paraphrase perceptions. More students completed that text than
any other. It was the first text and the first time students engaged in the task of reading and
233
paraphrasing. When looking at the possibility that this was a text students really liked, the text
received two of the three total ratings of “didn’t like”among all texts, and 46% rated it “okay” or
“didn’t like,” which was somewhat in keeping with the ratings of all the texts, although it did
receive two-thirds of the rating of “didn’t like.” Furthermore, given the fact that this text was
considered the easiest of the texts, it was somewhat surprising that this text did not have the
highest percentage of paraphrases scored of the first four texts, or the lowest percentage of
paraphrases filtered out, something that may have been predicted given its relative ease. This
finding seemed to validate the literature emphasizing the need for texts that students like and
want to read.
This study focused on characteristics of texts that could be used in an online
comprehension intervention. Therefore, I wanted to see the role that technology possibly played
in student success. Technology is rapidly changing, and is changing the way students interact
with texts. Focusing on the first sub-question was vital to better understand how the students
interacted and struggled with the texts in an online format, what were reasons for their
experiences, and what factors of being a diverse learner in terms of language, ethnicity, and
socioeconomic status played a role in their success and/or struggle to comprehend.
Essential Text Characteristics in a Digital Environment
The first sub-question guiding this research focused on texts presented to students in an
online format, as the research was informing a larger research project to create an online
comprehension strategy tutoring tool for struggling students in grades three and four. The
question, What are essential text characteristics (length, readability, structure, cohesion, topics)
for the selection of informational texts that can be used for teaching reading comprehension
234
strategies, such as paraphrasing, self-explanation, summarization, and question asking, in a
digital format?, aimed to inform the role of technology in the student experience.
As mentioned previously, technology continues to change how we interact with text;
therefore, literacy continues to evolve (Hartman, et al., 2010; Leu, et al., 2015; Leu, et al., 2013).
Additionally, technology has changed the way we acquire and read information - from
240-character news “articles”, blog posts and text messaging to using the Internet to find and
process information (Goldman, et al., 2012; Leu, et al., 2015). Technology has also enabled
classrooms to utilize programs and other software to provide students with the opportunity for
independence, additional practice, and different formats in which to learn (Dalton & Proctor,
2008; Hooshyar, 2016). However, students must be taught how to use the technology. The term
digital divide defines the inequitable access to computers, the Internet, and other technologies.
This inequitable access results in inequitable technology skills that are needed for online reading.
While some students in this study understood how to access a search engine (a more
advanced online skill), most had extreme difficulty typing (a basic technology skill), which
slowed them down considerably. Some students typed with just one finger, hunting and pecking
to find the correct key to press. Others typed with two fingers; they were a bit faster in
construction of their paraphrases, but not much faster. I observed one student, however, who was
quite familiar with a keyboard. This student was able to complete more texts and generated a
high number of paraphrases. Not all of their paraphrases were of high quality; however, the
student used less cognitive load in the act of typing than did others who were struggling in their
typing.
235
While technology brings exciting opportunities, access to technology is quite limited for
many students, especially those from lower socioeconomic means and low-income students of
color (Auxier & Anderson, 2020). This lack of opportunity continues to disenfranchise already
marginalized students, and my observations of the students interacting with the technology
confirmed the review of the literature on the digital divide. Nearly all struggled with technology.
Diversity of Students in Grades Three and Four
The second sub-question for this research study focused on the students themselves, by
asking, What are additional considerations when selecting appropriate texts for (a) a racially
and ethnically diverse population of third and fourth grade students and/or (b) students who read
below grade level? The students who initially obtained permission to participate in this study
were all in third and fourth grade, the target grades for this research, and all had been identified
by the district as reading below grade level. All but two students qualified for free- or
reduced-price lunch, and all were students of color. Furthermore, all but two students were
English language learners (ELs). Research cited previously (Hoff, 2013; Neuman & Celano,
2001; Neuman & Moland, 2019) pointed out that the literacy achievement gap between these
populations of underserved students and their White, middle-class peers begins in kindergarten
and continues to persist as they matriculate through the grades. Culture and ethnicity play a
central role in engagement and motivation (Bingham & Okagaki, 2012). Research cited
previously (Allington, 2014; Lindsay, 2013; Luo, et al., 2019; McQuillan & Au, 200; Neuman &
Celano, 2001; Neuman & Moland, 2016) cites the need for students of color and low
socioeconomic students to have easy access to books, as access to resources and quality reading
time positively affect reading comprehension, growth, and achievement. The more students read,
236
the more they are building prior knowledge, including vocabulary and exposure to structure and
syntax, and the more they are able to apply that knowledge to new texts in order to comprehend
them (Afflerbach, 1990; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2017; Kendeou & O’Brien, 2016; Pearson &
Billman, 2016; Rupley, 1075; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017; Willingham, 2017;
Willingham, 2006). However, students from low-income families and multilingual learners, often
from low-income families, face challenges in procuring the types of resources needed to build
language and literacy skills. These challenges affect their ability to build the foundational
concepts and knowledge needed to grow in reading success and achievement, including
academic vocabulary (Allington, 2014; Pribesh, Gavigan, & Dickinson, 2011; Townsend, et al,
2020).
My observations of the struggles the students experienced while participating in the
reading and paraphrasing of texts validated the research uncovered through the review of the
literature. As I analyzed my qualitative observational notes, I found many students trying, often
unsuccessfully, to decode a word or figure out its meaning. They were having difficulty, and yet
the literature confirms that both decoding skills and comprehension skills contribute to reading
comprehension performance (Kendeou, et al., 2009; Shanahan & Shanahan, 2008; Storch &
Whitehurst, 2002). Further, EL students have smaller English vocabularies, which will affect
their ability to comprehend a text, and those gaps widen with age (Graves, 2015; Hart & Risley,
1005; Hoff, 2013; White, Graves, & Slater, 1990). This finding from the review of the literature
was evident in this study: students in grade 4 generated over twice the number of paraphrases as
did their third grade counterparts, with over ten times the number of paraphrases earning the
highest score and less paraphrases filtered out.
237
Because the amount of prior knowledge that a student has on a particular subject has such
influence on comprehension of that subject-area text, it was important to gain an understanding
of the students’ background knowledge in grade-level science concepts. When analyzing the
students’ scores on the science pre-assessment, it became clear that students had limited
knowledge of basic grade-level science concepts. In looking at the data, the students who had the
highest science pre-assessment scores (75-100%) also had the highest total number of
paraphrases, and the highest number of paraphrases earning scores of 2 or 1. This group also had
the lowest number of paraphrases that were previously filtered out. Conversely, while the group
of students scoring below 50% on the science pre-assessment had a greater number of total
paraphrases than the group of students scoring from 50%-74% on the science assessment, they
had the lowest number of paraphrases earning a score of 2 or 1 and the highest number of
paraphrases that were previously filtered out. This finding is in line with the review of the
research on prior knowledge and warrants continued research on the effects and successful ways
of building background knowledge of young learners as they continue on their reading journey.
I found that the lack of prior knowledge of science concepts was evidenced also by the
students’ struggle with vocabulary, and this contributed to the students’ struggle with
comprehension and paraphrasing. Knowledge of content-specific vocabulary is crucial to
navigating unfamiliar informational texts in an academic setting (Bailey & Heritage, 2008; Nagy,
et al., 2012), especially for English learners (Graves, 2015; Hart & Risley, 1995; Hoff, 2013;
White, et al., 1990) and this study confirmed the evidence found in the literature: Vocabulary was
one of the biggest obstacles the students encountered when trying to comprehend the texts. This
finding is unsurprising for a few reasons. First, the score of the narrativity component of the
238
Coh-Metrix T.E.R.A. analysis was the lowest score of all dimensions. Low narrativity
component scores are common with expository texts, which typically have a lower narrativity
component (Jackson, et al., 2016) and which have more complex structures and vocabulary that
need to be understood to make meaning (Duke, 2000; Nagy, et al., 2012). Texts that score high in
narrativity may have a relatively large number of more common words and more verbs, which
help students make connections (Jackson, et al., 2016).
Second, both the quantitative and qualitative data gathered in this study revealed the
students’ struggle with the words they were reading, yet strong comprehension skills are linked
to a wide vocabulary and strong understanding of syntax (Anderson & Freebody, 1981; August
& Shanahan, 2006; Chall & Jacobs, 2003; Hoff, 2013; McKenna & Dougherty Stahl, 2015;
Rupley & Nichols, 2005; Stahl & Fairbanks, 1986; Wharton-McDonald & Erickson, 2017). As
the texts presented to the students in this study became more difficult, fewer students completed
them and, for those who did complete them, the number of paraphrases earning higher scores
dwindled as well. This finding confirms the review of the literature which emphasized that a
primary grade focus on foundational reading skills, a greater use of narrative texts, less focus on
comprehension and text structure (especially with informational texts), and less explicit
meaning-making strategy instruction, puts students at a disadvantage (Beerwinkle, et al., 2018;
Chall & Jacobs, 2003;Chall, et al., 1990; Duke, 2000, Jeong, et al., 2010; Strong, 2020).
As mentioned earlier, while conducting observations, I noticed that the text on the screen
that students were asked to paraphrase was presented to them prior to more difficult vocabulary
being defined. In short, the definitions of vocabulary words were presented after the paraphrase
was captured by Qualtrics. This sequence neither helped the students make meaning nor helped
239
them create quality paraphrases. Some students were pointing at the computer screen, trying to
sound out words, asking me what words meant, and even using a search engine. While their
limited vocabulary hindered comprehension of the texts presented to them, it was both
fascinating and encouraging to observe them actively trying to problem solve to continue with
the text. At least initially, most students appeared to be motivated to continue.
Observing this motivation to continue was not lost on me. Engagement and motivation is
more of a concern for students of color (Fredricks, Blumenfeld, & Paris, 2004; Rumberger, 1987;
Rumberger, 1995) and, as stated previously, the students participating in this research study were
all students of color. Furthermore, they were identified by the district as below-grade level
readers, using state and local assessment data. Therefore, it was essential to capture the students’
general attitudes towards reading.
The Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS; McKenna & Kear, 1990; 1999) was
used to gather data on the students’ recreational reading and academic reading attitudes, and
provided an overall reading attitude score. The review of the literature revealed that attitude
impacts reading achievement (McKenna & Kear, 1990; Petscher, 2010; Walberg & Tsai, 1985),
and when analyzing the paraphrase quality data with the ERAS data, findings were predictable in
the total number of paraphrases generated and total number of paraphrases scored (not filtered
out). The group of students scoring between the 76th - 99th percentile on the total reading
attitude survey score generated the highest number of paraphrases and had the lowest number of
paraphrases filtered out, which validates the review of the literature on the positive impact of
reading attitude on comprehension.
240
Disaggregating the data by gender uncovered some very interesting findings. First, girls
had higher Elementary Reading Attitude Survey (ERAS) scores across the board, including
academic reading scores, than did the boys. This was somewhat surprising, given the general
view that boys tend to like non-fiction texts more than girls. While the boys in this study had
lower reading attitude scores than the girls in both grades 3 and 4, they gave higher text
perception ratings to the texts used than did the females, and while they generated fewer
paraphrases, their paraphrases were of higher quality than those generated by the females.
The girls in grade 4 also outperformed the boys on the science pre-assessment, which is
interesting, because it is in keeping with their academic reading attitude at that grade level: there
was also a five point difference between girls and boys in their academic reading attitude scores
in fourth grade. Although boys made up only 30.8% of the students participating in reading and
paraphrasing of texts, they generated nearly 50% of the paraphrases earning a quality score of 2.
Overall, while boys had lower ERAS scores, lower science pre-assessment scores, and lower
numbers of paraphrases, their paraphrase quality was stronger, which translated into better
comprehension of the texts. These findings suggest a potential dissociation between attitudes and
interest: While the boys had lower academic reading attitude scores than the girls, they were
more interested, and ultimately more successful, in navigating the science texts. Thus, when
contemplating informational texts used with third and fourth graders, it would be wise to
consider interests by gender at that age. When analyzing the findings I could not help but wonder
whether other texts related to the same topics would have been more interesting for the girls.
While the boys gave higher text perception ratings to the texts than did the girls, the text
topics were of overall interest to all students as evidenced by their ratings of the texts. Of the 40
241
ratings given on whether or not the students liked the texts, 55% were “really liked” and another
37.5% were “okay.” There were only three ratings of “didn’t like,” constituting 7.5% of the total
ratings. These findings are encouraging; the review of the literature showed that interest
contributes to comprehension (Barber, et al., 2018; Ebe, 2010; Ladson-Billings, 1995; Kourea,
Gibson, & Werunga, 2017; Tatum, 2006, Troyer, et al., 2019), and the texts overall received
positive ratings, which is a factor of text selection for all instructional purposes, but especially
for use with reading comprehension intervention instruction. Populating an intervention tool that
students will access independently with texts that match their interest will aid in the engagement
with the intervention.
Of the 187 total paraphrases that students generated, 31 earned a 2, the highest score,
which equals 16.6% of the total number of paraphrases. Further, 64 paraphrases, or 34.2% of the
total number, earned a paraphrase quality score of 1. Adding these two groups together, 95
paraphrases scored a 1 or 2, which is 50.8%, or slightly over half of all paraphrases generated.
This was a promising number, despite the struggles the students displayed when reading and
paraphrasing the texts.
Both the qualitative and quantitative data confirmed that all of the students did indeed
struggle to read and comprehend the texts presented to them in Qualtrics. However, they also
engaged in behaviors, such as watching the instructional video more than once, sounding out
words, and using a search engine. In my over 25 years in education, I have encountered teachers,
administrators, and other staff who continually view English learners (ELs), students of color,
and students from low socioeconomic families from a deficit lens - that they are somehow unable
to learn at levels of their White, middle class peers. I argue that these preconceived notions
242
perpetuate the reading achievement gap that continues to persist. Furthermore, I argue that the
students in this study were trying hard to be successful.
The belief in students and their efficacy must take a more prominent role in schools,
especially for students who are experiencing structural racism within impoverished communities,
continued negative stereotypes, and systemic racism in their schooling (Reynolds, Sneva, &
Beehler, 2010; Tatum, 2006; Thomas, 2018). Watching the students reading and paraphrasing
texts for this study confirmed to me that prior knowledge and lived experiences are less likely to
be prominent in many of the instructional materials and assessments used in our classrooms
(McCullough, 2013). It is as important for students to make personal connections to their school
experiences and learning as it is to provide students with new experiences and learning (Bishop,
1990; Christ & Sharma, 2018; Tatum, 2006). Additionally, it is imperative that we see them as
capable, resilient, determined learners. The students in this study engaged in behaviors that
showed their determination to succeed. Observing them engaging in this type of
problem-solving, especially utilizing online resources such as search engines, holds great
promise for future research. This view of students is not limited to the grade level students who
participated in this study. We must hold this view of promise for all students, beginning with our
youngest learners. This study, however, focused on third and fourth grade and used specific texts,
which delimitations will be discussed in the next section.
Delimitations to the Research Design
Delimitations to this particular research study are based upon the online reading strategy
tool that will be ultimately created and for which this study provided initial data. This research
243
helped to inform a larger study funded by the Institute for Educational Sciences (IES), which is
being conducted in conjunction with a partnership between two highly-respected research
universities in the United States. This partnership was developed to create a comprehension
strategy tutoring tool that will be ultimately created for third and fourth grade students across the
United States who are struggling with reading comprehension. The tutoring tool will focus
specifically on the comprehension of informational, expository texts, based upon the research
that shows early elementary literacy instruction focuses little on informational texts (Beerwinkle,
et al., 2018; Duke, 2000, Jeong, et al., 2010; Strong, 2020), yet beginning in fourth grade there is
a marked increase of informational texts used to learn content (Castles, Rastle, & Nation, 2018;
Goldman, Snow, & Vaughn, 2016; Guthrie, et al., 2004; Sweet & Snow, 2003). Therefore,
because of the scope of the larger study, the cohort of students participating in this study were
limited to third and fourth grade students. While limiting, this factor helped to focus on two
important grade levels in a student’s academic and literacy trajectory.
A second delimitation was that the texts were limited to expository texts. In the case of
this study, texts focusing specifically on grade level science concepts as determined by the state
science standards. This is an important focus, however, because science is a subject that is
language-rich, requiring students to develop strong background knowledge and academic
language (Nagy, et al., 2012; Townsend, et al., 2020). I chose to use science texts for this study
because limited time has been given to science instruction in the elementary grades and this
provided another avenue for students to read about science topics. The iSTART-Early
comprehension intervention strategy tutoring tool, currently being created by researchers at the
large university with which my district partnered, will focus on expository text, and it will
244
require a large bank of accessible expository texts for students to read, including texts that
continually expose students to science concepts, among others. This will be a very useful tool for
students and teachers.
I was excited for the scope of the i-START-Early research and my contribution to it,
given my background as a strong literacy educator and my position as a district administrator
overseeing K-12 literacy. Further, there was a strong partnership between the district and the
large research university for the past several years, with the district agreeing to be part of
research that focused on the creation of an intervention tool for inference in the primary grades.
My research study was helping to inform texts which would be populated in the iSTART-Early
comprehension intervention tutoring tool, and served research already in progress. When initially
designing this research, neither I nor anyone on the university’s research team could possibly
imagine what would face us beginning in the spring of 2020.
Limitations to the Research Design
The COVID-19 pandemic disrupted the face of K-12 public education, not only in the
United States, but around the world, with 188 countries closing or severely limiting in-person
learning (Hebebci, Bertiz, & Alan, 2020) in the spring of 2020. The district in which this study
took place closed schools and hastily turned to distance learning. Further, the district altered its
summer targeted services program. All of these factors led to significant limitations in both
phases of this research study. The first limitation came with regard to the work of the focus
group.
245
Limitations of Study 1
Focus Group Work. In the case of the district in which this study took place, all
pre-K-12 in-person student learning was suspended beginning on March 17, 2020 (March 15,
2020 personal communication). The district struggled to provide devices to families who needed
them, and reliable access to the Internet in the homes of many district families was a
considerable issue with regard to student learning. The district was able to purchase a limited
number of hotspots, but it was not enough to satisfy the widespread need of families. Those staff
not directly interacting with students on a daily basis, including literacy experts and district
administrators, shifted focus to the creation of math and literacy materials that could be either
picked up by families who did not have access to the Internet, or mailed or driven to their homes.
These materials were created weekly and took up a tremendous amount of time and energy to
produce and distribute.
End-of-year state-required and district-required standardized testing was suspended. This
meant that teachers attempted to assess locally in reading and math; district staff and the literacy
experts, among others, were charged with helping teachers create and administer assessments.
These efforts were not always successful because of mitigating factors, such as sporadic access
to students. The rapid switch to distance learning and all that it required, including the need for
preparation and the full teacher, student and family support limited the focus group in two areas:
first, it limited the number of people who were willing to volunteer to be on the focus group, and
it limited the ability of the focus group to convene more than one time.
Because the focus group could only meet one time, the number of texts initially included
for them to read and review was limited as well. The group was provided with 23 short texts to
read prior to meeting, and they provided feedback on those texts. Because of the inability to meet
again, I coded the feedback to identify themes that I then sent to the text writers. They
246
communicated that this information was enough to create texts to be used for the study.
However, had the focus group been able to convene more than once, I believe that we could have
delved deeper into the themes initially identified to gain a better understanding of what were
important text characteristics to include for this tutoring tool.
Limitations of Study 2
The Summer Targeted Services Program. This study was planned to take place during
summer targeted services at the outset, which was an ideal time to focus on new ways of
teaching and learning. Further, because this study was helping to inform a larger study on
reading intervention, the students participating in summer targeted services were an ideal
population on which to focus. My original plan was to spend time with the students, teaching
them specific reading comprehension strategies and gathering a great deal of observational data
as part of this research. However, due to the continuation of the COVID-19 pandemic, the
traditional summer targeted services program was considerably altered in terms of the following:
the length of the summer program, the length of the day, and the format for learning, which now
was required to include both distance and in-person learning. This meant a significant decrease
in the number of students who attended in person.
The length of the summer targeted services program was reduced by 40%, from five
weeks to three weeks. However, the traditional number of days per week, which was four,
remained the same. The length of the day was reduced by one-third, from six hours to four hours.
Prior to 2020, the typical summer targeted services day started at 8:00 a.m. and ended at 2:00
p.m., with breakfast and lunch served on site, extensive academic programming, and recess.
Because of COVID-19, the hours were 8:00 a.m. until noon. These four hours included breakfast
and recess, but a take-home lunch was given to students as they left for the day.
247
The format of learning was considerably altered as well. The time still included breakfast
and recess as in the past. However, 25% of the summer targeted services instructional time was
devoted to social-emotional learning. This meant a marked decrease in the time devoted to
academic learning, including time for structured literacy work. The abbreviated literacy time
frame meant that, for the purposes of methodology, only one reading strategy could be taught
and assessed to show comprehension of texts created for the purposes of this study.
The decision to provide parents choice of in-person or remote learning for their students
further caused me to choose how I was going to conduct my research. I chose to conduct this
study on the students who were participating in person, rather than remotely, as I had more
control over their physical environment. Working with students in this environment enabled me
to engage with the students physically, encouraging them to stay engaged and helping them with
questions or issues that arose. Also, this environment enabled me to observe students both
individually and collectively. I was, however, only able to work with one reading strategy.
Finally, choice itself provided limitations. Due to the severe restrictions placed on school
districts, the choice given to families with regard to delivery of instruction, and the fear many
families had of their students being in person, there were a considerably smaller number of
students who were available to participate in this study than had been originally planned. Social
distancing considerably restricted the numbers of students attending the summer targeted
services program in person. There was limited space available for in-person instruction. Physical
classrooms for the elementary program were limited to nine students, with desks spaced a
minimum of six feet apart. There were strong recommendations on mask usage.
248
Many families chose to keep their children at home to participate in summer targeted
services remotely, and the decrease in in-person attendance was not consistent across
demographics, with decreases ranging from 49% (White participation) to 85% (students
identifying as two or more races). Students identifying as Hispanic/Latino had the highest
percentage of students participating overall in summer targeted services. Their in-person
numbers, although having decreased by 51% as a result of the choice of in-person or remote
services, was still the largest population attending summer targeted services in person. There
were few students who identified as Asian, White, and identifying as two or more races, which
meant a limited range of demographics of students participating in this study.
Comprehension Intervention Research Focus.
This study provided essential information for the eventual creation of the comprehension
intervention tool for students in grades 3 and 4, called iSTART-Early. This tool has a module
sequence that was designed similarly to its predecessor, iSTART, which was built for older
students of adolescent age and above. In iSTART-Early, there are five modules planned for the
tool. They incorporate six reading comprehension strategies (paraphrasing, comprehension
monitoring, question asking, elaboration, bridging, and summarizing). The five modules are as
follows:
Ask It, which is an overview of comprehension monitoring and question asking;
Reword It, which incorporates the paraphrasing strategy;
Find It, which also works with paraphrasing;
Explain It, which works on elaboration, bridging, question asking, and paraphrasing, and
249
Summarize It, which works on summarization and also includes work on finding the
main idea.
The modules are presented in sequential design, with the strategies building off one another.
Reword It, or paraphrasing, is the first module in which students generate responses (the Ask It
module requires only identification tasks).
Because of the COVID-19 pandemic, the researcher had limited time with the students.
Therefore, the team building the eventual comprehension intervention tool for third and fourth
grade, along with this researcher, made the decision to begin with the lowest level generative
strategy only, which is the paraphrasing strategy. Further, due to the little time I could be in front
of the students, the research team created a short paraphrasing strategy instruction video, which
each student accessed via their computer and a set of headphones. The video was a little over
four minutes long. I observed the students and recorded data as they watched the instructional
video.
The paraphrasing video contained modeling of the strategy, and the video could be
accessed over and over again during their time interacting with the texts. However, it was more
difficult to ascertain the level of understanding and learning of the paraphrasing strategy than it
would have been with live, direct instruction. Because of my experience as a teacher, I would
have preferred to use the gradual release of responsibility strategy through modeling, partnering,
and finally individual practice with the strategy to better assess student understanding. The
natural disconnect created between the student and the teacher as a result of the use of a video
may have impacted motivation, engagement, and ultimately the success of the paraphrasing.
250
Student Participation Due to Absence.
The rapid spread of the virus during what was still an early phase in the pandemic
touched the families and the students who were part of this study. Even during the research phase
of this study, the recommendation of wearing face shields was first promoted and then rescinded.
Staff began the summer targeted services program by wearing face shields, which provided the
opportunity for students to see their teachers’ full faces when talking. Shortly into the summer
targeted services session, however, the recommendation of the use of face shields was replaced
with a recommendation for the use of face masks, by all teachers and students. Young students
were still learning how to wear a mask, and despite their best efforts, it was hard for many young
students to wear the masks properly; the masks also were observed to be somewhat of a
distraction to students who were not used to them. The virus spread very rapidly in the early
summer, and sickness and positive COVID diagnoses of students and/or family members
required a number of students who were attending in person to be absent from in-person
learning, with some of the students, including some of those participating in this research study,
not returning at all.
The absences were more numerous than was typical in a summer targeted services
program in this district (personal communication, June, 2020). Students who began the study
were absent one or more times, or left the summer program altogether. Although 19 students
originally participated in this research study, complete data sets on only eleven students were
possible due to absences. Teachers did not know, from day to day, who would be coming back to
251
school. As the researcher, I was in constant contact with the teachers of the participating
students, and they kept me informed as to the status of the students.
Researcher and Teacher.
I would be remiss not to include the biases that I as a former teacher carry with me that
could have influenced the interpretation of the results of this study. The abbreviated time spent
with students required a change in delivery of instruction on paraphrasing. I was unable to build
strong connections with the students and get to know them better as learners, scaffolding as
appropriate and gathering formative data on their understanding of the paraphrasing strategy.
Having worked in this diverse district for over six years, I understand the role that relationship
plays, especially with students of color.
Because of these limitations, which led to the small number of participants and an
abbreviated time frame, there is an inability to generalize the results of the study. However, the
data obtained from in-person observations, focus group discussion, and short student interviews
provided this researcher with information that helped the large research university in their
creation of iSTART-Early. That information will be shared in the next section. Further, it
provided recommendations for further research on reading comprehension pedagogy and the
selection of texts to utilize with students, as it is imperative that the trajectory for a child who is
struggling in reading be shifted. As has been amply supported by the literature, and observed
firsthand during this study, a child’s struggle to read can lead to a much larger struggle in
learning content. That fact alone was my purpose for research in the first place, and I was
252
fortunate to be able to partner with a large research university in their creation of an online
comprehension tutoring tool that will ultimately help mitigate that struggle.
The Use of This Study in the Creation of the iSTART-Early Interface
Over the last several years, I was honored to have built a strong, collaborative partnership
between the school district used for this study and a major research university located in the
same metropolitan area. This was a partnership that initially focused on the role that inference
plays in successful reading comprehension in the younger grades. The major research university
had created online tools for improving inference-making in reading; a strategy known to increase
reading comprehension (Kendeou, McMaster, & Christ, 2016; Wanzek, et al., 2018).
The strong partnership between the district and the research university provided me the
opportunity to design a study that would help to inform their creation of a new online
comprehension intervention tool titled Interactive Strategy Training for Active Reading and
Thinking for young developing readers (iSTART-Early), designed specifically for students
during a very critical period in reading development: grades three and four. This tool is a version
of the Interactive Strategy Training for Active Reading and Thinking (iSTART) tool, which was
previously developed for older students. The iSTART-Early tool will focus on informational
texts, particularly science texts. Doing so will provide additional instruction and practice for
students who are struggling with the comprehension of more challenging science texts, thus
improving their comprehension and knowledge of the content. The iSTART-Early tool will focus
on the strategies of comprehension monitoring, paraphrasing, inference
(prediction/bridging/elaboration), question asking, explanation, and summarization by providing
253
comprehension instruction and practice that is adaptive in nature and is supplemental to direct
instruction from the classroom teacher. For the purposes of this study, and due to the limited time
spent with students, the decision was made to focus on the paraphrasing strategy.
Paraphrasing is a strategy that can help a reader clarify the meaning of a text, whether it
is a sentence or a paragraph, and thus help improve understanding of main ideas and details in a
text (Hageman, et al., 2012; Nicola, Dascalu, Newton, Orcutt, & McNamara, submitted for
publication). Creating an online tool that would provide feedback to students regarding the
quality of their paraphrases, rather than just letting them know if they paraphrased or not, will
provide an additional layer of understanding for both the student and the classroom teacher.
Using a tool such as this will provide both the student and teacher clarity in how the student
paraphrased the text, such as using semantic similarity or elaboration. The larger research study
needed benchmark information from students to help make decisions on how to populate the
iSTART-Early intervention tool with texts.
The target population for iSTART-Early is students specifically in grades three and four;
therefore, obtaining paraphrases from students in those grades would help build a corpus of
paraphrases that could continue the research on iSTART-Early. This research study on essential
text characteristics provided that opportunity. This study faced significant limitations and
challenges due to the COVID-19 pandemic; however, it obtained a small but significant corpus
of paraphrases which were used in the next steps of iSTART-Early research (Nicula, et al.,
submitted for publication).
The paraphrases that students generated during summer targeted services were used to
create three algorithms designed to assess the quality of student paraphrases, which will then
254
provide immediate feedback to the students when they are working in the iSTART-Early tool.
The original algorithm was built to analyze adult paraphrases, and the researchers tested it with
the paraphrases generated in this study to see if the algorithm worked on children’s responses.
The algorithm was successful on children’s responses. As a result of the paraphrases collected
during this study, analyses were made on those paraphrases that are able to be generalized across
different texts, levels of reading, and language (Nicula, et al., submitted for publication).
Along with the quantitative data collected, the qualitative data compiled through this
study can inform continued research in several different areas, including the behaviors of
students when reading and comprehending, as reading is a complex cognitive process. In the next
section I will discuss recommendations for future research.
Recommendations for Future Research
This study attempted to answer questions related to texts needed for an online reading
comprehension intervention that focused specifically on historically underserved struggling
readers in third and fourth grade. The topic is a significant one in public education, as some
students continue to struggle in proficient reading comprehension. The topic is a timely one as
well, given that digital literacies continue to change the landscape of reading. I will discuss
several recommendations for future research.
First, research on reading comprehension strategies in a general education classroom lag
behind the research on basic reading skills. Therefore, more implementation research is needed
to better understand the impact of teaching reading comprehension strategies. The literature is
replete with evidence that the use of informational text helps students build knowledge across a
255
wide range of content areas and subject matter. However, the review of the literature also shows
that, especially in the primary grades, the explicit use of informational text to teach reading is
still minimal, despite the findings of Nell Duke over 20 years ago. Narrative text is still widely
used for read-alouds and explicit instruction in decoding and fluency. Informational texts,
though, can have complex structures that students must be taught to navigate, and the texts
require the student to utilize multiple comprehension strategies, which they also must be taught
to use when reading. Further, informational text provides students with rich, foundational and
content-specific vocabulary that can be connected to other subjects and other words, and the
texts provide readers with windows to the world they might not otherwise encounter. Conducting
studies that focus on explicit instruction in reading comprehension strategies and skills using
informational text, especially with younger students, would well serve to inform the body of
literature on reading comprehension.
Second, many students who are part of families with limited economic resources,
students of color, and students for whom English is not their first language continue to achieve
much lower than their White, middle class peers. This persistent achievement gap is caused by
factors that, despite best intentions, have not been eradicated. Not all students of color are
struggling readers, but many are, and we need continued research that focuses specifically on
how educators can and are connecting with and engaging struggling readers of color in the
classroom. Oftentimes, instruction and materials, including texts, are not inclusive of or
responsive to many students’ lived experiences and the assets that come with the varying
cultures, languages, genders, and races that are and can be enjoyed in classrooms of diverse
students. Many teachers bring their own class and experience-based beliefs to the classroom, and
256
can often have lower expectations for English learners and other underserved students (Almasi,
et. al, 2010; Emdin, 2016; Jiménez, et al., 2015; Marx, 2000; Moje, 2000; Redding, 2019). A
deeper understanding by teachers of the affective factors that impact reading for students of all
demographics, languages, and lived experiences continue to be warranted. These affective
factors include attitude and motivation, and the role of texts that are culturally relevant, dispel
demographic stereotypes, and empower students to believe in themselves. When analyzing the
qualitative observational data in this study, I was struck by the initial eagerness of the students to
successfully complete the tasks of reading and paraphrasing; for some students that eagerness
waned considerably early on when they realized the task was hard for them. I was also struck by
the determination of some students to persist and problem-solve when they encountered
challenges in making meaning from the texts. Because the texts were presented to the students in
an online format, they had tools at their ready that may not be available when reading printed
text with no device nearby. When observing the students try to use search engines and Google to
help them, I could not help but wonder how teachers can leverage those skills in the classroom. I
also wondered what the teachers would need as far as professional learning to help leverage
those skills that their students have. Studies focusing on students’ reading stamina, persistence,
determination and engagement in informational text, particularly, is warranted, as is a better
understanding of teachers’ perceptions of their students’ assets.
Future research could also focus on the role technology can play in helping to eliminate
gaps between underserved students and their White, middle class peers. Using technology that is
interactive, such as hyperlinks to word definitions, text-to-speech capability, and automated
speech recognition (ASR) for reading response, may provide invaluable scaffolding for
257
struggling readers who spend more effort trying to figure out a word than figuring out a main
idea. As I witnessed firsthand during this research study, if the students had been able to take
advantage of such advances in technology, I can only wonder how the results could have been
different. This is a burgeoning field for researchers. Additionally, exploration of the use of
graphics and gamification (such as the use of leveling up, badges, and tokens) and their effects
on engagement, motivation, and ultimately reading comprehension for struggling readers is a
burgeoning field of research to be explored.
With regard to student use of technology for literacy instruction, the well-quoted adage
in schools that “what gets assessed gets taught” must be considered. State assessments currently
do not assess online, digital reading comprehension, but only offline text skills in a computerized
format. The National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) is still in development of a
new reading framework; therefore, this would be an area of research to provide
recommendations. Since research can and does often influence policy, future research showing
students’ variance in digital versus offline reading comprehension would be warranted. Districts
are under tremendous pressure to raise reading test scores that currently only assess offline skills
(Leu, et al., 2008), yet students are spending increasing amounts of time reading using new
literacies, such as social media, blog posts, and apps. As discussed through the review of the
literature, new literacies present challenges, especially for students who already struggle with
traditional print reading, in terms of fluency and critical reading habits (Dalton & Proctor, 2008).
Students need to engage in specific reading comprehension instruction using online and digital
resources. Whether or not states assess digital literacy skills, research on digital literacy
comprehension would serve to inform the body of literature and teachers in the classroom.
258
Finally, I believe that this study should be replicated on a larger scale. Because of the
varied impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic and the small targeted sample, no finding could be
generalized. However, this study utilized design-based implementation research (DBIR), which
is a research framework that has been recommended for use with literacy (Mills, 2010). The
research design develops both theory and knowledge of students’ learning in the classroom, as
well as implementation of strong instructional practice (Fishman, et al., 2013). There was much
rich data and I was able to glean themes that I want to share with teachers and administrators in
my district to strengthen literacy instruction, especially in the grades leading up to those on
which this study focused. However, if this study could be replicated on a larger scale, with more
students and with more time, I believe we could learn much from the data and analysis that could
have far-reaching implications for future research and classroom practice.
Implications for Classroom Practice
I believe that research without implementation is just fun reading. My deep belief that
research should inform practice is the reason that I conducted a design-based implementation
(DBIR) research study. Designing and conducting a study using DBIR, no matter the limitations,
helped me to see my work as both a researcher and educational leader with fresh eyes, and I am
compelled to use the knowledge that I have gained to help improve classroom practice. Having
the results of research is one thing; seeing those results make a difference in the daily instruction
of teachers and the learning of students is what I believe is most important. I believe that my
research has several implications for classroom practice.
259
There are wide disparities in and low levels of reading achievement in the district in
which this study took place. I have since moved to a different district, and the story is similar:
students of color, students of limited socioeconomic means, and English learners achieve at rates
much lower than their White, middle class counterparts. These realities in different districts
solidify my belief that implementation of the recommendations of reading research in classrooms
is absolutely necessary. “Most education research must be situated - framed locally by the
conditions of class, community, program, local history, gender, teacher, and many other factors
even while employing disciplined, trust-inducing approaches” (Shulman, 1999, p. 163). One of
the reasons for my second sub-question for the research, which focused on racially and ethnically
diverse third and fourth grade readers, was because I could not ignore class, gender, ethnicity,
culture, and socioeconomic influences in my research on texts, just as I cannot ignore these
factors in district decision-making. Not only as a researcher but as a district leader, I am delving
into what I have squarely learned about so far in my research on the history of comprehension,
the construction-integration model of comprehension, that takes into account the interstices of
reader, text, and context/event/experience. I am bringing what I have learned through this
research study to conversations during curriculum review cycles and to professional
development planning for teachers and building and district leaders.
This research study cemented both my agreement with and my commitment to the work.
In other words, research must inform practice and it is the obligation of educators and education
leaders to conduct it, and/or read it, and finally use it both in the classroom and when making
systemic decisions. Researchers, administrators, and classroom teachers need to understand what
works for students - and not just what works, but what works well so that the achievement
260
disparities that continue to persist can be eradicated. To do this, we can and must create systems
that provide time to read research and plan how to implement what we learned from it.
The use of informational text with third and fourth grade readers made clear the
continued need to increase the use of informational text with early and intermediate readers. The
use of informational texts to teach comprehension skills proved successful albeit the small
number of participants. However, the results of this study aligned with the review of the
literature, once again confirming what many educators and researchers know: Reading
instruction with well selected informational texts is necessary. There are young children, much
like my son when he was younger, who love informational text and would choose to read it over
narrative text. Both types of texts are needed for sound literacy instruction at all grade levels.
The texts matter.
One effective instructional method using informational texts is to include them for
explicit reading strategy and skill instruction. Paraphrasing is an extremely important
comprehension skill. Students were engaged in the paraphrasing video used in this study, and
there were promising data on their creation of paraphrases. While this study focused only on
paraphrasing, using informational text to teach a variety of comprehension strategies (such as
comprehension monitoring, inference, question-asking, elaboration and summarizing) is
necessary, as these skills and strategies are needed to fully comprehend informational and
expository text.
Another sound idea is to use informational texts for read-alouds, including modeling of
think-alouds while the teacher is reading. Using informational text for read-alouds takes some
planning, especially with regard to connection across content areas and teachers’ use of
261
think-alouds while they are reading, but the benefits are invaluable. If teachers use informational
text to explicitly teach reading comprehension skills and strategies, they are helping students
gain access to the world of key ideas and details. They are providing opportunities for focused
learning on concepts students may never have known before. Finally, the teachers are helping all
students build vocabularies and an understanding of text structure that helps to ensure students’
ability to traverse complex text structure without having to work mightily to figure out the
meaning of multiple words in a sentence, a struggle I observed the students grapple with in my
research.
Yet another way we can improve the achievement of students in the classroom is to take
advantage of changing technology. I will begin by discussing the Coh-Metrix Text Ease and
Readability Assessor, which is an online tool that is free for educators to use. Before I conducted
this study, I had no idea this tool existed, even though I was a classroom teacher for 13 years and
a building and program coordinator for another three years. It is an easy tool to use: all teachers
need to do is create an account. Once that is done, they can then enter a short passage into the
tool, which will create a profile of the text. The profile includes percentile comparisons to other
texts, and a description of the component dimensions of the text itself. Introducing this tool to
teachers would require some professional learning on their part, but frankly not a lot, and this
would be a great tool for teachers when they are planning for small group reading instruction.
A second way of taking advantage of technology is thoughtful planning for reading
instruction and intervention with the technology at the forefront of the planning for instruction.
The district in which this study took place, and now my new district, transformed into 1:1
districts as a result of the COVID-19 pandemic, meaning that every student, K-12, in the district
262
has a device for learning. This fact alone can and should transform how technology is used in the
classroom for literacy instruction. However, students need to be taught how to use the
technology. I am having this conversation with my district as we plan for fall re-opening. The
district I am currently working in, as did the district in which this research took place, became a
1:1 district because of the COVID-19 pandemic. Having all students back in person provides a
necessary opportunity to think through how we are utilizing technology in the classroom.
Thoughtful planning on the part of the teacher or the grade level team could provide students
with greater and more relevant opportunities to build students’ background knowledge on topics
being read and learned in school, and provide opportunities for integration across content areas.
While conducting my research, I observed students using a search engine to find answers.
Teachers can leverage what students can do with technology, such as using a search engine, to
find, read, and evaluate online texts. Teachers can then plan ways to assess their students’
understanding of the texts they are reading online.
Using technology as a tool for formative and summative assessment of students’
comprehension can be very beneficial for both students and teachers. As students read the texts
in this study, they paraphrased sentences, which were captured in Qualtrics. This was real-time
assessment data that teachers would have at their fingertips. The same can be said about the
ratings that the students gave to both the texts and the paraphrase ease. Qualtrics is used as a
survey tool in both the district where this study took place and also in my current district. It can
provide a real-time dashboard for teachers to see where their students are at and where they are
struggling. As I mentioned earlier, technology can level the playing field for those students
historically underserved in our schools. However, districts must provide online professional
263
development for teachers, as this type of planning has not traditionally been taught in teacher
preparation programs, the tools are newer, and many teachers are not yet familiar with the
endless possibilities that technology can provide for their instruction and student learning.
Finally, it is important to create assessments that are authentic to what it is they are
assessing. In this study, the students struggled to read the science pre-assessment. The goal of
that assessment was to gauge their understanding of science concepts, not their reading ability.
Therefore, both I and their teachers helped them to read the words. If we rely solely on reading
for assessment, such as the use of multiple-choice tests, we miss an opportunity to understand
what our students know.
Concluding Thoughts
I began teaching in 1995, starting out in elementary school and moving to the secondary
level. This experience provided me with a broad range of understanding of what students
experience as they go through their educational journey. In addition to teaching English
Language Arts and literacy, I taught social studies and math to general education students, many
of whom struggled to comprehend texts. It was my responsibility as a teacher to assess what my
students knew, understood, and could do with the content in front of them. The barrier to
comprehension of that content made assessment of their learning much more difficult. While I
focused a considerable amount of my undergraduate learning on literacy, I was more determined
than ever as an educator to continue my journey on helping students to read successfully. My
masters thesis focused on literacy, and this doctoral journey continues that focus, especially as
technology continues to morph at warp speed.
264
This research study cemented in my mind the importance of strong reading
comprehension instruction, beginning in the early years. From the review of the literature, my
observations of struggling readers, and the quantitative research I amassed during this study, I am
fully convinced that as educators our work must not only continue, but we must understand that
the early years in education are crucial to success later on in a student’s academic career.
Instruction in phonics and phonemic awareness, while foundational to reading success, must not
take the place of opportunities to build background knowledge, provide exposure to a wide body
of vocabulary, and create endless opportunities to show young students how they already
comprehend so much. We must build on the funds of knowledge, understanding, and the many
assets that students bring with them everyday to our schools and classrooms.
We must use every minute we have with our students efficiently. In order to help them
make connections in their learning, we must integrate literacy into science, social studies, and
math, and bring knowledge of the world into literacy learning, which means an increase in the
use of informational text in literacy instruction. Furthermore, we must bring students’ knowledge
of their experiences, and their funds of knowledge, into literacy instruction, and connect their
understanding of the world with new knowledge that they are amassing. We must carefully and
intentionally plan learning experiences for students that continue to build their knowledge and
understanding, that help them see themselves as capable and contributing. To do this, we must
intentionally choose materials that not only engage our students but continue to build their
knowledge of the world. The texts matter.
In order to provide opportunities for readers to work on areas of difficulty, we need to
continue to leverage the advantages of technology, and we can do this by teaching literacy skills
265
through printed and digital literacies. We can implement interventions using online, web-based
platforms to take advantage of automated speech recognition, instantaneous definitions of words,
and the ability to listen to the text while reading - all that can be done by an independent learner
while the teacher is working with other students. Utilizing technology in this way can continue to
build what the data in this study showed as important: background knowledge, strong
vocabularies, and strong skill in reading on a computer screen.
When former President Barack Obama spoke to members of the American Library
Association, he remarked, “Reading is the gateway skill that makes all other learning possible”
(Obama, 2005). His words were focused on the change in skills needed for a successful
American workforce in the 21st century - a shift predicated on the revolution of technology and
the global connection requiring strong literacy and communication skills. He called this economy
“the knowledge economy” (Obama, 2005, para.15), and at its center is the ability to read well.
His words, spoken in front of librarians, have been my mantra ever since becoming a teacher.
I believe wholeheartedly that all students can and want to learn, and I am confident that
this research will impact reading instruction in my district. I have been hired to guide the
trajectory of students and, although I am no longer in the classroom, I am responsible for
creating systems that will ensure all students are prepared for a successful life after they
graduate. I feel a personal call to action after having conducted this research that compels me to
continue on my literacy journey.
The third and fourth grade students with whom I interacted in the summer of 2020 will
graduate in 2028 and 2029. I will continue to carry their experiences and their faces with me as I
research and learn, and as I lead. My journey, really, has only just begun. I contemplate Dewey’s
266
words that began this final chapter. As a result of this dissertation journey, I am more equipped
and feel more confident than ever before to create a brighter future, a different tomorrow, for all
of my students, no matter where the journey takes me.
267
REFERENCES
Afflerbach, P. (1990). The influence of prior knowledge on expert readers’ main idea
construction strategies. Reading Research Quarterly, 25(1), 31-46. Retrieved from
JSTOR
Afflerbach, P., Pearson, P. D., and Paris, S. G. (2008). Clarifying differences between reading
skills and reading strategies. The Reading Teacher, 61(5), 364-373.
doi:10.1598/RT.61.5.1.
Akbari, H., Ghonsooly, B., Ghazanfari, M., & Shahriari, H. (2017). Attitude toward reading: L1
or L2 or both. SAGE Open, July - September, 1-10. doi:10.1177(215844017717303).
Alexander, J. E., & Filler, R. C. (1976). Attitudes and reading. Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Allington, R. L. (2014). How reading volume affects both reading fluency and reading
achievement. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 7(1), 13-26.
Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1053794.pdf
Allington, R. L., McGill-Franzen, A., Camilli, G., Williams, L., Graff, J., Zeig, J., Zmach, C.,
Nowak, R., (2010). Addressing summer reading setback among economically
disadvantaged elementary students. Reading Psychology, 31(5), 411-427.
doi:10.1080/02702711.2010.505165
Almasi, J. F., & Fullerton, S. K. (2012). Teaching strategic processes in reading (2nd ed.). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
268
Almasi, J. F., Palmer, B. M., Madden, A., & Hart, S. (2010). Interventions to enhance narrative
comprehension. In A. Gill-Franzen & R. L. Allington (Eds.), Handbook of Reading
Disability Research, pp. 329-344. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.),
Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77-117). Newark, DE: International
Reading Association.
Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading
comprehension. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosenthal (Eds), Handbook
of reading research (pp. 255-291). New York: Longman, Inc. Retrieved from
http://festschrift.pdavidpearson.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/05/1984.Anderson.Pearson.
HRRI_.Ch-9-Schema-Theory.pdf
Anderson, R. C., Hiebert, E. H., Scott, J. A., & Wilkinson, I. A. G. (1985). Becoming a nation of
readers: The report of the Commission on Reading. Washington, DC: U. S. Department
of Education. Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED253865.pdf
Angiulli, A. D.; Siegel, L. S.; & Maggi, S. (2004). Literacy instruction, SES, and word-reading
achievement in English-language learners and children with English as a first language:
A longitudinal study. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 19(4), 202-213.
doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2004.00106.x
Angosto, A.; Sánchez, P.; Álvarez, M.; Cuevas, I.; & León, J. A. (2013). Evidence for top-down
processing in reading comprehension of children. Psicología Educativa, 19, 83-88.
Ardasheva, Y., Norton-Meier, L., & Hand, B. (2015). Negotiation, embeddedness, and
non-threatening learning environments as themes of science and language convergence
269
for English language learners. Studies in Science Education, 51(2), 2-1-249.
doi:10.1080/03057267.2015.1078019
Ash, G. E., & Baumann, J. F. (2017). Vocabulary and reading comprehension: The nexus of
meaning. In S. E. Israel (Ed.) Handbook of Reading Comprehension (2nd. ed., 377-405).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners:
Report of the National Literacy Panel o n language-minority children and youth.
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Auxier, B., & Anderson, A. (March 20, 2020). As schools close due to the coronavirus, some
U.S. students face a digital ‘homework gap’. Fact Tank: News in the Numbers.
Washington, D.C.: Pew Research Center. Retrieved from
https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/03/16/as-schools-close-due-to-the-coronavir
us-some-u-s-students-face-a-digital-homework-gap/
Azevedo, R., & Cromley, J. G. (2004). Does training on self-regulated learning facilitate
students’ learning with hypermedia? Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(3), 523-535.
doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.3.523
Bailey, A. L., & Heritage, M. (2008). Formative assessment for literacy: Building reading and
academic language skills across the curriculum. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.
Banks, J. A. (2003). Teaching literacy for social justice and global citizenship. Language Arts,
(81)1, 18-19.
Barber, M.; Cartledge, G.; Council III, M.; Konrad, M.; Gardner, R.; & Talisman, A. O. (2018).
The effects of computer-assisted culturally relevant repeated readings on English
270
learners’ fluency and comprehension. Learning Disabilities: A Contemporary Journal,
16(2), 205-229. Retrieved from https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1194568
Baron, N. S. (2017). Reading in a digital age. Phi Delta Kappan, 99(2), 15-20. Retrieved on
March 13, 2020, from https://kappanonline.org/reading-digital-age
Batalova, J., Blizzard, B., & Bolter, J. (2020). Frequently requested statistics on immigrants and
immigration in the United States [web page]. Washington, D. C.: Migration Policy
Institute. Retrieved from
https://www.migrationpolicy.org/article/frequently-requested-statistics-immigrants-and-i
mmigration-united-states
Beck, I. L., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing Words to Life: Robust Vocabulary Instruction. New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Beck, I. L., Perfetti, C. A., & McKeown, M. G. (1982). Effects of long-term vocabulary
instruction on lexical access and reading comprehension. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 74(4), 506-521. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Beerwinkle, A. L., Wikjelumar, K., Walpole, S., & Aguis, R. (2018). An analysis of the
ecological components within a text structure intervention. Reading and Writing, 31(9),
2041-2064. doi.org/10.1007/s11145-018-9870-5
Best, R. M., Floyd, R. G., & McNamara, D. S. (2008). Differential competencies contributing to
children’s comprehension of narrative and expository texts. Reading Psychology, 29,
137-164. doi:10.1080/02702710801963951
271
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2011). Language proficiency and its implications for monolingual
and bilingual children. In A.Y. Durgunoğlu, & C. Goldenberg (Eds.), Language and
literacy development in bilingual settings. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 121-138.
Bialystok, E., Majumder, S., & Martin, M. M. (2003). Developing phonological awareness: Is
there a bilingual advantage? Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 27-44.
doi:10.1017/S014271640300002X
Bingham, G. E. & Okagaki, L. (2012). Ethnicity and student engagement. In S. L. Christenson,
A. L. Reschly, & Wylie, C. (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, 65-93.
doi:10/1007/978-1-4614-2018_4
Bischoff, K., & Reardon, S. (2014). Residential segregation by income, 1970-2009. In J. Logan
(Ed.), Diversity and disparities: America enters a new century (pp. 208-233). New York,
NY: Russell Sage.
Bishop, R. S. (1990). Mirrors, windows, and sliding glass doors. Perspectives: Choosing and
Using Books for the Classroom, 6(3). Retrieved from
https://scenicregional.org/wp-content/uploads/2017/08/Mirrors-Windows-and-Sliding-Gl
ass-Doors.pdf
Brown, R. (2017). Comprehension strategies instruction for learners of English: Where we have
been, where we are now, where we still might go. In S. E. Israel (Ed.), Handbook of
research on reading comprehension (2nd ed., 543-567). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Brown, R., Pressley, M., Van Meter, P., & Schuder, T. (1996). A quasi-experimental validation of
transactional strategies instruction with previously low-achieving second-grade readers.
272
Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 18-37. Retrieved from
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED379636.pdf
Butcher, K. R., & Kintsch, W. (2003). Text comprehension and discourse processing. InA. F.
Healy, & R. W. Proctor (Eds.) & I. B. Weiner, (Ed.-in-Chief), Handbook of psychology,
Vol. 4, experimental psychology (28-41). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Buly, M. R.; & Valencia, S. (2003). Meeting the needs of failing readers: Cautions and
considerations for state policy. Retrieved February, 2020, from University of
Washington, Center for the Study of Teaching and Policy website:
https://www.education.uw.edu/ctp/sites/default/files/ctpmail/PDFs/Reading-MRBSV-04-
2003.pdf
Caccamise, D., Franzke, M., Eckhoff, A., Kintsch, E., & Kintsch, W. (2007). Guided practice in
technology-based summary writing. In D. S. McNamara (Ed.), Reading Comprehension
Strategies: Theory, Interventions, and Technologies. Mawah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates Publishing.
Cain, K., & Oakhill, J., (2011). Matthew effects in young readers: Reading comprehension and
reading experience aid vocabulary development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44(5),
431-443. doi:10.1177.9922219411410042.
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor-analytic studies. New York,
NY: Cambridge University Press.
Cartledge, G.; Keesey, S.; Bennett, J. G.; Ramnath, R.; & Council, M. R. III (2016). Culturally
relevant literature: What matters most to primary-age urban learners. Reading & Writing
273
Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 32(5), 399-426.
doi:10.1080/10573569.2014.955225.
Castek, J.; Coiro, J.; Henry, L. A.; Leu, D. J; & Hartman, D. K. (2015). Research on instruction
and assessment in the new literacies of online research and comprehension. In S. R.
Parris & K. Headley (Eds.) Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices
(3rd ed.), 324-339. New York, Guilford Press.
Castek, J.; Zawilinski, L.; McVerry, J.G.; O’Byrne, W. I.; & Leu, D. J. (2011). The new literacies
of online reading comprehension: New opportunities and challenges for students with
learning difficulties. In C. Wyatt-Smith, J. Elkins, & S. Gunn (Eds.), Multiple
perspectives on difficulties in learning literacy and numeracy, pp. 91-110. New York,
NY: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4020-8864-3_4
Castles, A., Rastle, K., & Nation, K. (2018). Ending the reading wars: Reading acquisition from
novice to expert. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 19(1), 5-51.
doi:10.1177/1529100618772271.
Cervetti, G., & Hiebert, E. H. (2015). Knowledge, literacy, and the Common Core. Language
Arts, 92(4), 256-269. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Chall, J. S. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Chall, J. S., & Jacobs, V. A. (2003). The classic study on poor children’s fourth-grade slump.
American Educator. Retrieved from
https://www.aft.org/periodical/american-educator/spring-2003/classic-study-poor-childre
ns-fourth-grade-slump
274
Chall, J. S., Jacobs, V. A., & Baldwin, L. E. (1990). The reading crisis: Why poor children fall
behind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Christ, T., & Sharma, S. A. (2018). Searching for mirrors: Preservice teachers’ journey toward
more culturally relevant pedagogy, Reading Horizons, 57(1), 55-73. Retrieved from
EBSCO.
Cobb, P., Confrey, J., diSessa, A., Lehrer, R., & Schauble, L. (2003). Design experiments in
educational research. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 9-15. Retrieved from JSTOR
Coiro, J. (2020). Toward a multifaceted heuristic of digital reading to inform assessment,
research, practice, and policy. Reading Research Quarterly, 0(0), 1-23.
doi:10.1002.rrq.302.
Coiro, J. (2011a). Predicting reading comprehension on the Internet: Contributions of offline
reading skills, online reading skills, and prior knowledge. Journal of Literacy Research,
43(4), 352-392. doi:10.1177/1086296X11421979
Coiro, J. (2011b). Talking about reading as thinking: Modeling the hidden complexities of online
reading comprehension. Theory Into Practice, 50(2), 107-115.
doi:10.1080/00405841.2011.558435
Coiro, J. (2003). Reading comprehension on the Internet: Expanding our understanding of
reading comprehension to encompass new literacies. The Reading Teacher, 56(5),
458-464. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Coiro, J.; Knobel, M.; Lankshear, C; & Leu, D. J. (2008). Central issues in new literacies and
new literacies research. In J. Coiro, M. Knobel, C. Lankshear, & D. L. Leu (Eds.),
275
Handbook of Research on New Literacies, 1-21. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
Commissioners Office (2020, January). Minnesota Department of Education. Retrieved from
https://education.mn.gov/MDE/about/cmsh/
Common Core State Standards Initiative (n.d.). English language arts standards>Anchor
standards>College and career readiness anchor standards for reading. Retrieved on
November 14, 2020 from http://www.corestandards.org/ELA-Literacy/CCRA/R/
Conley, M. W. (2017). Improving adolescent comprehension: Developing comprehension
strategies in the content areas. In S. E. Israel (Ed.), Handbook of research on reading
comprehension (2nd ed., 335-352). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Cook, A., Pérusse R., & Rojas, E. D. (2012). Increasing academic achievement and
college-going rates for Latina/o English language learners: A survey of school counselor
interventions. The Journal of Counselor Preparation and Supervision, 4(2), 24-40.
Retrieved from
https://repository.wcsu.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?referer=https://scholar.google.com/&http
sredir=1&article=1008&context=jcps
Cooperative Children’s Book Center (October 27, 2020). Books by and/or about Black,
Indigeneous, and People of Color 2018-. Retrieved on November 18, 2020 from
http://ccbc.education.wisc.edu/literature-resources/ccbc-diversity-statistics-by-and-or-abo
ut-poc-2018/
Cooperative Children’s Book Center (n.d.). Services. Retrieved November 18, 2020 from
http://ccbc.education.wisc.edu/about/services.
276
Cooperative Children’s Book Center (June 16, 2020). The numbers are in: 2019 CCBC diversity
statistics. Retrieved on November 18, 2020 from
http://ccblogc.blogspot.com/2020/06/the-numbers-are-in-2019-ccbc-diversity.html
Cresswell, J. W., & Cresswell, J. D. (2018). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative and mixed
methods approaches (5th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications, Inc. Retrieved
November 18, 2020 from
http://ccblogc.blogspot.com/2020/06/the-numbers-are-in-2019-ccbc-diversity.html
Crowe, E. C., Connor, C. M., & Petscher, Y. (2009). Examining the ore: Relations among
reading curricula, poverty, and first through third grade reading achievement. Journal of
School Psychology, 47(3), 187-214. doi:10.1060/j.jsp.2009.02.002
Cunningham, A., & Stanovich, K. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading
experience and ability 10 years later. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 934-945.
Retrieved from EBSCO
Cunningham, A., & Stanovich, K. (2003). Reading can make you smarter! Principal 83(2),
34-39. Retrieved from EBSCO
Dalton, B., & Proctor, C. P. (2008). The changing landscape of text and comprehension in the
age of new literacies. In J. Coiro, M. Knobel, C. Lankshear, & D. J. Leu (Eds.),
Handbook of research on new literacies, 297-324. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum
Associates.
De Naeghel, J., Van Keer, H., Vansteenkiste, M., & Rosseel, Y. (2012). The relation between
elementary students’ recreational and academic reading motivation, reading frequency,
277
engagement, and comprehension: A self-determination theory perspective. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 104(4), 1006-1021. doi: 10.1037/a0027800
Del Giudice, M. (2018). Middle childhood: An evolutionary-developmental synthesis. In N.
Halfon, C. B. Forrest, R. M. Lerner, & E. M. Faustman (Eds.), Handbook of life course
health development. Cham, CH: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-47143-3
Del Giudice, M. (2014). Middle childhood: An evolutionary-developmental synthesis. Child
Development Perspectives, 8(4), 193-200.
Diverse (n.d.). In Cambridge Dictionary. Retrieved from
https://dictionary.cambridge.org/us/dictionary/english/diverse
Duhaylongsod, L., Snow, C. E., Selman, R. L, & Donovan, M. S. (2015). Toward disciplinary
literacy: Dilemmas and challenges in designing history curriculum to support middle
school students. Harvard Educational Review, 85(4), 587-608. Retrieved from
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED574651.pdf
Duncan, G. L., Dowsett, C. J., Claessens, Al, Magnuson, K, Huston, A. C., Klebanov, P., Pagani,
L. S., Feinstein, L, Engel, M., & Brooks-Gunn, J. (2007). School readiness and later
achievement. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1428-1446.
doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1428
Duncan, G. J., Morris, P. A., & Rodrigues, C. (2011). Does money really matter? Estimating
impacts of family income on young children’s achievement with data from
random-assignment experiments. Developmental Psychology, 47(5), 1263-1279.
doi:10.1037/a0023875
278
Durkin, D.(1978). What classroom observation reveals about comprehension instruction.
ReadingResearch Quarterly 14(4), 481-533. Retrieved from
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED162259.pdf
Duke, N. K. (2000). 3.6 minutes per day: The scarcity of informational texts in first grade.
Reading Research Quarterly 35(2), 202-224. Retrieved from JSTOR
Duke, N.K., & Martin, N. M.. (2015). Best practices for comprehension instruction in the
elementary classroom. In S. R. Parris & K. Headley (Eds.), Comprehension Instruction:
Research-Based Best Practices (3rd. Ed.), 211-223. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Eason, S. H., Goldberg, L. F., Young, K. M., Geist, M. C., & Cutting, L. E. (2012). Reader-text
interactions: How differential texts and question types influence cognitive skills needed
for reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 104(3), 515-528.
doi:10.1037/a0027182
Ebe, A. E. (2010). Culturally relevant texts and reading assessment for English language
learners. Reading Horizons, 50(3), 193-210. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Emdin, C. (2016). For white teachers who teach in the hood … and the rest of y’all, too. Boston,
MA: Beacon Press.
Erickson, F. D. (2012). Culture and education (Perspectives in education). In J. A. Banks (Ed.),
Encyclopedia of Diversity in Education, Vol. 1, pp. 560-568. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE
Publishers. doi:10.4135/9781452218533.n177
Evans, M., Kelley, J., Skkora, J., & Treiman, D. (2010). Family scholarly culture and educational
success: Books and schooling in 27 nations. Research in Social Stratification and
Mobility, 28(2), 171-197. doi:/10.1016/j.rssm.2010.01.002
279
Fairbanks, C. M., Cooper, J. E., Webb, S. M., & Masterson, L. A. (2017) Reading
comprehension research and the shift toward culturally sustaining pedagogy. In S. E.
Israel (Ed.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension (2nd ed., 459-478). New
York, NY: Guilford Press.
Fiedler, K., & Beier, S. (2014). Affect and cognitive processes in educational contexts. In R.
Pekrun & L. Linnerbrink-Garcia (Eds.), International Handbook of Emotions in
Education, pp. 36-55. London, U. K.: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Fiester, L. (2010). Early warning: Why reading by the end of third grade matters. Baltimore,
MD: Annie E. Casey Foundation.
Fishman, B. J., Penuel, W. R., Allen, A. R., Cheng, B. H., & Sabelli, N. (2013). Design-based
implementation research: An emerging model for transforming the relationship of
research and practice. Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education,
112(2), 136-156.
Frazier, C. (1997). Cold Mountain. New York, NY: Grove Atlantic.
Fredricks, J. A.; Blumenfeld, P. C.; & Paris, A. H. (2004). School engagement: Potential of the
concept, state of the evidence. Review of Educational Research, 74(1), 59-109. Retrieved
from JSTOR.
Friere, P. (1970). Pedagogy of the oppressed. New York, NY: Continuum.
Fox, E., & Alexander, P. A. (2017). Text and comprehension: A retrospective, perspective, and
prospective. In S. E. Israel (Ed.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension (2nd
ed., 335-352). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
280
Gallego, M., & Hollingsworth, S. (1992). Multiple literacies: Teachers’ evolving perceptions.
Language Arts, 69(3), 206-213. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Gay, G. (2010). Culturally responsive teaching: Theory, research, and practice. New York, NY:
Teachers College Press.
Gee, J. (2003). What video games have to teach us about learning and literacy. New York, NY:
Palgrave Macmillan.
Glass, L. (2019). Reading chess rumble: Engaging disengaged readers through culturally
relevant literature discussions. Journal of Children’s Literature, 45(2), 56-68. Retrieved
from EBSCO.
Goldman, S. R.; Braasch, J. L. G.; Wiley, J.; Graesser, A. C.; Brodowinska, C. (2012).
Comprehending and learning from Internet sources: Patterns of better and poorer
learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(4), 356-381. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Goldman. S. R., & Snow, C. (2015). Adolescent literacy: Development and instruction. In A.
Pollatsek, & R. Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Reading (pp. 463-478). New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Goldman, S. R., Snow, C., & Vaughn, S. (2016). Common themes in teaching reading for
understanding: Lessons from three projects. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy,
60(3), 255-264. doi:10.1002/jaal.586
Goodman, K. S.; Goodman, Y. M.; & Allen, K. L. (2017). Research on helping readers make
sense of print: Evolution of comprehension-based pedagogy. In S. E. Israel (Ed.)
Handbook of Research on Reading Comprehension (2nd ed., 84-106). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
281
Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and
Special Education, 7(1), pp. 6-10. doi:10.1177/074193258600700104. Retrieved from
SAGE Journals.
Graesser, A. C. (2015). Deeper learning with advances in discourse science and technology.
Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 2(1), 42-50.
doi:10.1177/2372732215600888
Graesser, A. C., McNamara, D. S., & Kulikowich, J. M. (2011). Coh-Metrix: Providing
multilevel analyses of text characteristics. Educational Researcher, 40(5), 223-234.
Retrieved from JSTOR.
Graesser, A. C., & Person, N. K. (1994). Question asking during tutoring. American Educational
Research Journal, 31(1), 104-137. doi10.2307/1163269. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Graves, M. F. (2015). Building a vocabulary program that really could make a significant
contribution to students becoming college and career ready. In P. D. Pearson and E. H.
Hiebert (Eds.) Research-based practices for teaching Common Core literacy, 123-142.
New York, NY: Teachers College Press.
Greenleaf, C., & Valencia, S. (2016). Missing in action: Learning from texts in subject-matter
classrooms. In K. A. Hinchman, and D. A. Appelman (Eds.) Adolescent Literacies: A
Handbook of Practice-Based Research, 235-256. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Retrieved from
https://www.researchgate.net/publication/314040868_Missing_in_action_Learning_from
_texts_in_subject-matter_classrooms/link/58b1dd70a6fdcc6f03f9324b/download
282
Guthrie, J. T. (2015). Best practices for motivating students to read. In L. B. Gambrell, & L. M.
Morrow (Eds.), Best practices in literacy instruction (5th Ed.), 61-82. New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Guthrie, J. T., & Cox, K. E. (2001). Classroom conditions for motivation and engagement in
reading. Educational Psychology Review, 13(3), 283-301. doi:10.1023/A:1016627907001
Guthrie, J. T., Hoa, A. L. W., Wigfield, A., Tonks, S. M., Humenick, N. M., & Littles, E. (2007).
Reading motivation and reading comprehension growth in the later elementary years.
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32(3), 282-313.
doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.05.004
Guthrie, J. T., & Wigfield, A. (2000). Engagement and motivation in reading. In M. L. Kamil, P.
B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (3rd ed).,
pp. 403– 422). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishing.
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Barbosa, P., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., Davis, M. H., Scafiddi,
N. T., & Tonks, S. (2004). Increasing reading comprehension and engagement through
concept-oriented reading instruction. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(3), 403-423.
Retrieved from EBSCO.
Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., Humenick, N. M., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., & Barbosa, P.
(2006). Influences of stimulating tasks on reading motivation and comprehension, The
Journal of Educational Research, 99(4), 232-246. doi:10.3200/JOER.99.4.232-246
Hagaman, J. L., & Casey, K. J. (2016). Paraphrasing strategy instruction in content area text.
Intervention in School and Clinic, 52(4), 210-217. doi:10.1177/1053451216659468
283
Hagaman, J. L., Casey, K. J., & Reid, R. (2016). Paraphrasing strategy instruction for struggling
readers. Preventing School Failure, 60(1), 43-52. doi:10.1080/2014.966802
Hagaman, J. L., Casey, K. J., & Reid, R. (2012). The effects of the paraphrasing strategy on the
reading comprehension of young students. Remedial and Special Education, 33(2),
110-123. doi:10.1177/0741932510364548
Halladay, J. L.. (2012). Revisiting key assumptions of the reading level framework. The Reading
Teacher, 66(1), 53.62. doi:10.1002/TRTR.01093
Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young
American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Co.
Hartman, D. K.; Morsink, P. M.; & Zheng, J. (2010). From print to pixels: The evolution of
cognitive conceptions of reading comprehension. In Baker, E. A. (Ed.), The New
Literacies: Multiple Perspectives of Research and Practice. New York, NY:
GuilfordPress.
Hebebci, M. T., Bertiz, Y., & Alan, S. (2020). Investigation of views of students and teachers on
distance education practices during the Coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic.
International Journal of Technology in Education and Science, 4(4), 267-282. Retrieved
from
https://d1wqtxts1xzle7.cloudfront.net/64363593/113-453-2-PB.pdf?1599386328=&respo
nse-content-disposition=inline%3B+filename%3DInvestigation_of_Views_of_Students_
and_T.pdf&Expires=1606249712&Signature=DSsg2iB7xo0qn6Szg6JfTux7qboZVF~F8
CrBOC9vhT~EqPGm3ZRA3yRpF~xf6lSMuAAKtKE5uqlKlt0Ui5YjFrTnuu3oZVRA0x
rvtOnrOpbqnA1bIJ2SAvDuZWad0zOQ~98NZ38gvErTSwuxSRA8NpYYTWx6NewPza
284
NGPZZpf8koh~0lVuI39FV0PCB5cj-v64WZgoebAg670zap0VwJtigNrJd8MzyGtASsd7
uNzvD9pB2oYIth~yfMGmHwEsti13ZFZzLb57Ir8c-BMFu~T64tWSGBGy4txnyMPnO
QlUvxkkC5KOzW5t7OmdFwIPdq8dVuYQrld1dKnNX9aksffQ__&Key-Pair-Id=APKAJ
LOHF5GGSLRBV4ZA
Hebers, J. E., Cutuli, J. J., Supkoff, L. M., Heistad, D., Chan, C., Hinz, E., & Masten, A. S.
(2012). Early reading skills and academic achievement trajectories of students facing
poverty, homelessness, and high residential mobility. Educational Researcher, 41(9),
366-374. Retrieved from JSTOR
Henry J. Kaiser Foundation (2010). Generation M
2:
Media in the lives of 8- to 18-year-olds.
Retrieved from
https://www.kff.org/other/report/generation-m2-media-in-the-lives-of-8-to-18-year-olds/
Herbers, J.E.; Cutuli, J. J.; Supkoff, L. M.; Heistad, D.; Chan, C.; Hinz, E.; & Masten, A. S.
(2012). Early reading skills and academic achievement trajectories of students facing
poverty, homelessness, and high residential mobility. Educational Researcher, 41(9),
366-374. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Hillerich, R. L. (1976). Toward an accessible definition of literacy. The English Journal, (65)2,
50-55. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Hoff, E. (2013). Interpreting the early language trajectories of children from low SES and
language minority homes: Implications for closing achievement gaps. Developmental
Psychology, 49(1), 4-14. doi:10.1037/a0027238
Hooshyar, D., Ahmad, R., Yousefi, M., Fathi, M., Abdollahi, A., Horng, S., & Lim, H. (2016). A
solution-based intelligent tutoring system integrated with an online game-based formative
285
assessment: development and evaluation. Educational Technology Research and
Development, 64(4), 787–808. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11423-016-9433-x
Iltir, I. (2017). Improving the reading comprehension of primary-school students at
frustration-level reading through the paraphrasing strategy training: A multiple-probe
design study. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 20(1), 147-161.
doi:10.26822/iejee.2017131894
Israel, S., & Reutzel, D. R. (2017). The consequential pulse of reading comprehension research.
In S. Israel (Ed.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension (2nd ed., 3-11). New
York, NY: The Guilford Press.
Jackson, G. T., Allen, L. K., & McNamara, D. S. (2016). Common Core TERA: Text ease and
readability assessor. In D. S. McNamara & S. A. Crossley (Eds.), Adaptive educational
technologies for literacy instruction (pp. 49-68). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis,
Routledge.
Jeong, J., Gaffney, J. S., & Choi, J. (2010). Availability and use of informational texts in second-,
third-, and fourth-grade classrooms. Research in the Teaching of English, 44(4), 435-456.
Retrieved from JSTOR.
Jiménez, R. T., David, S., Pacheco, M., Risko, V. J., Pray, L., Fagan, K., & Gonzales, M. (2015).
Supporting teachers of English learners by leveraging students’ linguistic strengths. The
Reading Teacher, 68(6), 406-412. doi: 10.1002/trtr.1289. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Johnson, B. R., & Christensen, L. B. (2017). Educational research: Quantitative, qualitative, and
mixed approaches, Fifth edition. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications. Retrieved from
286
https://ismailsunny.files.wordpress.com/2017/07/educational-research_-quantitat-r-robert-
burke-johnson.pdf
Johnston, P. H. (1984). Assessment in reading. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. Kamil, & P.
Mosenthal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research, 147-182. New York: Longman.
Kapinus, B., & Long, R. (2015). The use of research in federal literacy policies. In P. D. Pearson,
& E. H. Hiebert (Eds.), Research-based practices for teaching Common Core literacy,
25-40. New York, NY: Teachers College, Columbia University.
Keis, R. (2006). From principle to practice: Using children’s literature to promote dialogue and
facilitate the “coming to voice” in a rural Latino community. Multicultural Perspectives,
8(1), 13-19. doi:10/1207/s15327892mcp0801_3
Kendeou, P., McMaster, K. L., & Christ, T. J. (2016). Reading comprehension: Core components
and processes. Policy Insights from the Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 3, 62-69. Doi:
10.1177/2372732215624707
Kendeou, P., & O’Brien, E. J. (2018). Theories of text processing: A view from the top-down. In
M. Schober, D. N. Rapp, & M. A. Britt (Eds.)., Handbook of Discourse Processes (2nd
ed.), 7-21. New York, NY: Routledge Publishing.
Kendeou, P., & O’Brien, E. J. (2017). Reading comprehension theories: A view from the top
down. In M. F. Schober, D. N. Rapp, & M. A. Britt (Eds.), Routledge Handbook of
Discourse Processes (2nd. ed), 7-21. New York, NY: Routledge Publishing.
Kendeou, P., & O’Brien, E. J. (2016). Prior knowledge: Acquisition and revision. In P.
Afflerbach (Ed.), Handbook of Individual Differences in Reading: Text and Context,
151-163. New York, NY: Routledge Publishing.
287
Kendeou, P., & van den Broek, P. (2007). The effects of prior knowledge and text structure on
comprehension processes during reading of scientific texts. Memory & Cognition, 35(7),
1567-1577. doi:10.3758/BF03193491
Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P. White, M. J., & Lynch, J. (2007). Comprehension in preschool
and early elementary children: Skill development and strategy interventions. In D. S.
McNamara (Ed.), Reading Comprehension Strategies: Theory, Interventions, and
Technologies. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erbaum.
Kintsch, W. (1988). The role of knowledge in discourse comprehension: A
construction-integration model. Psychological Review, 95(2), 163-182.
doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.2.163. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. New York, NY: Cambridge
University Press.
Kintsch, W. (2005). An overview of top-down and bottom-up effects in comprehension: The CI
perspective. Discourse processes, 39(2&3), 125-128. Retrieved from
https://condor.depaul.edu/dallbrit/extra/hon207/readings/kintsch-2005-overview-of-top-d
own-and-bottom-up-effects-CI.pdf
Kletzien, S. (2009). Paraphrasing: An effective comprehension strategy. The Reading Teacher,
63(1), 73-77. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Kourea, L., Gibson, L., & Werunga, R. (2017). Culturally responsive reading instruction for
students with learning disabilities. Intervention in School and Clinic, 53(3), 153-162.
doi:10.1177/1053451217702112
288
Krapp, A.; Kidi, S.; & rENNINGER, K. A. (1992). Interest, learning, and development. In K. A.
Renninger, S. Hidi, & A. Krapp (Eds.), The role of interest in learning and development,
3-26. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kulikowich, J. M. (2008). Experimental and quasi-experimental approaches to the study of new
literacies. In J. Coiro, M. Knobel, C. Lankshear, & D. J. Leu (Eds.) Handbook of
Research on New Literacies, 179-205. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Ladson-Billings, G. (1992). Reading between the lines and beyond the pages: A culturally
relevant approach to literacy teaching. Theory Into Practice, 31(4), pp. 312-320.
Retrieved from JSTOR.
Ladson-Billings, G. (1995). But that’s just good teaching! The case for culturally relevant
pedagogy. Theory into Practice, 34(3), 159-165. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Ladson-Billings, G. (2014). Culturally relevant pedagogy 2.0: a.k.a. the remix. Harvard
Educational Review, 84(1), 74-84. doi:10/17783/haer.84.1.p2rj131485484751
Ladson-Billings, G. (2017). Makes me wanna holler: Refuting the “culture of poverty” discourse
in urban schooling. The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social
Science, 673(1), 89-90. doi:10.1177/0002716217718793
Lagemann, E. C. (2000). An elusive science: The troubling history of education research.
Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press.
Lagemann, E. C. & Shulman, L. S. (1999). Issues in education research. Problems and
possibilities. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass Publishers.
289
Landi, N. (2009). An examination of the relationship between reading comprehension,
higher-level and lower-level reading sub-skills in adults. Reading and Writing, 23(6),
701-717. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9180-z
Lankshear, C., and Knobel, M. (2011). New literacies: Everyday practices and social learning.
New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Education.
Lee, C. D. (2020). Diversity and reading comprehension. In E. B. Moje, P. P. Afflerbach, P.
Enciso, & N. K. Lesaux (Eds.) Handbook of Reading Research, Volume V, 37-56. New
York: Routledge.
Leu, D. J. (1997). Caity’s question: Literacy as deixis on the Internet. The Reading Teacher,
51(1), 62-67. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Leu, D. J.; Coiro, J., Castek, J., Hartman, D., Henry, L. A., & Reinking, D. (2008). Research on
instruction and assessment in the new literacies of online reading comprehension. In C.
C. Block and S. Parris (Eds.), Comprehension instruction: Research-based best practices,
(2nd Ed.), 321-345. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Leu, D. J.; Forzani, E., Rhoads, C. Maykel, C., Kennedy, C., & Timbrell, N. (2015). The new
literacies of online research and comprehension: Rethinking the reading achievement
gap. Reading Research Quarterly, 50(1), 37-59. doi:10.1002/rrq.85.
Leu, D. J.; Kinzer, C. K.; Coiro, J.; Castek, J.; & Henry, L. A. (2013). New literacies: A
dual-level theory of the changing nature of literacy, instruction, and assessment. In D. E.
Alvermann, N. J. Unrau, & R. B. Ruddell (Eds.), Theoretical models and processes of
reading, 1150-1181. New York, NY: Guilford Press. doi:10.1177/002205741719700202
290
Leu, D. J.; McVerry, J. G.; O’Byrne, W. I.; Kiili, C.; Zawilinski, L.; Everett-Cacopardo, H.;
Kennedy, C.; & Forzani, E. (2011). The new literacies of online reading comprehension:
Expanding the literacy and learning curriculum. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy,
55(1), 5-14. doi:1598/JAAL.55.1.1
Lewis, O. (1966). The culture of poverty. Scientific American, 215(4), 19-25. Retrieved from
JSTOR.
Lipson, M. Y. (1982). Learning new information from text: The role of prior knowledge and
reading ability. Journal of Reading Behavior, 14(3), 243-261.
doi:10.1080/10862968209547453
Logan, S. & Johnston, R. (2009). Gender differences in reading ability and attitudes: Examining
where these differences lie. Journal of Research in Reading (32)2, 199-214.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.01389.x
Lovejoy, A. (2013). A governors guide to early literacy: Getting all students reading by third
grade. Washington, DC: National Governors Association Center for Best Practices.
Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED583168.pdf
Luo, R., Tamis-LeMonda, C. S., & Mendelsohn, A. L. (2020). Children’s literacy experiences in
low-income families: The content of books matters. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(2),
213-233. doi:10.1002/rrq.263
Marsh, J. (2011). Young children's literacy practices in a virtual world: Establishing an online
interaction order. Reading Research Quarterly, 46(2), 101-118. doi:10.1598/RRQ.46.2.1
291
Martínez, R. S.; Aricak, O. T.; & Jewell, J. (2008). Influence of reading attitude on reading
achievement: A test of the temporal-interaction model. Psychology in the Schools,
45(10), 1010-1022. doi:10.1002/pits20348. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Marx, S. (2000). An exploration of pre-service teacher perceptions of second language learners
in the mainstream classroom. Texas Papers in Foreign Language Education, 5(1),
207-221. Retrieved from https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED444962.pdf
Massey, D. D., & Miller, S. D. (2017). Self-regulation and reading comprehension: Moving
beyond the individual’s cognition in regulated learning. In S. E. Israel (Ed.), Handbook of
research on reading comprehension (2nd ed., 293-315). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Maxwell, J. A. (2013). Qualitative research design: An interactive approach (3rd ed.). Los
Angeles, CA: SAGE Publications.
McCullough, R. G. (2013). The relationship between reader response and prior knowledge on
African American students’ reading comprehension performance using multicultural
literature. Reading Psychology, 34(5), 397-435. doi:10.1080/02702711.2011.643531.
McKenna, E. (1997). Gender differences in reading attitudes. Kean College of New Jersey.
Retrieved from http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED407653.pdf
McKenna, M. C., Conradi, K., Lawrence, C., Jang, B. G., & Meyer, J. P. (2012). Reading
attitudes of middle school students: Results of a U. S. Survey. Reading Research
Quarterly, 47(3), 283-306. doi:10.1002/rrq.021.
McKenna, M. C., & Dougherty Stahl, K. A. (2015). Assessment for reading instruction (3rd Ed.).
New York, NY: Guilford Press.
292
McKenna, M. C., & Kear, D. J. (1999). Garfield revisited: Unlimited extension of permission to
copy the ERAS. The Reading Teacher, 53(3), 244-244. Retrieved from JSTOR.
McKenna, M. C., & Kear, D. J. (1990). Measuring attitudes toward reading: A new tool for
teachers. The Reading Teacher, 43(9), 626-639. Retrieved from JSTOR.
McKenna, M. C., Kear, D. J., & Ellsworth, R. A. (1995). Children’s attitudes toward reading: A
national survey. Reading Research Quarterly, 30(4), 934-956. Retrieved from JSTOR.
McKeown, M. G.; Beck, I. L.; & Blake, R. G.K. (2009). Rethinking reading comprehension
instruction: A comparison of instruction for strategies and content approaches. Reading
Research Quarterly, 44(3), 218-253. doi:10.1598/RRQ.44.3.1
McKeown, M. G.; Beck, I. L.; Omanson, R. C., & Perfetti, C. A. (1984). The effects of
long-term vocabulary instruction on reading comprehension: A replication. Journal of
Reading Behavior, 15(1), 3-18. doi:10.1080/10862968309547474
McMillan, J. H., & Schumacher, S. (2010). Research in education: Evidence-based inquiry (7th
Ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education, Inc.
McNamara, D. S., Boonthum, C., Levinstein, I. B., & Millis, K. (2007a). Evaluating
self-explanations in i-START: Comparing word-based and LSA algorithms. In T.
Landauer, D. S. McNamara, S. Dennis, & W. Kintsch (Eds.), Handbook of Latent
Semantic Analysis, (pp. 227-241). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Publishing.
McNamara, D. S., Ozuru, Y., & Floyd, R. G. (2011). Comprehension challenges in the fourth
grade: The roles of text cohesion, text genre, and readers’ prior knowledge. International
293
Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 4(1), 229-257. Retrieved from
https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/EJ1070456.pdf
McQuillan, J., & Au, J. (2001). The effect of print access on reading frequency. Reading
Psychology, 22(3), 225-248. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Merriam-Webster (n.d.). Culture. In Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary. Retrieved November 18,
2020 from https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/culture.
Mezynski, K. (1983). Issues concerning the acquisition of knowledge: Effects of vocabulary
training on reading comprehension. Review of Educational Research, 53(2), 253-279.
Mills, K. A. (2010). A review of the “digital turn” in the New Literacy Studies. Review of
Educational Research, 80(2), 246-271. doi:10.3102/0034654310364401
Minnesota Department of Education (2020). Commissioners office [web page]. Retrieved on
March 1, 2020 from: https://education.mn.gov/MDE/about/cmsh/
Minnesota Department of Education (2020). English learner education [web page]. Retrieved on
October 22, 2020 from https://education.mn.gov/MDE/dse/el/
Minnesota Department of Education (2010). English language arts [web page]. Retrieved on
May 26, 2020 from https://education.mn.gov/MDE/dse/stds/ela/
Minnesota Department of Education (2020). Minnesota Report Card. Retrieved on July 20, 2020
from
https://rc.education.mn.gov/#assessmentsParticipation/orgId--10280000000__groupType-
-district__test--MCA-III__subject--R__accountabilityFlg--FOC_NONE__year--trend__g
rade--all__p--22
294
Mohd-Asraf, R., & Abdullah, H. (2016). Elementary schoolers’ attitudes toward reading in
English: How boys feel relative to girls. English Language Teaching, 9(6), 134-140.
doi:10.5539/elt.v9n6p134. Retrieved from https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ1101194.
Moje, E. B. (2008). Foregrounding the disciplines in secondary literacy teaching and learning: A
call for change. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 52(2), 96-107. Retrieved from
JSTOR.
Moje, E. B. (2005). “To be part of the story”: The literacy practices of gangsta adolescents.
Teachers College Record, 102)3, 651-690. Retrieved from
http://www-personal.umich.edu/~moje/pdf/Journal/ToBePartoftheStory.pdf
Moje, E. B., Ciechanowski, K., Kramer, K., Ellis, L., Carrillo, R., & Collazo, T. (2004). Working
toward third space in content area literacy: An examination of everyday funds of
knowledge and discourse. Reading Research Quarterly, 39(1), 38-70. Retrieved from
JSTOR.
Mokhtari, K., Kymes, A., & Edwards, P. (2009). Assessing the new literacies of online reading
comprehension: An informative interview with W. Ian O’Byrne, Lisa Zawilinski, J. Greg
McVerry, and Donald J. Leu at the University of Connecticut. The Reading Teacher
62(4), 34-357. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Mol, S. E., & Bus, A. G. (2011). To read or not to read: A meta-analysis of print exposure from
infancy to early adulthood. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 267-296.
doi:10/1037/a0021890
Montoya, S. (2018, October 17-18). Defining literacy [PowerPoint slides]. Global Alliance to
Monitor Learning, Fifth Meeting. Hamburg, Germany: United National Educational,
295
Scientific, and Cultural Organization. Retrieved from
http://gaml.uis.unesco.org/wp-content/uploads/sites/2/2018/12/4.6.1_07_4.6-defining-lite
racy.pdf
Nagy, W., Townsend, D., Lesaux, N., & Schmitt, N. (2012). Words as tools: Learning academic
vocabulary as language acquisition. Reading Research Quarterly, 47(1), 91-108.
Retrieved from JSTOR.
Nation, K. (2005). Children’s reading comprehension difficulties. In M. Snowling, & C. Hulme
(Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook, 248-265. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
National Assessment for Educational Progress (2009). NAEP questions tool. Retrieved from
https://nces.ed.gov/NationsReportCard/nqt/Search#
National Assessment Governing Board. (2019). Reading framework for the 2019 National
Assessment of Educational Progress. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Education.
Retrieved from:
https://www.nagb.gov/content/nagb/assets/documents/publications/frameworks/reading/2
019-reading-framework.pdf
National Center for Education Statistics (2019). Adult literacy in the United States. Washington,
DC: Institute of Education Sciences, U. S. Department of Education. Retrieved from:
https://nces.ed.gov/datapoints/2019179.asp
National Center for Education Statistics (2019). Back to school statistics. Washington, DC:
Institute of Education Sciences, U. S. Department of Education. Retrieved on November
16, 2020 from https://nces.ed.gov/fastfacts/display.asp?id=372#PK12_enrollment
296
National Center for Education Statistics (2019). National student group scores and score gaps.
NAEP Report Card: Reading. Washington, DC: Institute of Education Sciences, U. S.
Department of Education. Retrieved from
https://www.nationsreportcard.gov/reading/nation/groups/?grade=4
National Center for Education Statistics (2019). The nation’s report card: 2019 NAEP reading
assessment. Washington, DC: Institute of Education Sciences, U. S. Department of
Education. Retrieved from https://www.nationsreportcard.gov/highlights/reading/2019/
National Center for Education Statistics (November 13, 2019). NAEP history and innovation.
Retrieved from https://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/about/timeline.aspx
National Center for Education Statistics (2015). Reading and math score trends. Washington,
DC: Institute of Education Sciences, U. S. Department of Education. Retrieved from
https://nces.ed.gov/programs/coe/indicator_cnj.asp
National Council of Teachers of English (2020, January 20). Re: Literacy in a digital age (blog
post). Retrieved from
https://ncte.org/blog/2020/01/january-2020-nctechat-literacy-in-a-digital-age/
National Reading Panel (U.S.), & National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(U.S.). (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching children to read: An
evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its
implications for reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Washington, DC: National
Institute of Child Health and Human Development, National Institutes of Health.
Retrieved from
https://www.nichd.nih.gov/sites/default/files/publications/pubs/nrp/Documents/report.pdf
297
Ness, M. (2011). Explicit reading comprehension instruction in elementary classrooms: Teacher
use of reading comprehension strategies. Journal of Research in Childhood Education
25(1), 98-117. doi:10.1080/02568543.2010.531076
Ness, M. (2011). Teachers’ use of and attitudes toward informational text in K-5 classrooms.
Reading Psychology, 32(1), 28-53. doi:1080/02702710903241322
Neuman, S. B., & Celano, D. (2001). Access to print in low-income and middle-income
communities: An ecological study of four neighborhoods. Reading Research Quarterly,
36(1), 8-26. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Neuman, S. B., & Moland, N. (2019). Book deserts: The consequences of income segregation on
children’s access to print. Urban Education, 54(1), 126-147.
doi:10.1177/0042085916654525
Neumann, A., Pallas, A. M., & Peterson, P. L. (1999) Preparing education practitioners to
practice education research. In C. E. Lagemann, & L. S. Shulman (Eds.), Issues in
education research: Problems and possibilities, 247-285. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass
Publishers.
Nicula, B., Dascalu, M., Newton, N., Orcutt, E., & McNamara, D. S. (submitted for publication).
Automated paraphrase quality assessment using recurrent neural networks and language
models. Journal of INtelligent Transportation Systems.
Oakhill, J. V., Cain, K., & Bryant, P. E. (2003). The dissociation of word reading and text
comprehension: Evidence from component skills. Language and Cognitive Processes
18(4), 443-468. doi:10.1080/01690960344000008
298
Obama, B. (2005, June 25). Literacy and education in a 21st century economy. Retrieved on
April 17, 2021 from
http://obamaspeeches.com/024-Literacy-and-Education-in-a-21st-Century-Economy-Oba
ma-Speech.htm
Organisation for Economic Cooperation and Development (2011). Context of the PISA digital
reading assessment. In PISA 2009 results: Students on line: Digital technologies and
Performance, Vol. VI. Paris, FR: OECD Publishing. doi:10.1787/97892641192995-5-en
Orellana, J. F., Reynolds, J., & Martínez, D. C. (2010). Cultural modeling: Building on cultural
strengths as an alternative to remedial reading approaches. In A. Gill-Franzen, & R. L.
Allington (Eds.) Handbook of Reading Disability Research, pp. 273-278. Retrieved from
http://ebookcentral.proquest.com
Ortlieb, E., & Schotz, S. (2020). Student’s self-efficacy in reading - connecting theory to
practice. Reading Psychology, 41(7), 735-751. doi: 10.1080/02702711.2020.1783146
Owens, A., Rearson, S. F., & Jencks, C. (2016). Income segregation between schools and school
districts. American Educational Research Journal, 53(4), 1159-1197.
doi:10.3102/0002831216652722
Palmer, B.C., Shakelford, V. S., Miller, S. C., & Leclere, J. T. (2007). Bridging two worlds:
Reading comprehension, figurative language instruction, and the English-language
learner. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 50(4), 258-267.
doi:10.1598/JAAL.50.4.2
Pardo, L. S. (2004). What every teacher needs to know about comprehension. The Reading
Teacher, 58(3), 272-280. doi:10.1598/RT.58.3.5
299
Paris, S. G., & Hamilton, E. E. (2009). The development of children’s reading comprehension. In
S. E. Israel & G. G. Duffy (Eds.), Handbook of research on reading comprehension,
32-53. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Pearson, P. D. (2007). An endangered species act for literacy education. Journal of Literacy
Research, 39(2), 145-162. doi:10.1080/10862960701331878
Pearson, P. D., & Billman, A. (2016). Reading to learn science: A right that extends to every
reader- expert or novice. In Z. Babaci-Wilhite (Ed.), Human rights in language and
STEM education, 17-34. Boston, MA: Sense Publishers.
doi:10.1007/978-94-6300-405-3_2
Pearson, P. D., & Cervetti, G. N. (2017). The roots of reading comprehension. In S. E. Israel
(Ed.), Handbook of Reading Comprehension, (2nd. ed., 12-56). New York, NY: The
Guilford Press.
Pearson, P. D.., & Cervetti, G. N. (2015). Fifty years of reading comprehension theory and
practice. In D. P. Pearson, & E. H. Hiebert (Eds.), Research-based practices for teaching
Common Core literacy, 1-24. New York, NY: Teachers College, Columbia University.
Penuel, W. R., Fishman, B. J., Cheng, B. H., & Sabelli, N. (2011). Organizing research and
development at the intersection of learning, implementation, and design. Educational
Researcher 40(7), 331-337. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Perewardy, C. (1993). Culturally responsible pedagogy in action: An American Indian magnet
school. In E. Hollings, J. King, & W. Hayman (Eds.), Teaching diverse populations:
Formulating a knowledge base (pp. 77-92). Albany, NY: State University of New York
Press.
300
Perfetti, C. A. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of
Reading, 11(4), 357-383. doi: 10.1080/10888430701530730
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2002). The lexical quality hypothesis. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbr, & P.
Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy (pp. 189-212). Amsterdam,
Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Perfetti, C. A., & Hart, L. (2001). The lexical basis of comprehension skill. In D. S. Gorfein
(Ed.), Decade of behavior. On the consequences of meaning selection: Perspectives on
resolving lexical ambiguity (p. 67–86). American Psychological Association.
doi.org/10.1037/10459-004
Perfetti, C. A., & Stafura, J. (2014). Word knowledge in a theory of reading comprehension.
Scientific Studies of Reading, 18(1), 22-37. doi:10/1080/10888438.2013.827687
Petscher, Y. (2010). A meta-analysis of the relationship between student attitudes towards
reading and reading achievement. Journal of Research in Reading, 33(4), 335-355.
doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2009.01418.x
Pew Research Center (March 16, 2020). As schools close due to the coronavirus, some U. S.
students face a digital ‘homework gap’ [web page]. Retrieved from
https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/03/16/as-schools-close-due-to-the-coronavir
us-some-u-s-students-face-a-digital-homework-gap/
Pressley, M., & Allington, R.L. (2015). Reading instruction that works (4th ed.). New York, NY:
Guilford Press.
Pribesh, S., Gavigan, K., & Dickinson, G. (2011). The access gap: Poverty and characteristics of
school library media centers. Library Quarterly, 81(2), 143-160. Retrieved from JSTOR.
301
RAND Reading Study Group (2002). Reading and understanding: Toward an R&D program in
reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation.
Rapp, D. N., & van den Broek, P. (2005). Dynamic text comprehension: an integrative view of
reading. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(5); 276-279.
doi:0.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00380.x. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Rapp, D. N., van den Broek, P., McMaster, K. L., Kendeou, P., & Espin, C. A. (2007).
Higher-order comprehension processes in struggling readers: A perspective for research
and intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11(4), 289-312.
doi:10.1080/10888430701530417
Reardon, S. F., & Portilla, X, A. (2016). Recent trends in income, racial, and ethnic school
readiness gaps at kindergarten entry. AREA Open, 2(3), 1-18. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE
Publications.
Redding C. (2019). A teacher like me: A review of the effect of student-teacher racial/ethnic
matching on teacher perceptions of students and student academic and behavioral
outcomes. Review of Educational Research, 89(4), 499-535. doi::
10.3102/0034654319853545.
Reschly, A. L, & Christenson, S. L. (2012). Jingle, jangle, and conceptual haziness: Evolution
and future directions of the engagement construct. In S. L. Christenson, A. L. Reschly, &
Wylie, C. (Eds.), Handbook of Research on Student Engagement, (pp. 3-20). New York,
NY: Springer.
302
Reynolds, A. L., Sneva, J. N., & Beehler, G. P. (2010). The influence of racism-related stress on
the academic motivation of Black and Latino/a students. Journal of College Student
Development, 51(2), 135-149. doi:10.1353/csd.0.0120
Rich, A. (1986). Blood, bread, and poetry: Selected prose 1979-1985. New York, NY: Norton &
Company.
Rideout, V., & Katz. V. (2016). Opportunity for all? Technology and learning in lower-income
families. A Report of the Families and Media Project. New York: The Joan Ganz Cooney
Center at Sesame Workshop. Retrieved November 18, 2020 from
https://www.joanganzcooneycenter.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/01/jgcc_opportunityfor
all.pdf
Roehler, L. R., & Duffy, G . G. (1984). Direct explanation of comprehension processes. In G. G.
Duffy, L. R. Roehler, & J. Mason (Eds.) Comprehension instruction: Perspectives and
suggestions (pp. 265-280). New York: Longman.
Rogers, S. E. (2016). Bridging the 21st century digital divide. Tech Trends: Linking Research
and Practice to Improve Learning, 60, 197-199. doi: 10.1007/s11528-016-0057-0
Roser, M.; Ritchie, H.; & Ortiz-Ospina, E. (2020). Internet [web page]. Published online at
OurWorldInData.org. Retrieved from: https://ourworldindata.org/internet
Rumberger, R. W. (1987). High school dropouts: A review of issues and evidence. Review of
Educational Research, 57(2), 101-121. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Rumberger, R. W. (1995). Dropping out of middle school: A multilevel analysis of students and
schools. American Educational Research Journal, 32(3), 583-625. Retrieved from
JSTOR.
303
Rupley, W. H., & Nichols, W. D. (2005). Vocabulary instruction for the struggling reader.
Reading and Writing Quarterly, 21(3), 239-260. doi:10.1080/10573560590949368
Santoro, L. E., Baker, S. K., Fien, H., Smith, J. L. M., & Chard, D. J. (2016). Using read-alouds
to help struggling readers access and comprehend complex, informational text. Teaching
Exceptional Children, 48(6), 282-292. doi:10.1177/00040059916650634
Sarroub, L., & Pearson, P. D. (1998). Two steps forward, three steps back: The stormy history of
reading comprehension assessment. The Clearing House, 72(2), 97-105.
doi:10.1080/00098659809599604
Schleicher, A. (2018). PISA 2018: Insights and Interpretations [pdf]. Paris, FR: Organisation for
Economic Cooperation and Development. Retrieved from:
https://www.oecd.org/pisa/PISA%202018%20Insights%20and%20Interpretations%20FI
NAL%20PDF.pdf
Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, C. (2008). Teaching disciplinary literacy to adolescents: Rethinking
content-area literacy. Harvard Educational Review, 78(1), 40-59. Retrieved from
https://dpi.wi.gov/sites/default/files/imce/cal/pdf/teaching-dl.pdf
Shuman, A. (1986). Storytelling rights: The uses of oral and written texts by urban adolescents.
Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Slavin, R. E., & Cheung, A. (2005). A synthesis of research on language instruction for English
language learners. Review of Educational Research, 75(2), 247-284. Retrieved from
JSTOR.
304
Shotter, E. R., & Raynor, K. (2015). The work of the eyes during reading. In A. Pollatsek & R.
Treiman (Eds.), The oxford handbook of reading, 44-59. Oxford, UK: The Oxford
University Press.
Simpson, A. (1996). Fictions and facts: An investigation of reading practices of girls and boys.
English Education, 28(4), 268-279. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Skinner, E. A., & Pitzer, J. R. (2012). Developmental dynamics of student engagement, coping,
and everyday resilience. In S. L. Christenson, A. L Reschly, & C. Wylie (Eds.)
Handbooks of research on student engagement. New York, NY: Springer.
doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-2018-7
Smith, M. C. (1990). A longitudinal investigation of reading attitude development from
childhood to adulthood. The Journal of Educational Research, 83(4), 215-219. Retrieved
from JSTOR.
Snow, C. E. , Science and Technology Policy Institute (Rand Corporation), & United States
(2002). Reading for understanding: Towards a research and development program in
reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: Rand. Retrieved from
http://rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/monograph_reports/MR1465/MR1465.ch2.pdf
Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Learning to read with a language impairment. In M. J.
Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook, pp. 397-412. Oxford,
UK: Blackwell Publishing.
Solheim, O. J. (2011). The impact of reading self-efficacy and task value on reading
comprehension scores in different item formats. Reading Psychology (32)1, 1-27.
doi:10.1080/02702710903256601
305
Sparks, R. L., Patton, J., & Murdoch, A. (2014). Early reading success and its relationship to
reading achievement and reading volume: Replication of ‘10 years later’. Reading and
Writing (27)1, 189-211. doi: 10.1007/s11145-013-9439-2
Spilich, G. J., Vesonder, G. T., Chiesi, H. L., & Voss, J. F. (1979). Text processing of
domain-related information for individuals with high and low domain knowledge.
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18(3), 275-290.
doi.org/10.1016/S0022-5371(79)90155-5
Spring, J. (2018). American education (18th ed.). New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.
Squire, K. D. (2008). Video-game literacy: A literacy of expertise. In J. Coiro, M. Knobel, C.
Lankshear, and D. J. Leu (Eds.), Handbook of research on new literacies, 635-669. New
York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Stahl, S. A., & Fairbanks, M. M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model-based
meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research 56(1), 72-110. Retrieved from
http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.968.2217&rep=rep1&type=pdf
Steinhuehler, C. (2008). Cognition and literacy in massively multiplayer online games. In J.
Coiro, M. Knobel, C. Lankshear, & D. J. Leu (Eds.) Handbook of research on new
literacies, 611-634. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishing.
Stemler, S. E. (2007). Cohen’s kappa. In N. Salkind (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Measurement and
Statistics, Vol. 1, pp. 164-166. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.
Stemler, S. E. (2004). A comparison of consensus, consistency, and measurement approaches to
estimating interrater reliability. Practical Assessment, Research, & Evaluation, 9(4),
1-19. Retrieved from Directory of Open Access Journals (DOAJ)
306
Stevens, K. C. (1980). The effect of background knowledge on the reading comprehension of
ninth graders. Journal of Reading Behavior, XXII(2), 151-154.
doi:10.1080/10862968009547365
Storch, S. A., & Whitehurst, G. J. (2002). Oral language and code-related precursors to reading:
Evidence from a longitudinal structural model. Developmental Psychology, 38(6),
934-947. doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.38.6.934
Strømsø, H. I., Bråten, I., & Samuelstuen, M. S. (2003). Students’ strategic use of multiple
sources during expository text reading: A longitudinal think-aloud study. Cognition and
Instruction, 21(3), 113-147. doi:10.1207/S1532690XCI2102_01
Strong, J. Z. (2020). Investigating a text structure intervention for reading and writing in grades 4
and 5. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(4), 545-551. doi:10.1002/rrq.356
Swalander, L., & Taube, K. (2007). Influences of family-based prerequisites, reading attitude,
and self-regulation on reading ability. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32(2),
206-230. doi:10.1016/j/cedpsych.2006.01.002
Sweet, A. P., & Snow, C. E. (2003). Rethinking reading comprehension. New York: Guilford
Press.
Tatum, A. W. (2006). Engaging African American males in reading. Educational Leadership,
63(5), 44-49. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Taylor, B. M., Frye, B. J., & Maruyama, G. M. (1990). Time spent reading and reading growth.
American Educational Research Journal, 27(2), 351-362.
doi:10.3102/00028312027002351.
307
Textcompare.org (2020). Flesch-Kincaid grade level readability. Retrieved February 11, 2021
from https://www.textcompare.org/readability/flesch-kincaid-grade-level.
Thomas, K. L. (2018). Building literacy environments to motivate African American boys to
read. The Reading Teacher, 72(6), 761-765. doi:10.1002/trtr.1784.
Tong, F., Irby, B. J., Lara-Alecio, R., & Koch, J. (2014). Integrating literacy and science for
English language learners: From learning-to-read to reading-to-learn. The Journal of
Educational Research, 107: 410-426. doi:10.1080/00220671.2013.833072
Townsend, D., Barber, A. T., & Carter, H. (2020). Learning academic language, comprehending
text. In E. B. Moje, P. P. Afflerbach, P. Enciso, and N. K. Lesaux (Eds.), Handbook of
Reading Research, Volume V., 345-364. New York, NY: Routledge.
Troyer, M., Kim, J. S., Hale, E., Wantchekon, K. A., & Armstrong, C. (2019). Relations among
intrinsic and extrinsic reading motivation, reading amount, and comprehension: A
conceptual replication. Reading and Writing, 32(5), 1197-1218.
doi:10.1007/s11145-018-9907-9 Retrieved from EBSCO.
Turnbill, J. (2002). The four ages of reading philosophy and pedagogy: A framework for
examining theory and practice. Reading Online, 5(6). Retrieved from
https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Jan_Turbill/publication/265118209_The_Four_Ages
_of_Reading_Philosophy_and_Pedagogy_A_Framework_for_Examining_Theory_and_P
ractice/links/56aee5d508ae19a3851639aa/The-Four-Ages-of-Reading-Philosophy-and-Pe
dagogy-A-Framework-for-Examining-Theory-and-Practice.pdf
308
United States Department of Education (n.d.) . Academic performance and outcomes for English
learners [web page]. Retrieved from
https://www2.ed.gov/datastory/el-outcomes/index.html
United States Department of Education (n.d.). Our nation’s english learners [web page].
Retrieved June 15, 2020 from
https://www2.ed.gov/datastory/el-characteristics/index.html
Unrau, N. J., & Quirk, M. (2014). Reading motivation and reading engagement: Clarifying
commingled conceptions. Reading Psychology, 35(3), 260-284.
doi:10.1080/02702711.2012.684426
van den Broek, P. (2010). Using texts in science education: Cognitive processes and knowledge
representation. Science, 328, 453-456. Retrieved from
https://folk.uib.no/gmset/sciencewriting/documents/mental%20representation%20of%20t
exts.pdf
van den Broek, P., Kendeou, P., Lousberg, S., & Visser, G. (2011). Preparing for reading
comprehension: Fostering text comprehension skills in preschool and early elementary
school children. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education 4(1), 259-268.
Retrieved from http://files.eric.gov/fulltext/EJ1068603.pdf
van Dijk, T. A., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York, NY:
Academic Press. Retrieved from
http://www.discourses.org/OldBooks/Teun%20A%20van%20Dijk%20%26%20Walter%2
0Kintsch%20-%20Strategies%20of%20Discourse%20Comprehension.pdf
309
van Dijk, J. A. G. M, & van Deursen, A, J. A. M. (2014). Digital skills: Unlocking the
information society. New York, NY: Palgrave McMillian. doi:10.1057/9781137437037.
Varuzza, M., Sinatra, R., Eschenauer, R. & Blake, B. E. (2014). The relationship between
English language arts teachers’ use of instructional strategies and young adolescents’
reading motivation, engagement, and preference. Journal of Education and Learning,
3(2), 108-119. doi:10.5539/jel.v3n2p108
Wade, Q. D. (2012) The relationship between reading attitude, self-efficacy, motivation, and the
reading achievement of fifth grade African-American males (Order No. 3513527).
Howard University, ProQuest Dissertations Publishing. Retrieved March 1, 2021 from
https://ezproxy.hamline.edu/login?url=https://www-proquest-com.ezproxy.hamline.edu/di
ssertations-theses/relationship-between-reading-attitude-self/docview/1027145572/se-2?a
ccountid=28109
Walberg, H. J., & Tsai, S. (1985). Correlates of reading achievement and attitude: A national
assessment study. Journal of Educational Research, 78(3), 159-157. Retrieved from
JSTOR.
Wanzek, J., Martinez, L., Fall, A., Roberts, G., Stillman, S., & Kent, S. C. (2018). Text reading
supports in social studies content instruction and their relationship to student knowledge
acquisition. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 34(4), 349-360.
doi:10.1080/10573569.2018.1446858
Wharton-McDonald, R., & Erickson, J. (2017). Reading comprehension in the middle grades. In
Israel, S. E. (Ed.), Handbook of Research in Reading Comprehension, (2nd ed., 353-376).
New York, NY: The Guilford Press.
310
What gets tested gets taught: Cautions for using college admissions tests in state accountability
systems (March, 2018). Retrieved from
https://www.achieve.org/files/CollegeAdmissionsExamBrief2018.pdf
White, G. E. (2017). The dissertation warrior: The ultimate guide to being the kind of person
who finishes a doctoral dissertation or thesis. Happy Valley, OR: Triumphant Heart
International, Inc.
White, T. G., Graves, M. F., & Slater, W. H. (1990). Growth of reading vocabulary in diverse
elementary schools: Decoding and word meaning. Journal of Educational Psychology,
82(2), 281-290. Retrieved from EBSCO.
Wigfield, A., Guthrie, J. T., Perencevich, K. C., Taboada, A., Klauda, S. L., McRae, A., &
Barbosa, P. (2008). Role of reading engagement in mediating the effects of reading
comprehension instruction on reading outcomes. Psychology in the Schools, 45(5),
432-445. doi10.1002/pits.20307
Wigfield, A., Mason-Singh, A., Ho, A. N., & Guthrie, J. T. (2014). Intervening to improve
children’s reading motivation and comprehension: Concept-oriented reading instruction.
In Sa. A. Karabenick, & T. C. Urdan (Eds.), Advances In Motivation and Achievement,
Vol. 18 (pp. 37-70). Bingley, U. K.: Emerald Group Publishing, Ltd.
Wijekumar, K. K., Meyer, B. J. F., & Lei, P. (2012). Large-scale randomized controlled trial with
4th graders using intelligent tutoring of the structure strategy to improve nonfiction
reading comprehension. Educational Technology Research and Development 60(6),
987-1013. doi:10.1007/s11423-012-9263-4
311
Williams, J. P. (2015). Reading comprehension instruction: Moving into a new era. In D. P
Pearson, & E. H. Hiebert (Eds.), Research-based practices for teaching Common Core
literacy, 79-92. New York: Teachers College Press.
Willingham, D. T. (2017). The reading mind: A cognitive approach to understanding how the
mind reads. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Willingham, D. T. (2006). The usefulness of brief instruction in reading comprehension
strategies. American Educator, 30(4), 39-50.
World Literacy Foundation (2018). The economic & social costs of illiteracy: A white paper by
the World Literacy Foundation. Retrieved from
https://worldliteracyfoundation.org/wp-content/uploads/2019/06/TheEconomicSocialCost
ofIlliteracy-2.pdf
Worthy, J., Moorman, M. & Turner, M. (1999). What Johnny likes to read is hard to find in
school. Reading Research Quarterly, 34(1), 12-27. Retrieved from JSTOR.
Wyatt-Smith, C. & Elkins, J. (2008). Multimodal reading and comprehension in online
environments. In J. Coiro, M. Knobel, Lankshear, & D. J. Leu (Eds.), Handbook of
Research on New Literacies, 899-934. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Yatvin, J. (2003). I told you so: The misinterpretation and misuse of the National Reading Panel
report. Education Week, 22(33), 44-45. Retrieved from:
https://www.edweek.org/ew/articles/2003/04/30/33yatvin.h22.html
312
APPENDIX A
Teacher Focus Group Demographic Survey Responses
Yrs in
Education
Yrs in
District
Yrs in
Current
Role
School
Grades
Taught
Age
group
Gender
Race/
Ethn.
Native
Language
Degree
Area of
licensure
Other areas in
which you are
licensed
29
29
1-3
R-
STEM
K-5
specialist/
coach
51-65
Female
White
English
Ed.
Specialist
or 6th
year
Elementary
Education
Middle School
Math
5
5
1-3
RDLS
K-5
specialist/
coach
21-30
Female
White
English
Masters
Degree
Elementary
Education
NA
22
2
1-3
RDLS
Grade 3
41-50
Female
White
English
Ed.
Specialist
or 6th
year
K-12 ESL,
Other
K-12 Spanish,
K-12 Principal
13
4
4-10
SH
K-5
specialist/
coach
31-40
Female
White
English
Masters
Degree
Elementary
Education,
K-12
Reading
NA
13
4
4-10
SH
Grade 4
31-40
Female
White
English
Masters
Degree
Elementary
Education,
K-12
Reading
Reading
specialist
31
7
15+
RDLS
Grade 4
51-65
Male
Two or
more
Races
Spanish
Doctorate
Elementary
Education,
Other
Elementary
Principal -
Administrative
License, K-9
Spanish
40
5
4-10
SH
K-5
specialist/
coach
51-65
Female
White
English
Masters
Degree
Elementary
Education
NA
26
21
4-10
R-
STEM
K-5
specialist/
coach
51-65
Female
White
English
Masters
Degree
Elementary
Education
N/A
313
APPENDIX B
Indices Relevant to Reading Comprehension
Lexical Sophistication
Psycholinguistic properties
Features related to word concreteness, familiarity, imagability, age of
acquisition, meaningfulness, hypernymy, polysemy, word neighborhood
effects, and word recognition norms
Academic Terms
Words and phrases common in academic discourse
Association Measures
Number of strong associations averaged across words
Syntactic Complexity
Clausal and Phrasal Complexity
Average number of words, structures, and dependents per clause or t-unit
Syntactic Sophistication
Frequency, type token ratio, attested items, and association strengths for
verb-argument constructions
Syntactic Similarity
Similarity between sentences in terms of POS tags and syntactic parses
Text Cohesion
Givenness
Semantic similarity between given and new text
Lexical Repetition
Type-token ratio
Temporal Cohesion
Repetition of tense and aspect
Causal Cohesion
Use of causal verbs and particles
Local Cohesion
Connectives
Frequency of connectives and conjunctions in the text
Lexical Overlap (sentence)
Word overlap between sentences
Semantic Overlap (sentence)
Semantic overlap between sentences
Global Cohesion
314
Lexical Overlap (paragraph)
Word overlap between paragraphs
Synonym Overlap (paragraph)
Overlap of words and synonymy between paragraphs
Semantic Overlap (paragraph)
Semantic overlap between paragraphs
Indices Relevant to Reading Comprehension (Continued)
Readability
RDFRE
Flesch-Kincaid Reading Ease Score
RDFKGL
Flesch-Kincaid Grade Level
RDL2
Second Language Readability Score
Syntactic Pattern Density
Noun Phrases
Incidence of noun phrases
Verb Phrases
Incidence of verb phrases
Prepositional Phrases
Incidence of prepositional phrases
Rhetorical Features
Amplifiers
Incidence of amplifiers (e.g. “very”)
Private verbs
Incidence of private verbs (e.g. “think”)
Hedges
Incidence of hedges (e.g. “maybe,” “likely”)
Connectives
Causal
Incidence of causal connectives
Logic
Incidence of logic connectives
Temporal
Incidence of temporal connectives
315
APPENDIX C
Summer Targeted Services Text Data
Title
Word
Count
Paragraphs
Sentences
FK Grade
Text (In
Order
Presented)
Narrativity
(percentile)
Syntactic
Simplicity
(percentile)
Word
Concreteness
(percentile)
Referential
Cohesion
(percentile)
Deep
Cohesion
(percentile)
How a Star
is Born
275
6
33
2.7
1
63
91
75
74
90
Eating
Healthy
384
16
35
3.7
2
54
93
80
44
89
Mars
375
5
38
3.0
3
60
79
30
88
24
Wildfires
254
3
20
5.1
4
47
90
74
44
99
Blood
361
5
32
4.3
5
67
57
97
100
50
Ostriches
390
5
35
4.0
6
53
69
83
49
30
Starfish
398
6
39
4.8
7
59
81
70
75
44
Avalanches
300
4
27
5.5
8
34
92
61
57
45
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
Mean
351.71
6.29
32.29
4.34
5.0
53.43
80.14
70.71
65.29
54.43
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
SD
54.01
4.39
6.73
0.86
2.16
10.63
13.32
21.18
22.57
28.64
316
APPENDIX D
Focus Group Initial Questions
1. How long have you worked in [the district]? (Follow up: How long have you taught 3rd
or fourth grade?)
2. How has reading comprehension instruction changed, if at all, in [the district where this
study took place] since you have been here?
3. What do you notice about the types of texts students in third and fourth grade like to
read? (Follow up: Is this true for groups of students or just some? Elaborate.)
4. In thinking about the texts that your students in third and fourth grade read, what do you
think are beneficial characteristics of texts to include for reading comprehension
instruction? (Follow up: Why?)
5. Think in terms of length, readability, cohesion (how the text “flows”), and subject matter.
What is important with regard to all of this when picking texts for students to read?
6. What do you think of your colleague’s answer just now? Anything you would like to
add?
7. What gaps do you think exist in the kinds of texts that your students are reading in
school? Think specifically about science texts.
317
APPENDIX E
Science Assessment (Modified from NAEP)
1. The surface of the Moon is covered with craters. Most of these craters were formed by
A. eruptions of active volcanoes
B. the impact of many meteoroids
C. shifting rock on the Moon’s surface (“moonquakes”)
D. tidal forces caused by the Earth and Sun
2. Which statement explains why light from the Sun can warm up water in a glass?
A. Light travels very fast.
B. Light travels in straight lines.
C. Water reflects light energy.
D. Water absorbs light and energy.
3. Which animal develops inside its mother before it is born alive?
A. Butterfly
B. Duck
C. Cat
D. Frog
4. When you are riding a bicycle at night, your bicycle’s reflectors help people in cars see
your bicycle. How do bicycle reflectors work?
A. They are made of a special material that gives off its own light.
B. They are hooked up to batteries that allow them to produce light.
318
C. They bounce light back from other sources.
D. They are covered with paint that glows in the dark.
5. A farmer thinks that the vegetables on her farm are not getting enough water. Her son
suggests that they use water from the nearby ocean to water the vegetables. Is this a good idea?
A. Yes, because there is plenty of ocean water.
B. Yes, because ocean water has many natural fertilizers.
C. No, because ocean water is too salty for plants grown on land.
D. No, because ocean water is much more polluted than rainwater.
6. Where does water in a lake get most of its energy to evaporate?
A. The sun heating the lake
B. Green plants living in the lake
C. Streams entering the lake
D. Cold springs under the lake
319
APPENDIX F
Student Observation Form Text Reading and Responding
Paraphrasing Observation Form
Date:
Number of students observed:
Start time:
End time:
Fidelity Check Instructions: Observe students as they work through one text. Complete the
checklist to the extent that the components were observed, and write detailed notes regarding
these or other components observed.
In preparation. Gather/complete the following information prior to the observation.
List of students who you will be observing
List of ID numbers for the students being observed
Observation Notes:
Student(s) are seated in an area that is not overly distracting (i.e., student is able to focus on the module
the majority of the time).
Very few minor to no distractions Some distractions Very distracting for the majority of
the time
Were the student(s) able to get started on the activity (begin loading a module) within a reasonable
amount of time (e.g., 2 minutes) of transitioning? Yes No
Anything else to note regarding the environment, tech issues, behavior of the children, or other things
noteworthy:
320
Instructions and Code.
Use momentary time sampling (look to the designated student at each interval and determine
whether or not they are displaying the desired behavior at that moment in time). Select a logical
order of students to observe and rotate through, looking at one student and then the next for each
time interval. Focus only on the designated student for that interval and record only on that
student’s behavior when your interval timer beeps. Write a *, 1, or a 0 in the column under the
text title section indicating the observed behavior. If the student had a glitch, place an “X” in this
column and simply skip over student during their interval. Observe their use of directions and
record in this column as well.
* Glitch (e.g., freezing, link did not work, etc.)
1 Engagement was observed
0 Engagement was not observed
Observation Order
Student ID
Text Title /Directions
1
2
3
Time
Student
Reading
Paraphrasing
Vocabulary
Other
0:10
1
0:20
2
0:30
3
0:40
1
0:50
2
1:00
3
1:10
1
321
1:20
2
1:30
3
1:40
1
1:50
2
2:00
3
2:10
1
2:20
2
2:30
3
2:40
1
2:50
2
3:00
3
3:10
1
3:20
2
3:30
3
3:40
1
3:50
2
4:00
3
4:10
1
4:20
2
4:30
3
4:40
1
4:50
2
5:00
3
5:10
1
5:20
2
5:30
3
322
5:40
1
5:50
2
6:00
3
6:10
1
6:10
1
6:20
2
6:30
3
6:40
1
6:50
2
7:00
3
7:10
1
7:20
2
7:30
3
7:40
1
8:00
3
8:10
1
8:20
2
8:30
3
8:40
1
8:50
2
9:00
3
9:10
1
9:20
2
9:30
3
9:40
1
9:50
2
323
10:00
3
10:10
1
10:20
2
10:30
3
10:40
1
10:50
2
11:00
3
11:10
1
11:20
2
11:30
3
11:40
1
11:50
2
12:00
3
12:10
1
12:20
2
12:30
3
12:40
1
12:50
2
13:00
3
13:10
1
13:20
2
13:30
3
13:40
1
13:50
2
14:00
3
14:10
1
324
14:20
2
14:30
3
14:40
1
14:50
2
15:00
3
15:10
1
15:20
2
15:30
3
15:40
1
15:50
2
16:00
3
16:10
1
16:20
2
16:30
3
16:40
1
16:50
2
17:00
3
17:10
1
17:20
2
17:30
3
17:40
1
17:50
2
18:00
3
18:10
1
18:20
2
18:30
3
325
18:40
1
18:50
2
19:00
3
19:10
1
19:20
2
19:30
3
19:40
1
19:50
2
20:00
3
Subtotals: column
observations/possible
/
= %
/
= %
/
= %
/
= %
Overall Total: total observations possible
/ = %
326
APPENDIX G
Coding Table for Student Paraphrase Responses
FILTER: Too
short
0 = the response is more than
one word long
1 = the response is only one word
DO NOT CONTINUE SCORING
FILTER:
Copy/Paste
0 = the response is not a
copy/paste of the target
sentence (minor
morphological changes do not
count, i.e. “don’t” to “do not”
or “big” to “bigger”)
1 = the response is a copy/paste or retyping
of the sentence without anything added
(e.g., only one word is different but it is a
stem or slight morphological change); if
some minor words are cut out and no new
words are added, it is counted as a
copy/paste; it does not have to be the entire
TS copied, but counts if the entire response
was directly copied from the TS; ask
yourself: would I want to give the
respondent feedback of “please try again
and please change some more words”
DO NOT CONTINUE SCORING
FILTER:
Clause
Reversal (copy
and paste)
0 = no clause reversal
1 = Copy and paste of the target sentence,
just reversed clauses
DO NOT CONTINUE SCORING
FILTER
Garbage
0 =the response makes sense
and uses words, the response
is meaningful and makes
sense
1= the response is garbage, difficult to
derive meaning from response
DO NOT CONTINUE SCORING
FILTER:
Irrelevant
0 = the response is at least
partially relevant to the text
1= the response is irrelevant information to
the text
DO NOT CONTINUE SCORING
Category
Rating
327
Paraphrase
Presence
0 = no attempt at
paraphrasing the target
sentence
1 = attempt at paraphrasing the target sentence
Lexical
similarity
0 = Low similarity in exact
words from target sentence
1 = Uses a majority of the same words
(emphasis on the content words) to the target
sentence, regardless of syntax (words can be
in a random order essentially)
Syntactic
similarity
0 = Low similarity in
syntactic forms; adding a
clause/prepositional phrase
does not suffice if the main
clause is similar
1 = High syntactic similarity (uses similar
grammatical forms); look at the main
subject-verb clause; if a prepositional clause
simply move locations in the sentence, this
does not count as syntactic dissimilarity
Semantic
similarity
0 = response not
semantically similar to the
TS
1 = most (~60-100 percent) of the
subjects/verb/meaning captured
Elaboration (or
bridging)
Refers to a response
regarding the theme of
the target sentence
rather than a
restatement of the
sentence.
0 = No added relevant
content beyond the target
sentence
1 = Other ideas are included beyond the
information from the target sentence, such as,
elaborations or previous ideas from the text.
Also includes anaphoric references (the direct
object a pronoun is referring to); the purpose
is to see if there is added content to the
sentence that is not a direct paraphrase of the
target sentence; a synonym replacing a word
from the target sentence is not sufficient
Inaccuracy
0 = No misconception/
misinformation present; the
paraphrase accurately
reflects the idea(s) of the
target sentence
1 = Misconceptions or misinformation
present; change of meaning/semantics
Other content
0= No other content
included beyond relevant
content (paraphrase,
elaborations, bridges)
1 = Other content is included beyond the
information from the target sentence and is
NOT elaborating/bridging
328
Paraphrase
Quality
0 = Poor paraphrase, does not
demonstrate the use of
paraphrasing strategies
1 = Fair quality
paraphrase,
demonstrates the use
of at least 1
paraphrase strategy,
but there is room for
improvement;
captures only half
(or less) of the main
ideas
2 = High quality
paraphrase, uses at
least 1 paraphrasing
strategy; captures
most of the main
ideas
329
APPENDIX H
De-Identified Student Pre-Assessment Data
De-
Identifier
Grade
zGemder
Ethnicity
EL
FRP
Reading
Rec/%
Reading
Acad/%
Reading
Total/%
Science
Assess
(out of 8)
10001
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
35/84th
32/79th
67/83rd
6
10002
4
M
Hispanic
Y
Reduced
28/41st
26/46th
54/41st
5
10003
3
F
Hispanic
Y
None
28/38th
34/83rd
62/64th
2
10004
4
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
31/60th
28/58th
59/59th
7
10005
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
31/57th
32/74th
63/67th
4
10006
3
M
Hispanic
N
Free
30/51st
30/63rd
60/58th
absent
10007
4
F
Hispanic
N
Free
30/54th
26/41st
56/44th
5
10008
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
29/45th
28/52nd
57/48th
1
10009
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
28/38th
23/26th
51/28th
absent
10010
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
29/45th
28/52nd
57/48th
2
10011
4
M
Black/
Af. Am
N
Free
20/6th
21/20th
41/9th
1
10012
3
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
37/90th
35/88th
72/91st
3
10013
3
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
17/1st
22/22nd
39/6th
3
10014
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
36/88th
34/87th
70/89th
6
10015
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
33/72nd
31/75th
64/75th
6
10016
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Free
34/78th
37/95th
71/91st
2
10017
4
M
Hispanic
Y
Free
26/26th
28/58th
54/41st
4
10018
4
F
Hispanic
Y
Reduced
32/66th
24/35th
56/48th
6
10019
3
M
Asian
Y
None
26/26th
34/83rd
60/58th
4
330
APPENDIX I
Elementary Reading Attitude Survey Data
Student
Recreational
Score/Percentile
Academic
Score/Percentile
Total Score//Percentile
10001
35/84th
32/79th
67/83rd
10002
28/41st
26/46th
54/41st
10003
2838th
34/83rd
62/64th
10004
31/60th
28/58th
59/59th
10005
31/57th
32/74th
63/67th
10006
30/51st
30/63rd
60/58th
10007
30/54th
26/41st
56/44th
10008
29/45th
28/52nd
57/48th
10009
28/38th
23/26th
51/28th
10010
29/45th
28/52nd
57/48th
10011
20/6th
21/20th
41/9th
10012
37/90th
35/88th
72/91st
10013
17/1st
22/22nd
39/6th
10014
36/88th
34/87th
70/89th
10015
33/72nd
31/75th
64/75th
10016
34/78th
37/95th
71/91st
10017
26/26th
28/58th
54/41st
10018
32/66th
24/35th
56/48th
10019
26/26/th
34/83rd
60/58th
Recreational Score Mean: 29.47
Academic Score Mean: 29.11
Total Score Mean: 58.58
Recreational Score SD: 5.0
Academic Score SD: 4.68
Total Score SD: 8.85
331
APPENDIX J
Themes from Student Observations
Activity
Notes
Video
Students engaged in watching video
Too many strategies all at one time – students had a hard time
retelling any strategies (observer asked for a retell/summary
from each student)
3/16 students watched the video more than one time
Passage Reading –
Ease/Difficulty
Two students were unable to complete even one passage. They
abandoned it after the first paraphrase.
One student was only able to complete one passage in two
sessions equaling 35 minutes.
Reading seemed easier for 5 of the 8 fourth grade students.
Reading was hard for all third grade students
Reading stamina was an issue for many students observed, in
both third and fourth grade.
Reading stamina was not an issue for three students (all fourth
grade).
Passage Reading -
Vocabulary
Vocabulary is an issue with students: e.g. “what is a clump?”
Fahrenheit was a difficult word for all students.
Some difficult words were not defined within context (e.g.
“satellite”).
Some difficult words were defined after the sentence needed
paraphrasing (e.g. “nutrient”).
Students exhibiting need for ability to have passage read and to
have definition available via technology
One student exhibited a larger oral vocabulary (used the term
“research” to combine the ideas of “satellite” and “telescopes”)
than ability to use vocabulary in written form
Several students exhibit rereading and attempting to sound out
words
332
Paraphrasing –
Content
Some students seemed to simply begin to retype highlighted
sentence
Several fourth grade students got through several passages of
paraphrasing in one sitting of 30 minutes
Shorter highlighted sentences seemed harder for students to
paraphrase
Paraphrasing –
Creation
Typing was hard for all but one student – all students used either
one or two fingers to type. All but two third graders typed with
one finger. The reverse is evident in fourth grade: all but one
student in fourth grade typed with two fingers.
Over half the students exhibited a strong focus on correct
spelling
Some students exhibited frustration when trying to paraphrase
(e.g. “I’m stuck” and “How do you put it in a sentence?”)
Need for ASR for students to create a paraphrase more quickly.
Frustration came with typing.
Engagement in
Task
All students engaged in video for the entire time
All students exhibited a willingness to try hard at the beginning
Engagement wanes immediately when students begin to struggle
– either with reading or with paraphrasing – exhibiting
disengagement by looking away from screen
Difficulty in typing led to disengagement
Once a student disengaged, s/he had a hard time re-engaging on
his/her own
Struggling students were more easily distracted by classroom
noises
When observer provided help to a student, the student exhibited
engagement again
Disengaged students asked how many they had to do
Engagement in correct spelling, even if it slows down the
student (e.g. asking how to spell a word, going into Spellcheck,
using Google)
333
APPENDIX K
Student Text and Paraphrase Perception Data
Text Perceptions
Text Name
# of
Responses
Text Perception
Text Difficulty Perception
Text Perception (%)
Text Difficulty Perception
(%)
Dislike
Okay
Liked
Hard
Okay
Easy
Dislike
Okay
Liked
Hard
Okay
Easy
How a Star is
Born
13
2
4
7
2
4
7
15%
31%
54%
15%
31%
54%
Eating Healthy
9
0
4
5
1
5
3
0%
44%
56%
11%
56%
33%
A Visit to Mars
7
1
2
4
1
2
4
14%
29%
57%
14%
29%
57%
Wildfires
5
0
3
2
0
3
2
0%
60%
40%
0%
60%
40%
Blood
2
0
0
2
0
1
1
0%
0%
100%
0%
50%
50%
Ostriches
2
0
1
1
0
1
1
0%
50%
50%
0%
50%
50%
Starfish
2
0
1
1
0
1
1
0%
50%
50%
0%
50%
50%
Paraphrase Perceptions
Text Name
# of
Responses
Paraphrase Perception
Paraphase Difficulty
Perception
Paraphrase Perception
(%)
Paraphrase Difficulty
Perception (%)
Dislike
Okay
Liked
Hard
Okay
Easy
Dislike
Okay
Liked
Hard
Okay
Easy
How a Star is
Born
13
0
5
8
1
8
4
0%
38%
62%
8%
61%
31%
Eating Healthy
9
0
2
7
0
6
3
0%
22%
78%
0%
67%
33%
A Visit to Mars
7
1
2
4
0
5
2
14%
29%
57%
0%
71%
29%
Wildfires
5
0
3
2
0
4
1
0%
60%
40%
0%
75%
25%
Blood
2
0
1
1
0
1
1
0%
50%
50%
0%
50%
50%
Ostriches
2
0
1
1
0
1
1
0%
50%
50%
0%
50%
50%
Starfish
2
0
1
1
0
1
1
0%
50%
50%
0%
50%
50%
334
APPENDIX L
Student Interview Constructed Responses
Student ID
Question 1: Do you remember
what paraphrasing is?
Question 2: How did you
figure out how to paraphrase
the sentences?
Question 3: Do you think
paraphrasing helped you read
better? Why?
10001
It’s like just, if there’s like a word
that you don’t know you could
like, put it in a new word, like if
it was perfect you could say...is
perfect like great? It is, so instead
of saying perfect you can say
great.
All I really did was I just read
the entire word. And then in my
head I started adjusting words,
and then when I got the perfect
one I wrote it down. LIke I
typed it.
Yeah, it helped me because at
home I even use it. My brother
would say… “give it to you” and
I was like, I don’t know what
that means. You should really
start paraphrasing because don’t
use long words around me
because I don’t know much
words, okay? Because I don’t
have a dictionary.
10005
Change a couple words because if
you don’t know the sentence,
makes more sense to you.
I don’t know.
Uh huh, because you can make
sense to you. Helps you out to
make more sense.
10008
No
I don’t really know.
Mmm hmmm (shakes head yes).
Paraphrasing helps me read
better from harder books and
easy books. It helps me read a
little bit of words.
10009
Mmm hmmm (shakes head).
Paraphrasing is… I forgot… it is
something to do with sentences
I just take them and see in my
head. And then, and then I
started to write on a piece of
paper.
Yeah. Because it helps me learn
much better and read better and
write better.
10010
No.
Don’t remember.
Yeah. Sometimes when I read
and write, it shows how to read
more.
10012
Like changing sentences and
making them up again but with
your own words.
My mind just start with, hey,
let’s do a new word for
paraphrasing something. My
mind just thought of new words.
Yep. It did. It helped me with a
lot of chapter books. Not a lot,
one chapter book.
10014
Describing things you read in
your own words
I remembered different words to
use for another word.
Yes, because it’s like when we
do like an essay we can use our
own words.
10015
Um… like changing something,
to make it like more sense, and
better. Like at the beginning,
instead of saying… um, it’s like
having to say… and like… and so
in the end it’s like you say
something at the end.
Yeah, like if they don’t say then,
I should have put like that thing
in a words that says then… and
they… (listing words “then,”
“they,” and “and”)
Yeah. Because it makes more
sense
335
10016
When you take your own words
and put it… when the author
writes sentences you put it in
your own words. Put it in a
paragraph
Read the sentence first. And
then, I was thinking what should
I write. And then I got it and
wrote it in my words.
Yeah. Because it helped me
make a sentence in my own
words.
10017
No. [Then] I had to try to make it
like smaller, like the sentence, a
little bit shorter.
LIke… different things, like…
after running out of space and
you still have a lot. You like
kind of have to like make the
words a little bit shorter for to
make more space. I tried to find
the words that are like kind of
not important. Like, I can just
take those out and put, like, just
leave an empty for to make it
smaller.
Yeah, because I’m reading the
word, and I’m learning to like
make them smaller, like the ways
to make it small.
10018
I don’t remember stuff
I don’t remember
Yeah, because I’m sometimes…
I don’t understand words and
that helps me a little bit to do.