Old Dominion University Old Dominion University
ODU Digital Commons ODU Digital Commons
STEMPS Faculty Publications STEM Education & Professional Studies
Spring 2014
Spatial Visualization Ability and Impact of Drafting Models: A Spatial Visualization Ability and Impact of Drafting Models: A
Quasi Experimental Study Quasi Experimental Study
Petros J. Katsioloudis
Old Dominion University
Vukica Jovanovic
Old Dominion University
Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.odu.edu/stemps_fac_pubs
Part of the Educational Assessment, Evaluation, and Research Commons, and the Engineering
Education Commons
Original Publication Citation Original Publication Citation
Katsioloudis, P. J., & Jovanovic, V. (2014). Spatial visualization ability and impact of drafting models: A
quasi experimental study.
Engineering Design Graphics Journal, 78
(2), 1-11.
This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the STEM Education & Professional Studies at ODU
Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in STEMPS Faculty Publications by an authorized
administrator of ODU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected].
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
1
Spatial Visualization Ability and Impact of Drafting Models:
A Quasi Experimental Study
Petros J. Katsioloudis
Old Dominion University
Vukica Jovanovic
Old Dominion University
Abstract
A quasi experimental study was done to determine significant positive effects among three different types
of visual models and to identify whether any individual type or combination contributed towards a positive
increase of spatial visualization ability for students in engineering technology courses. In particular, the
study compared the use of different visual models - a 3D printed solid object, a 3D computer generated
drawing and a 2D drawing.
Introduction
It is recognized that the ability to visualize is an important tool required of engineers in
order to function effectively (Deno, 1995; Miller, 1990; Pleck, 1991; Sorby & Baartmans,
2000). More specific, visualization of problems is critical for success in engineering
education (Sorby & Baartmans, 2000), and for that reason spatial abilities have been
used as a predictor of success in several engineering and technology disciplines
(Strong & Smith, 2001).
However, these abilities are not determined genetically, but rather a result of a long
learning process. It has been shown by several studies that some type of intervention,
whether a short course or a semester long course, can improve spatial abilities in
students who score low on tests in this area (Hsi, Linn, & Bell, 1997; Martín-Dorta,
Saorín, & Contero, 2008; Sorby, 2001). For this study, the following was the primary
research question. Is there a difference between the impact of model type (2D drawing,
3D computer generated drawing, 3D printed object) on spatial visualization ability? The
following hypotheses will be analyzed to attempt to find a solution to the research
question. The hypotheses that guided this study were:
H
0
: There will be no difference in spatial visualization ability between the impact of
model type (2D drawing, 3D computer generated drawing, 3D printed object).
H
A
: There will be significant difference in spatial visualization ability between the impact
of model type (2D drawing, 3D computer generated drawing, 3D printed object).
Review of Literature
According to Piagetian theory, an individual acquires spatial visualization ability through
three distinct stages of development (Bishop, 1978). During the first stage, children
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
2
acquire topological spatial visualization skills with the ability to discern an object’s
topological relationship with other objects. During the second stage of development,
projective representation is acquired and children can conceive what an object will look
like from a different perspective. At the third stage of spatial visualization development,
the individual learns to combine projective abilities with the concept of measurement.
Due to the reduced amount of instructional time given for engineering graphics content
in many engineering and technology programs, faculty have expressed concern that
students’ ability to visualize 3D parts from 2D drawings is not being developed as well
as in the past (Branoff, T. J. & M. Dobelis, 2013; Branoff, 2007; Clark & Scales, 2000;
Meyers, 2000). To measure an individual’s spatial ability, a plethora of standardized
tests are available.
The most commonly used tests include:
a) The Purdue Spatial Visualization Test: Rotations (PSVT:R), devised to test a
person’s ability at the second stage of development (Sorby, 2005).
b) The Mental Rotation Test (MRT) a test designed to assess a person’s ability to
visualize rotated solids (Sorby, 2005).
c) The Differential Aptitude Test: Space Relations (DAT:SR) consists of 50 items
and with a role to test spatial ability (Monahan, Harke and Shelley, 2008).
d) The Mental Cutting Test (MCT) that requires individuals to create a split view of
an object; therefore, forcing to visualize and choose the correct cross-section
among five alternatives (Tsunumi, 2004).
Several studies have been conducted to examine the usefulness of an engineering
graphics literacy test (Branoff & Dobelis, 2012a, 2012b, 2012c; T. J. Branoff & M.
Dobelis, 2013) and some of them have proven to be great predictors of an individual’s
ability to visualize (Kelly, Clark, & Branoff, 2013).Some of the factors that have been
identified by various graphics education researchers are spatial visualization, spatial
relations, spatial orientation, spatial cognition, spatial intelligence, spatial ability, and
visualization (Hartman & Bertoline, 2005; Martin- Dorta, Saorin, & Contero, 2008; Miller
& Bertoline, 1991; Sorby, 1999a).
According to Bodner and Guay (1997) two factors emerged from spatial ability
research: spatial orientation, which involves not being puzzled by changes in visual
inputs, and spatial visualization, which involves the ability to manage visual input
components (Kelly, 2012). Eliot and Smith (1983) showed factors, such as spatial
relations, in the context of mental rotation of objects, spatial orientation as the
understanding of how an object would appear from a different perspective, and
visualization from a surface development context (Kelly, 2012) According to Juhel
(1991) the focus is on three factors: spatial orientation, which determines how an object
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
3
will appear from a different position; spatial visualization, which involves the mental
transformation of an object; and speeded rotation, which is the mental rotation of
objects (Kelly, 2012).
In recent years, 3D spatial abilities have received much attention. Several studies have
involved different interfaces to attempt to manipulate a person's understanding of 3D
space (Carriker, 2009).Cockburn (2004) asked whether or not a person would have a
better spatial memory if they were given a 3D representation of the object's location. For
the specific study, the user is not allowed to move; it is only a simple comparison of
perspective effects in the displays (Carriker, 2009). Cockburn (2004) also added visual
cues that gave the illusion of a 3D object, including shadows, lighting and size, to see if
individuals could recall the 3D objects better than their 2D counterparts. He found that
there were no significant differences between the averages of the 2D and 3D conditions.
Authors, Tan, Gergle, Scupelli, & Pausch (2004) performed a study that was designed
to examine the effects of physical display size on an individual’s cognitive strategy and
performance on an interactive 3D navigation task (Carriker, 2009). Comparable to the
prior study by Cockburn, Tan et al. attempted to analyze 3D spatial ability using different
displays. However, they also addressed whether that performance is directly affected by
the task being interactive or not. Tan et al. (2004) attempted to examine not only the
implications of the display, but the effect on the subject when allowed different means of
interaction with the 3D world.
In addition, several researchers have suggested that spatial ability can be enhanced
and taught by some instructional designs (Alias, Black, & Gray, 2002; Kwon, 2003;
Lajoie, 2003; Potter & Merwe, 2001; Woolf, Romoser, Bergeron, & Fisher, 2003). Many
works demonstrated that instructions using computer-based 3D visualizations can
provide learners with adequate spatial experiences for developing their spatial ability
(Kwon, 2003; Woolf et al., 2003). However, few empirical studies have established the
causal relationships in greater depth (Wang, Chang & Li, 2006).Moreover; few studies
have explored the effects of two-dimensional (2D) versus three dimensional (3D) media
representations on the influence of the spatial ability of undergraduate students (Wang,
Chang & Li, 2006).
Based on this research, it is clear that changing the software or hardware has a high
correlation to a student's understanding of 3D space. This encourages future research
to find the most efficient tools to improve 3D spatial visualization ability for all students.
Methodology
A quasi-experimental study was selected as a means to perform the comparative
analysis of spatial visualization ability during the fall semester of 2013. The study was
conducted in an engineering graphics course, MET 120 (Computer Aided Drafting),
offered at Old Dominion University as a part of the Engineering Technology program.
The participants from the study are shown in Table 1. From the 54 students, 12 were
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
4
females and 18 were African American and using a convenience sample there was a
near equal distribution of the participants between the three groups.
Table 1. Research Design Methodology
Group 1
n1=20
MCT
Sketch from 2D drawing
Group 2
n2=16
MCT
Sketch from 3D image
Group 3
n3=18
MCT
Sketch from 3D object
The engineering graphics course emphasized “hands on” practice using 2-D and 3D
AutoCAD software in the computer lab, along with the various methods of editing,
manipulation, visualization and presentation of technical drawings. In addition, the
course included the basic principles of engineering drawing/hand sketching, dimensions
and tolerance principles.
The students attending the course during the fall semester of 2013 were divided in to
three groups according to the section of the course that they chose to participate the
semester prior to the study. The three groups (n1=20, n2= 16 and n3=18 with an overall
population of N = 54) were presented with a visual representation of an object (drafting
model) and were asked to create a sectional view. The first group (n1) received a 2D
drawing of the cone (see Figure.1), the second group (n2) received a 3D PC generated
image of the cone (see Figure. 2) and the third group (n3) received a 3D printed cone
using a 3D rapid prototyping machine (see Figure. 3).
Figure1. 2D Drawing Figure 2. 3D Computer Figure 3. 3D Printed Object Using
Generated Drawing Additive Technology
In addition, all groups were asked to complete the MCT instrument 2 days prior to the
completion of the sectional view drawing to identify level of visual ability and show
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
5
equality between the three groups. According to Nemeth and Hoffman (2006) the MCT
has been widely used in all age groups. The “standard MCT” consists of 25 problems.
The Mental Cutting Test (hereafter MCT), a sub-set of the CEEB Special Aptitude Test
in Spatial Relations has been used by Suzuki et al.to measure spatial abilities in
relation to graphics curricula (Tsunumi, 2004).
In each problem, subjects are given a perspective drawing of a test solid, which is to be
cut with a hypothetical cutting plane. Subjects are then asked to choose one correct
cross section from among 5 alternatives. There are two categories of problems in the
test (Tsutsumi, 2004). Those of the first category are called `pattern recognition
problems', in which the correct answer is determined by identifying only the pattern of
the section. The other are called `quantity problems' or `dimension specification
problems', in which the correct answer is determined by identifying not only the correct
pattern but also the quantity in the section, e.g., the length of the edges or the angles
between the edges (Tsutsumi, 2004).
Upon completion of the MCT the instructor of the course placed the 2D drawing, 3D
computer generated image and 3D printed object in a central location in the classroom
(the three groups were positioned in to three different rooms) and asked the students to
create a sectional view of the cone. The engineering drawing that was used in this
research was a sectional view of a cone which had different levels of different materials.
These levels had different colors. Sectional views are very useful engineering graphics
tool, especially for parts that have complex interior geometry. Sections are used to
clarify the interior construction of a part that cannot be clearly described by hidden lines
in exterior views (Plantenberg 2013). By taking an imaginary cut through the object and
removing a portion, the inside features could be seen more clearly. Students had to
mentally discard the unwanted portion of the part and draw the remaining part. The
rubric used included the following parts: 1) use of section view labels; 2) use of correct
hatching style for cut materials; 3) accurate indication of cutting plane; 4) appropriate
use of cutting plane lines; and 5) appropriate drawing of omitted hidden features.
Maximum score for the drawing was 6 points.
Data Analysis
Analysis of MCT Scores
The first method of data collection involved the completion of the MCT instrument prior
to the treatment to show equality of spatial ability between the three different groups.
The researchers graded the MCT instrument as described in the guidelines of the MCT
creators. A standard paper-pencil MCT was conducted, in which the subjects were
instructed to draw intersecting lines on the surface of a test solid with a green pencil
before selecting alternatives. The maximum score that can be received on the MCT is
25 and as it can be seen in Table 2, n1 had a mean of 21.47, n2 had a mean of 19.76
and n3 had a mean of 21.37. There was no significant difference between the three
groups as far as spatial ability as measured by the MCT instrument.
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
6
Table 2. MCT Descriptive Results
95% Confidence Interval for Mean
N
Mean
Std Error
Lower Bound
Upper Bound
2D
16
21.471
3.213
13.629
17.513
3D PC
20
19.766
2.096
14.169
19.164
3D Solid
18
21.314
2.390
18.049
21.379
Total
54
21.85
2.56
16.28
19.352
Analysis of Drawing
The second method of data collection involved the creation of a sectional view drawing.
As shown in Table 3, the group that used the 2D drawing as visual aid (n =16) had a
mean observation score of 4.06. The groups that used the 3D computer generated
visual (n = 20) and the 3D printed solid cone (n = 18) had higher scores of 5.87 and
5.12 respectively. A one-way ANOVA was run to compare the mean scores for
significant differences among the three groups. The result of the ANOVA test, as shown
in Table 4, was significant, F(2, 52) = 14.54, p < 0.01. The data was dissected further
through the use of a post hoc Tukey’s honest significant difference (HSD) test. As it can
be seen in Table 5, the post hoc analysis shows a statistically significant difference
between the 3D Solid vs. 3D PC (p < 0.001, d = 2.08) and the 3D Solid vs. 2D (p =
0.008, d = 1.54), with 3D Solid vs. 2D being significantly lower in both cases.
Table 3. Sectional View Drawing Descriptive Results
95% Confidence Interval for Mean
N
Mean
SC
Std Error
Lower Bound
Upper Bound
2D
16
4.061
.2672
.0724
3.917
4.235
3D PC
20
5.876
1.287
.3075
5.209
6.424
3D Solid
18
5.122
.8492
.2547
4.594
5.790
Total
54
5.036
1.163
.1859
4.691
5.301
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
7
Table 4. Sectional View Drawing ANOVA Results
Quiz
SS
df
MS
F
p
Between Groups
13.531
2
11.765
14.536
< 0.001*
Within Groups
22.376
50
.809
Total
55.907
52
* Denotes statistical significance
Table 5. Sectional View Drawing Tukey HSD Results
Visual Aids (1 vs. 2)
Mean Diff. (1-2)
Std. Error
p
3D Solid vs. 3D PC
-1.80
0.334
< 0.001*
3D Solid vs. 2D
-1.07
0.340
0.008*
3D PC vs. 2D
0.724
0.334
0.090
* Denotes statistical significance
Discussion
This study was done to determine significant positive effects among three different
types of visual models and to identify whether any individual type or combination
contributed towards a positive increase of spatial visualization ability for students in
engineering technology courses. In particular, the study compared the use of different
visual models- a 3D printed solid object, a 3D computer generated drawing and a 2D
drawing. It was found that the 3D printed solid model and 3D computer generated
image both provided statistically significant higher scores than the 2D drawing. While
not statistically significant, the students who received treatment via the 3D printed solid
model outperformed their peers who received treatment from the other two models in
the drawing. This could indicate that students were better able to comprehend visual
data given from 3D solid models, over 3D computer generated models or 2D drawings.
It should be noted that the majority of visual models used in the past and today are 2D
drawings, asking the students to recreate different views. Using 3D solid models as
visualizations aids for engineering graphics courses has great potential. With the current
status of additive technologies instructors have the ability to design and built almost any
model in a very short time frame. However, potential issues include: a) availability of 3D
printers at all institutions and b) it appears that more research is needed utilizing
populations with different background. This small quasi experimental study provided
results contrary to the commonly used method of 2D visual modeling. Instead, a 3D
solid model seems to give the students a better understanding of the task being taught.
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
8
Future Plans
Future plans include, but are not limited to:
Repeating the study to verify the results by using additional types of models.
Repeating the study using a different population such as technology education,
science or mathematics students.
Repeating the study by adding additional visual cues during the display of 3D
objects, including shadows, lighting and size.
Repeating the study by comparing males vs females as it has been suggested
that males tend to do better on spatial ability tasks than females (Carriker, 2009).
References
Alias, M., Black, T. R., & Gray, D. E. (2002). Effect of instructions on spatial visualization ability in civil
engineering students. International Educational Journal, 3(1), 112.
Bodner, G. M., & Guay, R. B. (1997). The purdue visualization of rotations test. The Chemical Educator,
2(4), 117.
Branoff, T. J., & Dobelis, M. (2013, June 23-26). The Relationship Between Students’ Ability to Model
Objects from Assembly Drawing Information and Spatial Visualization Ability as Measured by the
PSVT:R and MCT. Paper presented at the 120th ASEE Annual Conference & Exposition, Atlanta,
GA.
Branoff, T. J. (2007). The state of engineering design graphics in the United States. Paper presented at
the Proceedings of the 40th Anniversary Conference of the Japan Society for Graphic Science,
Tokyo, Japan.
Branoff, T. J., & Dobelis, M. (2012a). Engineering graphics literacy: Measuring students’ ability to model
objects from assembly drawing information. Paper presented at the Proceedings of 66th Midyear
Conference of the Engineering Design Graphics Division of the American Society of Engineering
Education, Galveston, Texas.
Branoff, T. J., & Dobelis, M. (2012b). Engineering graphics literacy: Spatial visualization ability and
students’ ability to model objects from assembly drawing infromation. Paper presented at the
Proceedings of the 2012 American Society for Engineering Education Annual Conference and
Exhibition, San Antonio, Texas.
Branoff, T. J., & Dobelis, M. (2012c). The relationship between spatial visualization ability and student’s
ability to model 3D objects from engineering assembly drawings. Engineering Graphics Journal,
76(3), 37-43.
Branoff, T. J., & Dobelis, M. (2013, June 23-26). The relationship between student’s ability to model
objects from assembly drawing information and spatial visualization ability as measured by the
PSVT:R and MCT. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the 2013 American Society for
Engineering Education Annual Conference and Exhibition, Atlanta, Georgia.
Bishop, J. E. (1978). Developing Students Spatial Ability. Science Teacher, 45, 20-23.
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
9
Carriker, A. W. (2009). Effectiveness of 3D Input on Spatial Abilities. (Master of Science), North Carolina
State University, Raleigh, North Carolina.
Cockburn, A. (2004). Revisiting 2D vs 3D Implications on Spatial Memory. ACM International Conference
Proceeding Series, 53, 25-31.
Clark, A. C., & Scales, A. Y. (2000). A study of current trends and issues related to technical/engineering
design graphics. Engineering Design Graphics Journal, 64(1), 24-34.
Eliot, J. & Smith I. M. (Ed.) (1983). An international directory of spatial tests Windsor, Berkshire : NFER-
Nelson ; Atlantic Highlands, N.J. : distributed in the USA by Humanities Press, 1983. Retrieved
from http://search.trln.org.www.lib.ncsu.edu:2048/search?id=DUKE000615210.
Deno, J. (1995). The Relationship of Previous Experiences to Spatial Visualization Ability. Engineering
Design Graphics Journal, 59(3).
Hartman, N.W., & Bertoline, G.R. (2005). Spatial abilities and virtual technologies: Examining the
computer graphics learning environment. Proceedings of the Ninth International Conference on
Information Visualization.
Hsi, S., Linn, M. C., & Bell, J. E. (1997). The role of spatial reasoning in engineering and the design of
spatial instruction. Journal of Engineering Education, 86(2), 151- 158.
Juhel, J. (1991). Spatial Abilities and Individual Differences in Visual Information Processing. Intelligence
15, 117-137.
Kelly, W. F., Clark, A. C., & Branoff, T. J. (2013, October 20-22). Spatial Test Correlation in an
Introductory Communications Course. Paper presented at the 68th Mid-Year Conference,
Worchester, MA.
Kelly, W. F., Jr. (2012). Measurement of spatial ability in an introductory graphic communications course.
(Order No. 3575633, North Carolina State University). ProQuest Dissertations and Theses, 268.
Retrieved from http://search.proquest.com/docview/1459459189?accountid=12967.
(1459459189).
Kwon, O. N. (2003). Fostering spatial visualization ability through web-based virtual-reality program and
paperbased program. Lecture Notes in Computer Science, 2713, 701706.
Lajoie, S. P. (2003). Individual differences in spatial ability: developing technologies to increase strategy
awareness and skills. Educational Psychologist, 38(2), 115125.
Martín-Dorta, N., Saorín, J. L., & Contero, M. (2008). Development of a fast remedial course to improve
the spatial abilities of engineering students. Journal of Engineering Education, 97(4), 505-513.
Meyers, F. D. (2000). First year engineering graphics curricula in major engineering colleges. Engineering
Design Graphics Journal, 64(2), 23-28.
Miller, C.L., & Bertoline, G.R. (1991). Spatial visualization research and theories: their importance in the
development of an engineering and technical design graphics curriculum model. Engineering
Design Graphics Journal, 55(3), 5-14.
Miller, C. L. (1990). Enhancing Spatial Visualization Abilities Through the Use of Real and Computer-
Generated Models. Paper presented at the 1990 ASEE Annual Conference.
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
10
Monahan, J. S., Harke, M. A., & Shelley, J. R. (2008). Computerizing the mental rotations test: Are
gender differences maintained? Behavior Research Methods, 40(2), 422-427.
Németh, B. (2007). Measurement of the development of spatial ability by Mental Cutting Test. Annales
Mathematicae et Informaticae, 34, 123128.
Nemeth, B. & Hoffmann, M. (2006). Gender differences in spatial visualization among engineering
students. Annales Mathematicae Et Informaticae, 33, 169-174.
Plantenberg , K. (2013). Engineering Graphics Essentials with AutoCAD 2014 Instruction. Mission, KS
SDC Publications.
Pleck, M. H. (1991). Visual Literacy-An Important Aspect of Engineering Design. Paper presented at the
1991 ASEE Annual Conference.
Potter, C., &Merwe, E. (2001). Spatial ability, visual imagery and academic performance in engineering
graphics. In Proceedings of the international conference on engineering education. Oslo/Bergen,
Norway.
Sorby, S. A. (2005). Developing 3-D spatial visualization skills for non-engineering students. Paper
presented at the American Society for Engineering Education Annual Conference and Exposition.
Sorby, S. A. (2001). Improving the spatial visualization skills of engineering students: Impact on graphics
performance and retention. Engineering Design Graphics Journal, 65(3), 31-36.
Sorby, S. A., & Baartmans, B. J. (2000). The Development and Assessment of a Course for Enhancing
the 3-D Spatial Visualization Skills of First Year Engineering Students. Journal of Engineering
Education, 89(3), 301-307.
Sorby, S.A. (1999a). Developing 3-d spatial visualization skills. Engineering Design Graphics Journal,
63(2), 21-32.
Tan, D. S., Gergle, D., Scupelli, G., & Pausch, R. (2004). Physically large displays improve path
integration in 3D virtual navigation tasks. Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems,
6(1), 439-446.
Tsutsumi, E. (2004). A Mental Cutting Test Using Drawings of Intersections. Journal for Geometry and
Graphics, 83, 117-126.
Tsutsumi, E., Shiina, K., Suzaki, A., Yamanouchi, K., Saito, T., & Suzuki, K. (1999). A mental cutting test
on female students using a stereographic system. Journal for Geometry and Graphics, 3(1), 111-
119.
Woolf, B., Romoser, M., Bergeron, D., & Fisher, D. (2003). Tutoring three-dimensional visual skills:
dynamic adaptation to cognitive level. In Proceedings of the eleventh international conference on
artificial intelligence in education. Sydney, Australia.
Engineering Design Graphics Journal (EDGJ) Copyright 2014
Spring 2014, Vol. 78, No. 2 ISSN: 1949-9167
http://www.edgj.org
___________________________________________________________________
11
___________________________________________________________________
About the Authors
Petros J. Katsioloudis is an Associate Professor and the Industrial Technology Program Leader,
Department of STEM Education and Professional Studies, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA. His
research focuses on improving teacher and student performance in STEM education, and enhancing the
development of a national STEM-educated workforce.
Email: pkatsiol@odu.edu
Vukica Jovanovic is an Assistant Professor of Mechanical Engineering Technology Department, Frank
Batten College of Engineering and Technology, Old Dominion University, Norfolk, VA. Her areas of
interest include the following: Mechatronics, Product Lifecycle Management, Digital Manufacturing,
Engineering Collaboration, RFID, and Assembly Systems.
Email: v2jovano@odu.edu